PSYN-09876; No of Pages 10 Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychresns

The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary functional magnetic resonance imaging study of delay discounting of monetary and cigarette rewards in smokers

James MacKillop a,b,⁎, Michael T. Amlung a, Lauren M. Wier c, Sean P. David d,e, Lara A. Ray f, Warren K. Bickel g, Lawrence H. Sweet h a Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, United States b Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States c Boston University School of Public Health, Boston, MA, United States d Department of Community Medicine, Stanford University School of Medicine, Palo Alto, CA, United States e SRI International, Menlo Park, CA, United States f Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, United States g Center for Substance Abuse Research, Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, Roanoke, VA, United States h Translational Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States article info abstract

Article history: Neuroeconomics integrates behavioral economics and cognitive neuroscience to understand the neurobio- Received 18 March 2011 logical basis for normative and maladaptive decision making. Delay discounting is a behavioral economic Received in revised form 22 August 2011 index of impulsivity that reflects capacity to delay gratification and has been consistently associated with Accepted 7 October 2011 nicotine dependence. This preliminary study used functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine Available online xxxx delay discounting for money and cigarette rewards in 13 nicotine dependent adults. Significant differences between preferences for smaller immediate rewards and larger delayed rewards were evident in a number Keywords: Nicotine dependence of regions of interest (ROIs), including the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior insular cortex, middle temporal Smoking gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, and cingulate gyrus. Significant differences between money and cigarette re- Tobacco wards were generally lateralized, with cigarette choices associated with left hemisphere activation and Behavioral economics money choices associated with right hemisphere activation. Specific ROI differences included the posterior Neuroeconomics parietal cortex, medial and middle frontal gyrus, ventral striatum, temporoparietal cortex, and angular Delay discounting gyrus. Impulsivity as measured by behavioral choices was significantly associated with both individual Impulsivity ROIs and a combined ROI model. These findings provide initial evidence in support of applying a neuroeco- nomic approach to understanding nicotine dependence. © 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction economic burden throughout the world (Wipfli and Samet, 2009a, 2009b). Smoking is believed to be largely motivated by clinical or 1.1. Understanding nicotine dependence using behavioral economics and subclinical levels of nicotine dependence and understanding the neuroeconomics factors that cause and maintain nicotine dependence may improve prevention and treatment (Ray et al., 2009). Cigarette smoking remains the single largest cause of preventable Behavioral economics, a hybrid field integrating insights from psy- morbidity and mortality in the US and the world (Mokdad et al., chology and economics, has been extensively applied to understand 2004; World Health Organization, 2008). It is an established cause nicotine dependence and other addictive behaviors (for reviews, see of cardiovascular disease, respiratory disease, and an array of cancers Bickel and Vuchinich, 2003; Vuchinich and Heather, 2003). In partic- (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2003; 2008) and is esti- ular, high levels of delay discounting (DD), a behavioral economic mated to be annually responsible for 450,000 deaths in the United index of impulsivity, have been consistently associated with smoking States and 5,000,000 deaths worldwide (World Health Organization, (Reynolds, 2006a; MacKillop et al., 2011). Specifically, delay discount- 2008). Beyond disease burden, smoking also exerts a massive ing characterizes how much a person devalues a reward based on its delay in time, reflecting capacity to delay gratification. This form of impulsivity is particularly relevant to nicotine dependence and ⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, United States. Tel.: +1 706 542 3033; fax: +1 706 542 8048. other substance use disorders because these conditions prototypically E-mail address: [email protected] (J. MacKillop). reflect persistent preferences for the transient short-term reward of

