BE Manuscript Version for Press

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

BE Manuscript Version for Press Title: Risk assessment and the use of novel shortcuts in spatial detouring tasks in jumping spiders Abbreviated title: Route assessment in jumping spiders Samuel Aguilar-Argüello¹; Daniel Gerhard²; Ximena J. Nelson¹* ¹School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand ²School of Mathematics and Statistic, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand *Author for correspondence: [email protected] Address: School of Biological Sciences, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand. Phone: +6433695198 Abstract Selection on individuals that incorporate risk to quickly and accurately make a priori navigational assessments may lead to increased spatial ability. Jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae) are characterized by their highly acute vision, which mediates many behaviors, including prey capture and navigation. When moving to a specific goal (prey, nest, a potential mate, etc.), salticids rely on visual cues and spatial memory to orient in three-dimensional space. Salticid spatial ability has been studied in homing and detour tasks, with Portia being considered one of the most skillful genera in terms of spatial ability in the family. Commonly living in complex environments, salticids are likely to encounter a wide variety of routes that could lead to a goal, and, as selection favors individuals that can accurately make assessments, they may be able to assess alternative route distances to select the most efficient route. Here, we tested whether two salticid species (Portia fimbriata and Trite planiceps) can discriminate and assess between different available routes by their length, and riskiness to escape from a stressful scenario. Results suggest that while Portia is more likely to choose 1 the easiest and shortest escape routes, Trite is faster in both decision-making about which route to take, and to escape. However, some individuals were able to use novel shortcuts instead of the routes expected, with Portia containing a higher proportion of shortcut-takers than Trite. These differences in spatial ability seem to correspond with the environmental complexity inhabited by each species. Key Words: Cognition, salticid, decision-making, detour behavior, environmental complexity, route choice. 2 INTRODUCTION Assessment is the process by which animals evaluate perceived stimuli, converting them to an informational state to determine a specific level of risk or benefit (Blumstein and Bouskila 1996), while decision-making is a cognitive process that allows animals to evaluate their environment, so they can avoid less favorable situations. Thus, decision-making follows assessment and precedes observable behavior (Blumstein and Bouskila 1996). Because of increased ability to take the most efficient route to a goal, selection favors those individuals that can more quickly and accurately make assessments (Helfman 1989, Lima and Bednekoff 1999, Mirza and Chivers 2001, Brown 2003, Golub and Brown 2003). For example, in a food gathering task, selection of inefficient routes results in prolonged foraging, higher energetic cost, decreased time spent on other activities, and increased predation risk (Gibson et al. 2007). Efficient route use has been observed in bees that integrate information about flight path vectors (‘path integration’) to navigate to the colony or a food source (Cartwright and Collett 1983), allowing them to make novel shortcuts, even if they cannot see the goal (Dyer 1991, Menzel et al. 2005, 2011). Similarly, shortcuts have been observed in desert ants (Wehner and Wehner 1990) and wandering spiders (Seyfarth et al. 1982). In these cases, proprioceptive mechanisms are used, and the distance assessment of the alternative routes is only done after experience. Here, we investigate whether invertebrates with no previous experience can assess different routes beforehand and follow the most efficient route to reach a goal, without the use of path integration. Animals that pounce on their prey, such as jumping spiders (Salticidae), are ideal subjects to investigate decision-making. Salticids have a highly-developed visual system (Land et al. 2012) and perform precision jumps for predation and locomotion, accurately assessing the distance to the landing point (Nabawy et al. 2018). Furthermore, when a salticid identifies a prey, it is sometimes forced to take a detour, as the direct route is either inaccessible (Tarsitano and Jackson 1997), or disadvantageous (Jackson and Pollard 1996, Jackson and Wilcox 1993). Salticid detours are preceded by scanning behavior which is characterized by a systematic movement of the body in order to 3 visually inspect its surroundings (Tarsitano and Andrew 1999). Scanning is useful for visual inspection and route selection, but is also a crucial stage for navigation, as the individual potentially plans the route ahead of time (Cross and Jackson 2016). Previous detour-related tasks have shown that salticids can discriminate between routes that lead to a moving or non-moving prey item from routes that do not (Tarsitano and Jackson 1992, 1994, 1997; Tarsitano and Andrew 1999; Tarsitano 2006), but work to date has not considered risk assessment in detour tasks. Commonly living in complex environments, salticid ability to find the best route out of a vast number of alternative pathways could be crucial to save energy and time, and avoid predation. Therefore, salticids may be able to assess, beforehand, alternative routes to select the optimum route. While detouring is a spatial ability apparently widespread among the Salticidae, there is a particular genus that stands out due to its exceptional cognitive abilities and its behavioral flexibility (Jackson and Pollard 1996; Jackson and Cross 2011). Portia lives in complex rainforest habitats in which it preys upon spiders (Jackson and Wilcox 1990; Harland and Jackson 2000). To reach prey, Portia can perform complex detours spanning over a meter, while losing sight of its prey for more than 80 min (Jackson and Hallas 1986a; Jackson and Wilcox 1993). This suggests not only an outstanding sense of orientation in three-dimensional space, but also spatial memory due to the spider’s need to move out of line-of-sight of the prey to follow the detour. Here, our main objective was to determine if salticids can a priori assess route distance and riskiness, but our study differs from previous work in that there was no clear best goal (e.g., prey) which was reached by a single correct route; rather the goal itself was to choose the most efficient (or least risky) way out of a stressful situation. Our routes were also discontinuous, being made up of dowels, such that the problem may be conceived of as a series of sub-goals which needed to be connected in advance in order to achieve the least risky outcome (escape) which was, in itself, identical for all routes. We first tested if salticids differentiate the distance between the dowels and choose a route representing the safest option. Secondly, we tested whether they can discriminate between different routes according to length, and choose the most suitable one in order to escape a 4 stressful scenario. Additionally, we explored whether there are intraspecific and interspecific differences in route assessment. Being from a complex habitat (Jackson and Blest 1982), and known for its cognitive ability (Jackson and Pollard 1996; Jackson and Cross 2011), we predicted that Portia fimbriata would be more effective at making efficient route choices compared with a salticid from a less complex habitat and not known for exceptional cognitive ability, Trite planiceps. Our prediction is based on the clever foraging hypothesis, which postulates that individuals living in more complex environments have better neurobiological navigational abilities (Striedter 2005; Park and Bell 2010). As salticid performance improves with knowledge about the environment (Edwards and Jackson 1994; Aguilar-Arguello et. al 2018), we also predicted that adults would outperform subadults. Additionally, we predicted that females would outperform males, as they are typically the more motivated sex, at least in predation-based or learning tasks (Jackson and Wilcox 1990; Jackson and Pollard 1996; Jakob and Long 2016). METHODS Test animals and maintenance Experiments were carried out from 0800 to 1300 h in the laboratory at the University of Canterbury. Trite planiceps Simon is a large (6-13 mm) salticid endemic to New Zealand and is typically found in coastal areas where it inhabits the rolled-up flax leaves of Phormium tenux and Cordyline spp. (Forster 1979). T. planiceps were field-collected in Christchurch, and were transferred to the laboratory, where they were housed individually in 1 L transparent plastic containers. Individuals were held in captivity for at least one week before testing. Spiders were fed once a week with two adult Musca domestica. Water supply was available through a cotton wick submerged in water which protruded into the container. Portia fimbriata Doleschall is a large (6-11 mm) salticid from the rainforests of Northern Australia (Jackson and Hallas, 1986a). P. fimbriata were lab-reared and, being predominantly araneophagic (spider-eating), were fed a combination of Musca domestica and a Badumna longinqua 5 spider once a week. Individuals were housed as above. Test arena Salticid aversion to water is frequently used as a motivational tool in detour experiments (Cross and Jackson 2016). Here our aim was for spiders to choose a route to escape the stressful scenario of being surrounded by water by getting to the platform edge, rather than choosing a route to a specific target goal, such as prey. Our trials were performed in a 43 x 43 x 7 cm plastic container (pool) filled with water to a depth of 6 cm. From a central starting platform (PVC dowel, 9 cm high x 3.5 cm diameter) four possible escape routes extended to each of the four sides of the pool. Escape routes consisted of a series of PVC dowels (7 cm high x 1 cm diameter) protruding 1 cm from the water. These led to identical high-resolution pictures of foliage which surrounded the pool to both motivate the spiders to exit the pool and to provide visual obstruction of external visual stimuli.
Recommended publications
  • Jumping Spiders Make Predatory Detours Requiring Movement Away from Prey
    JUMPING SPIDERS MAKE PREDATORY DETOURS REQUIRING MOVEMENT AWAY FROM PREY by M.S. TARSITANO1) and JACKSON, R.R.2,3) (1Sussex Centre for Neuroscience, University of Sussex, Brighton, Falmer, East Sussex, BN19QG, UK; 2Department of Zoology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand) (With2 Figures) (Acc. 26-VII-1994) Summary The terms "reversed-route detours" and "forward-route detours" are introduced to distin- guish between detours that require moving away from a goal and those that do not. We provide the first evidence under controlled laboratory conditions that salticids can perform reversed-route detours. Two species were tested: 1) Portia fimbriata,a web-invading salticid from Queensland, Australia, that normally preys on web-building spiders; 2) Triteplaniceps, an insectivorous cursorial salticid from New Zealand. Although both of these species completed reversed-route detours, Triteplaniceps was much more dependent on prey move- ment than Portia fimbriata. Interspecific differences appear to be related to the different predatory styles of these two salticids. Introduction Salticids, or jumping spiders, have unique, complex eyes and acute vision (LAND, 1985; BLEST, 1985). Considering their sophisticated visual system, it is not surprising that most salticid species are hunting spiders which, instead of building webs to ensnare their prey, rely on vision during predatory sequences in which they stalk, chase and leap on active insects (DREES, 1952; FORSTER, 1982). The salticid visual system consists of a pair of forward-facing principal, or antero-median (AM), eyes and three pairs of secondary eyes - the antero-lateral (AL), postero-medial (PM) and 3) We thank Tracey ROBINSONfor her help in the preparation of the manuscript.
    [Show full text]
  • Myrmarachnine Jumping Spiders of the New Subtribe Levieina from Papua
    A peer-reviewed open-access journal ZooKeys 842: 85–112 (2019) New myrmarachnine jumping spiders 85 doi: 10.3897/zookeys.842.32970 RESEARCH ARTICLE http://zookeys.pensoft.net Launched to accelerate biodiversity research Myrmarachnine jumping spiders of the new subtribe Levieina from Papua New Guinea (Araneae, Salticidae, Myrmarachnini) Wayne P. Maddison1, Tamás Szűts2 1 Departments of Zoology and Botany and Beaty Biodiversity Museum, University of British Columbia, 6270 University Boulevard, Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T 1Z4, Canada 2 Department of Ecology, University of Veterinary Medicine Budapest, Budapest, H1077, Rottenbiller u. 50, Hungary Corresponding author: Wayne P. Maddison (wayne.maddison@ ubc.ca) Academic editor: Jeremy Miller | Received 10 January 2019 | Accepted 12 March 2019 | Published 7 May 2019 http://zoobank.org/D911C055-FF4B-4900-877B-123951761AC1 Citation: Maddison WP, Szűts T (2019) Myrmarachnine jumping spiders of the new subtribe Levieina from Papua New Guinea (Araneae, Salticidae, Myrmarachnini). ZooKeys 842: 85–112. https://doi.org/10.3897/zookeys.842.32970 Abstract A previously unreported radiation of myrmarachnine jumping spiders from New Guinea is described, which, although having few known species, is remarkably diverse in body forms. This clade is the new subtribe Levieina, represented by seven new species in three new genera. Within Leviea gen. n. are three new species, L. herberti sp. n., L. lornae sp. n., and L. francesae sp. n., all of which are unusual among the myrmarachnines in appearing as typical salticids, not antlike. Papuamyr gen. n. superficially resembles Ligonipes Karsch, 1878 or Rhombonotus L. Koch, 1879 as a compact antlike spider, but lacks their laterally- compressed palp and bears an ectal spur on the paturon of the chelicera.