0925-4927/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 2 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx the drug at the cost of much larger long-term outcomes in an array of activity in stimulant dependent individuals in the dorsolateral PFC, domains. Moreover, impulsive discounting has been linked with nic- precuneus, and VS compared to controls. No studies, to our knowl- otine dependence in numerous studies. Compared to non-smokers edge, have applied a neuroeconomic approach to understanding nic- and ex-smokers, nicotine dependent individuals exhibit significantly otine dependence or examined differences in impulsivity for both more impulsive delay discounting (Bickel et al., 1999; Mitchell, domain-general rewards and addictive commodities. 1999; Baker et al., 2003; Reynolds et al., 2004; Reynolds, 2006b) and, among smokers, greater impulsivity is associated with greater 1.2. Current investigation cigarette consumption and nicotine dependence (Epstein et al., 2003; Reynolds, 2006b; Sweitzer et al., 2008). In addition, more im- The current study was a preliminary investigation of the differential pulsive delay discounting has been found to predict the onset brain activity associated with delay discounting of monetary rewards of smoking over the course of adolescence (Audrain-McGovern and cigarette rewards in nicotine dependent adults. Clarifying the differ- et al., 2009) and is a negative prognostic factor in smoking cessation ences in brain activity responsible for general rewards versus cigarette (Krishnan-Sarin et al., 2007; Yoon et al., 2007; MacKillop and rewards may further inform the maladaptive decision-making that Kahler, 2009; Sheffer et al., 2012). In most studies delay discounting maintains nicotine dependence. Based on previous studies of delay dis- is assessed using measures employing the domain-general commodity counting in healthy adults, we predicted that discounting would be char- of money (i.e., a reward that can be used in a variety of different do- acterized by activity in the medial and inferior PFC, AIC, PPC, PC, and VS. mains), but discounting can also be assessed for domain-specificcom- In addition, we predicted that, a commodity effect would be present for modities (i.e., a commodity that is a specific type of reward), including behavioral choices (i.e., greater impulsivity for cigarette rewards) and, cigarettes. Several studies have found nicotine dependent individuals for brain activity, although activity would be similarly localized to to exhibit even greater discounting for cigarette rewards (Bickel et al., money choices, there would be significant differences in activity magni- 1999; Baker et al., 2003) and other drugs (Madden et al., 1997; Petry, tude compared to money, reflecting the more impulsive orientation 2001; Coffey et al., 2003). This suggests that nicotine dependent individ- toward cigarette rewards. uals and individuals with other addictive disorders are both more impul- sive in general and particularly so when it comes to their drug of choice. 2. Method and materials A major step forward in behavioral economics has come from in- tegrations of its methods with those from cognitive neuroscience, 2.1. Participants commonly termed neuroeconomics. This is particularly the case for delay discounting, which has been perhaps the most extensively Fifteen participants were recruited from the community using studied decision-making process to date. Using functional magnetic advertisements. Inclusion criteria were: 1) 18 years-old or older; resonance imaging (fMRI), a number of studies have revealed a pro- 2) daily self-reported smoking of 20+ cigarettes; 3) expired carbon file of cortical and subcortical brain regions that appear to be respon- monoxide (CO) of 10+ ppm; 4) Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence sible for discounting preferences (McClure et al., 2004; Kable and (FTND) score of 6+ (Heatherton et al., 1991); 5) no interest in quitting Glimcher, 2007; Ballard and Knutson, 2009; Bickel et al., 2009). A re- smoking within the next year; 6) right handed. Exclusion criteria were: cent meta-analysis of fMRI discounting studies identified common re- 1) any contraindicating conditions for magnetic resonance imaging gions of significant activation across diverse methodologies and (MRI) screening; 2) pregnancy (females only; confirmed by First samples (Carter et al., 2010), including differential activation in the Response® pregnancy test); 3) living with anyone else enrolled in the medial and inferior prefrontal cortex (PFC), the anterior insular cor- study. Two participants who completed the protocol were subsequently tex (AIC), the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), posterior cingulate excluded based on missing and incorrect responses to control items in (PC), and subcortical limbic activation in the ventral striatum (VS). the scanner, suggesting low effort. No data from those participants From a functional standpoint, these regions are theorized to reflect were included. Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. two conflicting systems, one comprising motivational brain regions that are responsible for drive, reward, and incentive value (e.g., AIC, 2.2. Assessment VS) and the other comprising regions responsible for behavioral inhi- bition and future orientation (e.g., PFC, PPC) (McClure et al., 2004; 2.2.1. Self-report and biological indices Bechara, 2005; Bickel et al., 2007). Metaphorically, the motivational Eligibility for the MRI scan was assessed using an MRI safety and con- regions represent the “gas pedal” and the inhibitory regions represent traindication screening questionnaire. Demographics were assessed via a the “brakes,” with observed preferences being determined by the self-report questionnaire. Nicotine dependence and cigarettes/day were balance of the two. However, there is also evidence supporting a assessed using the psychometrically validated FTND (Heatherton et al., more general single system approach (Kable and Glimcher, 2007; 1991). Expired CO was assessed using a PiCO+ Smokerlyzer (Bedfont Monterosso and Luo, 2010). Scientific, Ltd; Rochester, UK). In spite of its promise, there have been limited applications of neuroeconomics to nicotine dependence and other substance use dis- Table 1 orders. To date, three cross-sectional studies have examined dis- Participant characteristics (N=13). FTND=Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence. counting using fMRI, one studying alcohol dependent individuals Characteristic #/mean (S.D.)/(median) and two studying stimulant dependent individuals. In the first case, Boettiger et al. (2007) found differential PFC, PPC, and hippocampal Sex 7 males/6 females Age 40.15 (13.10) activation in comparing alcohol dependent individuals to controls. Race 9 Whites, 1 Black, 1 Native American/Pacific Islander, With regard to stimulant dependence, two studies examined dis- 2 Asians counting in individuals with stimulant dependence and controls Hispanic ethnicity 12 non-Hispanic/1 Hispanic (Monterosso et al., 2007; Hoffman et al., 2008). These studies used Income $20,000–$29,999/annum (median) “easy” and “hard” discounting choices which were determined using Cigarettes/day 22.31 (8.55) FTND 6.54 (1.20) pre-scan assessment. Monterosso et al. (2007) found that stimulant fMRI responses dependent individuals exhibited less differential PFC and PPC activa- Control choices 34 (3.28) tion relative to controls in a contrast of “easy” and “hard” choices. Impulsive choices 52 (10.46) This was theorized to reflect more inefficient cognitive processing. Restrained 20 (10.71) Hoffman et al. (2008) identified similar patterns, with reduced choices