    [Show full text]
  • Australasian Arachnology 83.Pdf
    Australasian Arachnology 83 Page 1 Australasian Arachnology 83 Page 2 THE AUSTRALASIAN ARTICLES ARACHNOLOGICAL SOCIETY The newsletter Australasian Arachnology depends on the contributions of members. www.australasian-arachnology.org Please send articles to the Editor: Acari – Araneae – Amblypygi – Opiliones – Palpigradi – Pseudoscorpiones – Pycnogonida – Michael G. Rix Schizomida – Scorpiones – Uropygi Department of Terrestrial Zoology Western Australian Museum The aim of the society is to promote interest in Locked Bag 49, Welshpool DC, W.A. 6986 the ecology, behaviour and taxonomy of Email: [email protected] arachnids of the Australasian region. Articles should be typed and saved as a MEMBERSHIP Microsoft Word document, with text in Times New Roman 12-point font. Only electronic Membership is open to all who have an interest email (preferred) or posted CD-ROM submiss- in arachnids – amateurs, students and ions will be accepted. professionals – and is managed by our Administrator (note new address ): Previous issues of the newsletter are available at http://www.australasian- Volker W. Framenau arachnology.org/newsletter/issues . Phoenix Environmental Sciences P.O. Box 857 LIBRARY Balcatta, W.A. 6914 Email: [email protected] For those members who do not have access to a scientific library, the society has a large number Membership fees in Australian dollars (per 4 of reference books, scientific journals and paper issues): reprints available, either for loan or as photo- *discount personal institutional copies. For all enquiries concerning publica- Australia $8 $10 $12 tions please contact our Librarian: NZ/Asia $10 $12 $14 Elsewhere $12 $14 $16 Jean-Claude Herremans There is no agency discount.
    [Show full text]
  • Directions in Conservation Biology Author(S): Graeme Caughley Source: Journal of Animal Ecology, Vol
    Directions in Conservation Biology Author(s): Graeme Caughley Source: Journal of Animal Ecology, Vol. 63, No. 2 (Apr., 1994), pp. 215-244 Published by: British Ecological Society Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/5542 Accessed: 12-02-2017 00:32 UTC REFERENCES Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article: http://www.jstor.org/stable/5542?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms British Ecological Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Animal Ecology This content downloaded from 134.197.56.15 on Sun, 12 Feb 2017 00:32:59 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Journal of Animal REVIEW Ecology 1994, 63, 215-244 Directions in conservation biology GRAEME CAUGHLEY CSIRO Division of Wildlife and Ecology, Box 84, Lyneham, Canberra, ACT 2602, Australia Summary 1. Conservation biology has two threads: the small-population paradigm which deals with the effect of smallness on the persistence of a population, and the declining- population paradigm which deals with the cause of smallness and its cure.
    [Show full text]
  • Crapemyrtle Bark Scale Acanthococcus Lagerstroemiae Kuwana (Hemiptera: Eriococcidae): Analysis of Factors Influencing Infestation and Control
    CRAPEMYRTLE BARK SCALE ACANTHOCOCCUS LAGERSTROEMIAE KUWANA (HEMIPTERA: ERIOCOCCIDAE): ANALYSIS OF FACTORS INFLUENCING INFESTATION AND CONTROL A Thesis by KYLE ANDREW GILDER Submitted to the Office of Graduate and Professional Studies of Texas A&M University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Chair of Committee, Kevin M. Heinz Co-Chair of Committee, Mengmeng Gu Committee Members, Mike Merchant Head of Department, Phillip Kaufman December 2020 Major Subject: Entomology Copyright 2020 Kyle Andrew Gilder ABSTRACT Crapemyrtle bark scale, Acanthococcus lagerstroemiae (Kuwana), a new non-native pest from Asia first discovered in the U.S. in 2004 has now been reported in 14 states. The scale jeopardizes the future of crapemyrtles use as a popular ornamental landscape tree in the U.S. Management of this pest will likely include biological strategies. Before such strategies can be implemented it is important to examine relative abundances and distributions of arthropod species associated with the scale in the geographic area targeted for biological control. In the first objective, surveys of crapemyrtle ecology from two varietal groups of crapemyrtle trees (Lagerstroemia spp.) were undertaken in Tarrant and Brazos counties across six consecutive seasons in 2018 – 2019. A rich arthropod community was discovered. The most common predators were spiders, coccinellids, and chrysopids. Insects in the families Eriococcidae, Aphididae, and Thripidae were common herbivores on Lagerstroemia spp. Numerous phytophagous and mycophagous mites were also collected. These herbivores constitute a reservoir of alternative prey for generalist predators that may also feed on A. lagerstroemiae. A food web was constructed to illustrate direct and indirect effects of the predator community on A.