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx 3

2.2.2. Delay discounting paradigm shot gradient echo pulse sequence (repetition time (TR)=2500 ms, The delay discounting paradigm was based on the Monetary Choice echo time (TE)=28 ms, field of view=1922 mm, matrix=642,voxel Questionnaire (MCQ; Kirby et al., 1999), which has been previously size=3 mm3, with 42 contiguous 3 mm slices collected axially). Two used to study discounting in nicotine dependent adults (e.g., dummy TRs, for which no data were collected, preceded the functional MacKillop and Kahler, 2009). The items presented two amounts of scans to permit the scanner to reach steady-state equilibrium. money or cigarettes, each with an accompanying interval of time Participants completed four delay discounting decision-making (Fig. 1). The active stimuli were dichotomous choices between smaller runs in an event-related design. Each imaging run comprised 27 immediate rewards (money or cigarettes) and larger delayed rewards total items, with 18 active items and 9 control items (run dura- (money or cigarettes), and are provided in the supplementary mate- tion=5:20 min). Each item was presented for 6000 ms for observa- rials. Control items were dichotomous choices between larger and tion and response input, followed by jittered inter-stimulus interval smaller rewards (money or cigarettes) that were both available today. (ISI) (average duration=4000 ms). Monetary rewards were pre- Cigarette amounts were generated by converting the monetary amounts sented during the first and third runs and cigarette rewards were in the MCQ by a conversion factor of $4.63/pack, the approximate na- presented during the second and fourth runs. tional average cost of a pack of cigarettes (Boonn, 2007). Participants To ensure maximum relevance and salience, participants received were informed that the cigarettes in the task referred to their preferred one actual outcome from their choices. Specifically, following the brand. Participants' responses were coded as “impulsive” (i.e., selecting scan, participants selected one poker chip from a fishbowl containing the smaller immediate reward over the larger delayed reward), a chip for every item and received their choice for the item to which “restrained” (i.e., selecting the larger delayed reward over the smaller the poker chip pertained. This applied to all items, including both immediate reward), or “control” (i.e., selecting the larger immediate active and control and monetary rewards and cigarette rewards. If a reward over the smaller immediate reward). Level of impulsivity exhib- participant selected the immediate reward for their outcome choice, ited during the scans as a function of choices made was quantified as the they received that reward as they left the session; if participants hyperbolic discounting function, k (Mazur, 1987). Responses were received a delayed reward, they received the reward by mail or in- entered with the index and middle finger of the right hand on an MRI- person, at their preference, after the prescribed number of days. compatible response box placed on the participant's hip. 2.3. Procedure 2.2.3. Functional neuroimaging protocol Imaging data were collected at the Brown University Magnetic Res- Participants were recruited from the community using advertise- onance Imaging Facility on a Siemens 3 T TIM Trio scanner. Structural ments. Those appearing eligible after a telephone screen were invited imaging used a high-resolution T1 scan (voxel size 1 mm3, field of for an in-person screen. There, eligible participants were given an view=1922 mm, matrix=2562, slice thickness=1 mm). Functional overview of the study and those interested were enrolled and under- imaging used echo planar imaging (EPI) of T2* scans using a single- went informed consent. Enrolled participants were also given a more