    [Show full text]
  • Clark Thesis.Pdf (8.256Mb)
    THE ROLE OF CHEMICAL CUES , IN THE PREDATORY AND ANTI-PREDATORY BEHAVIOUR OF JU~PING SPIDERS (ARANEAE, SAL TICIDAE) A Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in, Zoology at the University of Canterbury by Robert John Clark 2000 CONTENTS Abstract 1· Chapter 1: Introduction 3 Chapter 2: Theoretical background 9 Chapter 3: Chemical cues elicit prey capture in P. fimbriata 56 Chapter 4: Web use during predatory encounters between P. fimbriata, an araneophagic 91 jumping spider, and its preferred prey, other jumping spiders Chapter 5: Speculative hunting by an araneophagic jumping spider 108 Chapter 6: Chemical cues from ants influence predatory behaviour in Habrocestum pulex 125 (Hentz), an ant eating jumping spider (Araneae, Salticidae) Chapter 7: Reactions of Habrocestum pulex, a myrmecophagic salticid, to potential 147 kairomones from ants Chapter 8: Dragllnes and assessment of fighting ability in cannibalistic jumping spiders 160 Chapter 9: Relationship between violent aggression in saltlcids and use of pheromones to 178 obtain information on conspeclfics Chapter10: Discussion 189 Acknowledgements 198 References 199 2 9 MAR 2000 1 ABSTRACT The role of chemical cues in prey-capture behaviour is studied in jumping spiders (Salticldae). Prior to this study, little attention has been given to how chemical cues influence the predatory behaviour of these spiders with complex eyes and visual acuity unrivalled In any other animals of comparable size. Three categories of predation are considered: salticids preying on conspecifics (cannibalism), salticids preying on non-conspecific spiders (araneophagy) and salticids preying on ants (myrmecophagy). Primary study animals are Portia spp.
    [Show full text]
  • Spiders from Red Mercury Island, by D. J. Court, P 87-9012 Spiders From
    87 SPIDERS FROM RED MERCURY ISLAND. by D.J. Court* SUMMARY Spiders were collected from Red Mercury Island in August, 1971. Abundance and habitats of twenty-six genera including thirty-four species are noted. The occurrence and distribution of certain species are discussed. INTRODUCTION The study of spiders in New Zealand is still essentially at the taxonomic stage and few comprehensive collections of spiders from a restricted locality have been described. The present study is therefore limited, as most spiders can be identified only to generic level and no basis for comparison exists for collections of spiders. It is of interest to find how many introduced species are present on this island. A number of species introduced to New Zealand would be expected, as the prevailing westerly winds could carry spiderlings over the comparatively short distance from the mainland. On Red Mercury Island, the number of spider habitats appears reduced when compared with those of the mainland; habitat diversity however remains wide. Spiders were collected systematically from all parts of the island. Within the coastal forest, sifting of leaf litter provided a few specimens only, many more spiders being found on tree trunks or foliage. An interesting fauna was found in the maritime and spray zones, and very large populations were present on sheltered coastal cliff faces. The collection is held at the Otago Museum, Dunedin. The abundance scale given here is subjective, being based upon ease of capture and general obser• vations. Nomenclature follows Forster (1967, 70) and Forster and Wilton (1968). GENERA AND HABITATS Order Araneae Sub-order Mygalomorpha Dipluridae Aparua sp.
    [Show full text]
  • International Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol
    International Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol. 8, No. 2, 1995 JUMPING SPIDERS ALTERNATE TURNS IN THE ABSENCE OF VISUAL CUES Phillip W. Taylor University of Canterbury, New Zealand ABSTRACT: The abilities of four species of diurnal jumping spiders (Helpis minitabunda, Portia fimbriata, Trite auricoma, and Trite planiceps) and one species of nocturnal clubionid spider (Clubiona cambridgei) to maintain approximately straight paths by alternating turns in the absence of visual cues was investigated. Under infra-red light (observed using infra-red video), individual spiders were run through a maze comprising a single forced turn and then a choice of turning in the same or opposite direction to the forced turn. At the second (free) turn, each species turned in the direction opposite to the forced turn (i.e., alternated turns) more frequently than it turned in the same direction. There was no evidence that species differed in tendency to alternate turns. In nature, jumping spiders may use this ability to navigate in the absence of visual cues when foraging or escaping predators in darkness. It is suggested that alternation of turns by jumping spiders depends on proprioceptive information gathered during previous turns. INTRODUCTION Turn alternation is the tendency of animals to turn in the direction opposite to the previous turn after a forced turn or at successive choice situations. This tendency has been described in a wide taxonomic range of animals (for review, see Hughes, 1989), and it has been suggested that turn alternation is used by animals in nature as a means of approximating straight paths in the absence of external directional cues (Dingle, 1965; Hughes, 1978).
    [Show full text]
  • Phylogenomic Analysis of Copepoda (Arthropoda, Crustacea) Reveals Unexpected Similarities with Earlier Proposed Morphological Ph
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Papers from the Nebraska Center for Biotechnology Biotechnology, Center for 1-2017 Phylogenomic analysis of Copepoda (Arthropoda, Crustacea) reveals unexpected similarities with earlier proposed morphological phylogenies Seong-il Eyun University of Nebraska - Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/biotechpapers Part of the Biotechnology Commons, Molecular, Cellular, and Tissue Engineering Commons, Other Genetics and Genomics Commons, and the Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecology Commons Eyun, Seong-il, "Phylogenomic analysis of Copepoda (Arthropoda, Crustacea) reveals unexpected similarities with earlier proposed morphological phylogenies" (2017). Papers from the Nebraska Center for Biotechnology. 10. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/biotechpapers/10 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Biotechnology, Center for at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Papers from the Nebraska Center for Biotechnology by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Eyun BMC Evolutionary Biology (2017) 17:23 DOI 10.1186/s12862-017-0883-5 RESEARCHARTICLE Open Access Phylogenomic analysis of Copepoda (Arthropoda, Crustacea) reveals unexpected similarities with earlier proposed morphological phylogenies Seong-il Eyun Abstract Background: Copepods play a critical role in marine ecosystems but have been poorly investigated in phylogenetic studies. Morphological evidence supports the monophyly of copepods, whereas interordinal relationships continue to be debated. In particular, the phylogenetic position of the order Harpacticoida is still ambiguous and inconsistent among studies. Until now, a small number of molecular studies have been done using only a limited number or even partial genes and thus there is so far no consensus at the order-level.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstract Book
    ABSTRACT BOOK Canterbury, New Zealand 10–15 February 2019 21st International Congress of Arachnology ORGANISING COMMITTEE MAIN ORGANISERS Cor Vink Peter Michalik Curator of Natural History Curator of the Zoological Museum Canterbury Museum University of Greifswald Rolleston Avenue, Christchurch Loitzer Str 26, Greifswald New Zealand Germany LOCAL ORGANISING COMMITTEE Ximena Nelson (University of Canterbury) Adrian Paterson (Lincoln University) Simon Pollard (University of Canterbury) Phil Sirvid (Museum of New Zealand, Te Papa Tongarewa) Victoria Smith (Canterbury Museum) SCIENTIFIC COMMITTEE Anita Aisenberg (IICBE, Uruguay) Miquel Arnedo (University of Barcelona, Spain) Mark Harvey (Western Australian Museum, Australia) Mariella Herberstein (Macquarie University, Australia) Greg Holwell (University of Auckland, New Zealand) Marco Isaia (University of