Fig. 1. Delayed reward discounting (DRD) paradigm. Active stimuli reflect choices between smaller immediately available money or cigarette rewards compared to larger delayed amounts of money and cigarettes, whereas control items had no delay. Notes: the cigarette amounts are the corresponding numbers based on the conversion factor used in the study itself; ISI=inter-stimulus interval; s=second.

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 4 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx thorough orientation to the study's procedures, including the types of prefrontal, anterior insular, posterior parietal, and inferior prefrontal choices they would be making (without disclosing stimuli) and the cortices (all bilaterally); the left posterior cingulate and temporopar- fact they would receive one of those choices. ietal cortices; and the right ventral striatum. Comparisons for a priori At the imaging session, participants were first asked to smoke a ROI analyses were between mean t-statistics from each sphere and cigarette to ensure no significant withdrawal effects and equate used only the active stimuli because the a priori ROIs were based on exposure to tobacco. The participants completed a 1-hour orientation previous studies comparing active stimuli. Second, functional ROIs to the procedures, including a practice scan in a demonstration MRI are defined as ROIs that are identified based on their empirical levels device. The MRI scan was then completed and the randomly selected of activation. In this case, we examined our hypotheses in regions choice was determined, after which participants concluded the ses- recruited by the fMRI paradigm in our sample, using both the active sion with a debriefing interview. and control items. Specifically, we generated a disjunction (Boolean In terms of compensation, participants received $15 for the in- “OR” logic) mask for choices (impulsive, restrained, or control) and a person screen, regardless of outcome, and $30 for the MRI session, second for commodity (cigarette or money discounting choices), both plus one of their in-scanner choices, with a maximum value of $85. in comparison to activity during rest (ISI). This strategy has been vali- For the in-scanner choices, all participants received choices resulting dated in previous fMRI studies on discounting (e.g., Ballard and in immediate rewards, except one, who received a reward delayed by Knutson, 2009) and nicotine dependence (e.g., Lawrence et al., 2002; 14 days; 62% of the participants received monetary rewards and Sweet et al., 2010). Each mask includes all clusters of significant differ- 38% received cigarette rewards. All procedures were approved by ences in activity from all conditions, which avoids bias from any one the Brown University Institutional Review Board. condition. Functional ROIs were defined using a family-wise error rate of pb0.005 with a minimum cluster size of five adjacent voxels 2.4. Data analysis (voxel-wise error rate=0.000016) (Ward, 2000). This cluster thresh- old further minimizes Type I error because false positive voxel activity Prior to examining the fMRI data, participants' behavioral data is predicted to take place at random and not expected to aggregate in obtained during the scan were examined for validity, consistency of multiple adjacent voxels, whereas the opposite is true for true positive responding, consistency across runs, level of impulsive discounting signals (Lazar, 2008). Commodity effect analyses only included active exhibited and reaction time. With regard to discounting, impulsivity items, not control items. Hypothesis testing was conducted using paired was operationalized as the hyperbolic discounting function, k (Mazur, t-tests and analysis of variance (ANOVA) with mean task-associated 1987), and derived using standard MCQ methods (Kirby et al., 1999). brain response per ROI per individual as the dependent variable. Finally, Functional imaging data processing and analysis were conducted associations between brain activity and behavioral performance were using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages software (AFNI; Cox, examined to clarify the most relevant regions. Specifically, zero-order cor- 1996) with follow-up analyses using SPSS 16.0. Of note, technical relations (Pearson's r) were examined between the a priori and function- problems resulted in data loss during one imaging run for two partic- ally defined regions of interest within choice type to determine overall ipants, resulting in 75% of data. Functional datasets were initially associations. To determine specificity of association, stepwise regression aligned to the T1 anatomical dataset, volume-registered (motion- was used to enter all relevant candidates into regression and retain only corrected), and normalized into Talairach space using AFNI script the most relevant regions. These analyses used a conventional p≤0.05. align_epi_anat.py (Saad et al., 2009). Individual volumes from each of the four runs were registered to a base volume proximal to acquisition 3. Results of the anatomical dataset (i.e., the third volume of the first delay dis- counting run). The data were then spatially smoothed using a 3-mm 3.1. Delay discounting decision-making performance full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter, excluding non- brain voxels. Raw blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal was Analysis of behavioral performance suggested sufficient effort and scaled to percent signal change from the mean signal intensity and all overall valid task performance. The percentage of responses consid- four runs were concatenated together. First level analyses of individual ered valid responses was high (M=99%, range=96–100%); valid re- brain responses were characterized using separate general linear models sponses were defined as a control trial in which the larger immediate for choice type and commodity type. For discounting decision-making in reward was selected over the smaller immediate reward. In addition, general, 3dDeconvolve (Ward, 2006) applied three task-related regres- responses were provided in time for 99% of items and participants' sors (impulsive, restrained, and control) and six nuisance regressors to responses were generally highly consistent, with 98% (standard devi- account formation (x, y, z, roll, pitch, yaw); the regression model also in- ation (S.D.)=4.57; range 90–100%) of overall choices being non- corporated linear and quadratic trends in the data. Of note, we did not contradictory responses. Comparisons between equivalent items across also use a domain-general comparison of all discounting items with con- runs 1/3 and 2/4 revealed no differences (ps≥0.50), supporting the ag- trol items as the choice events were known to be of two opposite types gregation of equivalent items within the same commodity. There was (e.g., impulsive and restrained) and the primary questions of no evidence of a commodity effect in comparing money and cigarette interest pertained to differences based on those types. For commodity discounting (k Money=0.075, k Cigarettes=0.075; F [1, 12]=1.30, differences, the same approach was used but with two task-related p=0.28); the associated discounting curves are provided in the supple- regressors (monetary choices, cigarette choices). Mixed comparisons of mentary materials. Participants provided an average of 74% impulsive choice type and commodity type were not conducted because of low choices and 26% restrained choices. Using a factorial 3 (choice type: im- numbers of individual choices. In both cases, individual t-statistics per pulsive, restrained, control)×2 (commodity: money, cigarettes) ANOVA, voxel were used in group-level region of interest (ROI) analyses. there was a significant main effect of choice type (F [2, 1333]=51.18, 2 Both a priori and ‘data-driven’ functional ROI strategies were used. pb0.0001, ηp =0.08), but not of commodity type (F [1, 1333]=0.15, This was because some aspects of the study overlapped with previous p=0.70) or an interaction effect (F [2, 1333]=0.48, p=0.62) (Fig. 2). studies using discounting paradigms, but the novel focus on discount- Participants were fastest in responding to control stimuli, intermediate ing of cigarette rewards made the possibility of unique regions of for impulsive choices, and slowest for the restrained choices. activation highly plausible. Thus, the study could both leverage estab- lished regions of activation but be able to detect novel regions. First, a 3.2. Neurocognitive associates of delay discounting priori ROIs were identified based on the recent meta-analysis of fMRI discounting studies. Specifically, we generated 5-mm-radius ROIs Comparisons of impulsive and restrained decision making in the a centered on the Carter et al. (2010) coordinates for the medial priori ROIs revealed significant differences in two regions, medial PFC