Torino, Italy) Lizzy Lowe (Macquarie University, Australia) Anne Wignall (Massey University, New Zealand) Jonas Wolff (Macquarie University, Australia) 21st International Congress of Arachnology 1 INVITED SPEAKERS Plenary talk, day 1 Sensory systems, learning, and communication – insights from amblypygids to humans Eileen Hebets University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Nebraska, USA E-mail: [email protected] Arachnids encompass tremendous diversity with respect to their morphologies, their sensory systems, their lifestyles, their habitats, their mating rituals, and their interactions with both conspecifics and heterospecifics. As such, this group of often-enigmatic arthropods offers unlimited and sometimes unparalleled opportunities to address fundamental questions in ecology, evolution, physiology, neurobiology, and behaviour (among others). Amblypygids (Order Amblypygi), for example, possess distinctly elongated walking legs covered with sensory hairs capable of detecting both airborne and substrate-borne chemical stimuli, as well as mechanoreceptive information. Simultaneously, they display an extraordinary central nervous system with distinctly large and convoluted higher order processing centres called mushroom bodies.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploitation of Artificial Light at Night by a Diurnal Jumping Spider
    Peckhamia 78.1 Exploitation of artificial light by a jumping spider 1 PECKHAMIA 78.1, 12 December 2009, 1―3 ISSN 1944―8120 Exploitation of artificial light at night by a diurnal jumping spider Kenneth D. Frank 1 1 2508 Pine Street, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19103, email [email protected] Abstract Platycryptus undatus (De Geer 1778) (Araneae: Salticidae) stalked and seized insects attracted to porch lamps at night at two sites on Cape Cod, Massachusetts. Ordinarily this spider hunts during the day. Key words: salticid, night light niche, artificial lighting Few animals exploit artificial lighting at night for tasks that they ordinarily perform exclusively during the day. An example is Platycryptus undatus (De Geer 1778), a moderately large (9―12 mm) jumping spider common in the eastern and midwestern United States (Comstock 1940). The author and two others (Robert L. and Eric H. Edwards) have observed members of this species stalking and pouncing on prey near an artificial light at night. Solitary males and females of this species attacked insects attracted to a porch lamp at night at a suburban house at Quisset near Woods Hole, Massachusetts. This house is the residence of one of us. The spiders have hunted at the lamp every summer for several decades. In 2002, we noted this behavior at a porch lamp on another house, 6 km away near East Falmouth. This paper reports our observations during July and August of 2002 at Quisset. The porch lamp contained one frosted 60―watt incandescent light bulb face down in a close-fitting clear glass housing a few centimeters from the wall.
    [Show full text]
  • Genus Corambis Simon, 1901
    ZOBODAT - www.zobodat.at Zoologisch-Botanische Datenbank/Zoological-Botanical Database Digitale Literatur/Digital Literature Zeitschrift/Journal: European Journal of Taxonomy Jahr/Year: 2019 Band/Volume: 0578 Autor(en)/Author(s): Patoleta Barbara, Zabka Marek Artikel/Article: Salticidae (Arachnida: Araneae) of New Caledonia: Genus Corambis Simon, 1901 1-18 European Journal of Taxonomy 578: 1–18 ISSN 2118-9773 https://doi.org/10.5852/ejt.2019.578 www.europeanjournaloftaxonomy.eu 2019 · Patoleta B. & Żabka M. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution Licence (CC BY 4.0). Research article urn:lsid:zoobank.org:pub:0BE6D99E-905D-4094-A03F-1C40D3C07CC2 Salticidae (Arachnida: Araneae) of New Caledonia: Genus Corambis Simon, 1901 Barbara PATOLETA 1,* & Marek ŻABKA 2 1,2 Siedlce University of Natural Sciences and Humanities, Faculty of Exact and Natural Sciences, Prusa 14, 08-110 Siedlce, Poland. * Corresponding author: [email protected] 2 Email: [email protected] 1 urn:lsid:zoobank.org:author:321AD5CC-4081-4319-882D-E2B3F80DB458 2 urn:lsid:zoobank.org:author:65CFB20A-8CC4-43EA-9CDF-E21D2D816C68 Abstract. The genus Corambis Simon, 1901 includes fi ve species; three of them, C. jacknicholsoni sp. nov., C. logunovi sp. nov. and C. pantherae sp. nov., are described here as new. The female of the C. foeldvarii Szűts, 2002 is described for the fi rst time and a new generic diagnosis is proposed. The distribution and relationships of Corambis are discussed in terms of the geological and bioclimatic history of New Caledonia. Keywords. Jumping spiders, New Caledonia, endemism, taxonomy. Patoleta B. & Żabka M. 2019. Salticidae (Arachnida: Araneae) of New Caledonia: Genus Corambis Simon, 1901.
    [Show full text]