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx 5

Fig. 2. Response time by decision-making choice type. Significance levels reflect left comparisons; *** pb0.001. and AIC (Table 2). In both cases, impulsive choices were associated Fig. 3. Significant choice type and commodity type differences in the a priori ROIs. with lower activity in these brain regions, whereas restrained choices Panel A presents significant differences based on choice type, Panel B presents signifi- were associated with higher activity (Fig. 3A), suggesting that recruit- cant differences based on commodity type; * pb0.05, ** pb0.01, *** pb0.001. Abbrevi- ment of these areas plays an important role in the inhibition of a pref- ations: MePFC=medial prefrontal cortex; AIC-R=anterior insular cortex (right); erence for an immediate reward. Seven functionally defined ROIs VS=ventral striatum; TPL-L=temporoparietal lobe (left); I-PFC-L=inferior prefron- tal cortex (left); PPC-L/R=posterior parietal cortex (left)/(right); AI-PFC-R=anterior were identified (Fig. 4), reflecting regions associated with significant- inferior prefrontal cortex (right). ly different activity compared to rest at least in one of the three choice types (Table 3). Of these seven, significant differences between choice types were evident for five ROIs and reflected several different pat- temporal gyrus), and regions that were only specific to restrained terns. These patterns included regions that were specific to intertem- choices (e.g., cingulate gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, superior frontal poral choice decision making in general (e.g., medial frontal gyrus), gyrus). More simply, the different behavioral choice outcomes were regions that were only specific to impulsive choices (e.g., middle associated with different profiles of brain activity.

Table 2 Comparisons of choice type and commodity type in the a priori regions of interest. 3.3. Neurocognitive associates of choices for cigarettes vs. money Significant effects are in boldface R=right; L=left.

Talairach Impulsive vs. Money vs. With regard to commodity effects, the a priori ROIs revealed signifi- coordinates restrained cigarettes cant differences in six regions (Table 2), including VS, bilateral PPC and choices inferior PFC, and temporal parietal cortex (Fig. 5). Differences in activa- Region of interest xyzt(12) pt(12) p tion are presented in Fig. 3B and revealed three regions of significantly Medial prefrontal cortex (L) −24018−2.37 0.04 −0.05 0.96 greater activity for money choices (VS, left temporoparietal lobe, right Ventral striatum (R) 14 10 0 −1.53 0.15 3.43 b0.01 anterior inferior PFC) and cigarette choices (left inferior PFC, left and Posterior cingulate cortex (L) −8 −36 36 0.15 0.88 0.77 0.45 right PPC), respectively. Nineteen functionally defined ROIs were identi- − − − Anterior insular cortex (R) 36 18 2 2.25 0.04 0.07 0.95 fied (Table 4) and, of these, cigarette choices were associated with great- Anterior insular cortex (L) −30 18 −6 −1.69 0.12 0.58 0.57 Temporoparietal cortex (L) −48 −66 14 −0.69 0.50 2.66 0.02 er activity for 11 and choices for monetary rewards were associated with Posterior parietal cortex (L) −28 −60 44 0.20 0.84 −3.53 b0.01 greater activity for eight. Both commodities elicited activity in the inferi- Posterior parietal cortex (R) 26 −60 46 0.78 0.45 −3.04 0.01 or frontal gyrus and cuneus, albeit in different locations. Cigarette deci- Inferior prefrontal cortex (R) 48 34 10 −0.12 0.90 3.27 b0.01 sions were associated with greater activity in the medial frontal gyrus, − − − b Inferior prefrontal cortex (L) 42 4 30 1.27 0.23 3.89 0.01 middle frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, and superior parietal lobule,

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 6 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx

Fig. 4. Choice type disjunction mask indicating regions of significant differences in activity compared to rest (pb0.005, minimum cluster size=5 voxels). Radiological conventions are used and side of the brain is indicated by R or L (right, left). Note: MeFG=medial frontal gyrus; MFG=middle frontal gyrus; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; SPL=superior parietal lobule; CG=cingulate gyrus; PCN=precuneus; MTG=medial temporal gyrus. whereas money decisions were associated with significantly greater ac- successful delay of gratification. These regions were complemented tivity in the angular gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and occipital cortex. by the functionally defined ROIs, which were related to diverse as- pects of the task. Specifically, there was evidence that the precuneus 3.4. Relationships between brain activity and behavioral performance and superior parietal lobule were implicated in all of the types of decision making, that the medial frontal gyrus was associated with Of the a priori and functional ROIs that were implicated in choice- both types of intertemporal choice, that the middle temporal gyrus type differences, four regions were associated with behavioral impul- was specifically associated with choices for smaller immediate re- sivity at statistically significant, high magnitude levels (rs≥0.58, wards and that the middle frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, and superior psb0.05), including the VS, medial frontal gyrus, middle temporal frontal gyrus were specifically associated with preferences for larger gyrus, and temporal parietal cortex, and several were significantly delayed rewards. These findings generally converge with previous intercorrelated (Table 5). To determine specificity, stepwise regres- neuroeconomic studies and the regions implicated largely overlap sion concurrently examined all four and identified two regions that with those reported in the recent discounting activation likelihood were responsible for maximum unique variance in behavioral perfor- estimation (ALE) meta-analysis (Carter et al., 2010). These include mance. The observed level of impulsivity was largely a function of brain areas associated with processing and evaluating incentive sa- increases in medial frontal gyrus activity during restrained choices lience, action execution, emotional arousal, interoception, intention- and decreases in middle temporal gyrus activity during impulsive ality, and planning (Rushworth et al., 2004; Moratti et al., 2008; choices (R2 =0.62; Table 5). Desmurget et al., 2009; Naqvi and Bechara, 2009). In comparing decision-making for monetary rewards with ciga- 4. Discussion rette rewards, several findings diverged from our predictions. Behav- iorally, participants were not more impulsive for cigarette rewards 4.1. Neuroanatomical regions associated with delay discounting of money compared to monetary rewards and the differences in brain activity and cigarettes were more substantial than expected. Significant differences were ev- ident between commodities in both a priori and functionally defined The current investigation was a preliminary study of the differen- brain areas. This was not a categorical effect, with one reward type tial brain activity during delay discounting decision-making for recruiting substantially more active regions than the other. Across money and cigarette rewards in smokers. The results were generally the a priori and functionally defined ROIs, close to equal numbers of consistent with our hypotheses and previous studies of discounting regions for money and cigarette rewards exhibited significantly in healthy adults, but also revealed some unexpected and novel find- greater activity. In terms of specific regions, decision making for ciga- ings. In terms of delay discounting choices, a number of brain regions rette rewards most notably recruited significantly greater activity in were implicated irrespective of commodity. Among the a priori ROIs, PPC, medial and middle frontal gyrus, and precentral gyrus, whereas greater activity in the medial PFC and right AIC was associated with choices for money recruited significantly greater activity in the VS,

Table 3 Significant differences by choice type in functionally defined regions of interest identified using a disjunction mask. Extraction Type I error rates were family-wise pb0.005+cluster size ≥5 adjacent voxels (voxelwise pb0.000016). Imp=impulsive choices, Restr=restrained choices; CTL=control choices; SEM=standard error of the mean; L=left, R=right. Coordinates are in Talairach space.

L/R Region Extent (mm3) xy zF(1,12) Imp Restr CTL p t (SEM) t (SEM) t (SEM)

L Medial frontal gyrus (MeFG) 405 −2 −24 68 8.32 0.76 (0.15)a 0.85 (0.15)a 0.32 (0.15) b0.01 L Precuneus (PCN) 270 −5 −58 36 2.53 0.84 (0.15) 0.68 (0.22) 0.39 (0.19) 0.10 R Middle temporal gyrus (MTG) 243 56 −48 −8 3.77 0.79 (0.11)a 0.40 (0.22)b 0.30 (0.14) 0.04 R Superior parietal lobule (SPL) 189 37 −65 48 1.20 0.81 (0.18) 0.59 (0.15) 0.45 (0.18) 0.32 R Cingulate gyrus (CG) 162 1 30 34 5.73 0.38 (0.26) 0.79 (0.10)a,b −0.02 (0.22) 0.01 R Superior frontal gyrus (SFG) 135 13 24 56 3.73 0.59 (0.22) 0.89 (0.14)a 0.42 (0.19) 0.04 R Middle frontal gyrus (MFG) 135 29 12 57 5.67 0.64 (0.21) 1.03 (0.17)a,b 0.35 (0.20) 0.01

a Significantly different from CTL, pb0.05. b Statistical trend compared to Imp, pb0.10.

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx 7

Table 4 Significant differences by commodity type in functionally defined regions of interest identified using a disjunction mask. Extraction Type I error rates were family-wise pb0.005 +cluster size ≥5 voxels (voxelwise pb0.000016). Money=money choices; Cig=cigarette choices; SEM=standard error of the mean; L=left, R=right. Coordinates are in Talairach space. Significant effects are in boldface.

L/R Region of interest mm3 xy zMoney Cig t(12) p t (SEM) t (SEM)

L Medial frontal gyrus 2916 −2 −155−1.15 (0.31) 1.11 (0.14) −6.10 b0.001 R Middle frontal gyrus 594 30 −657−0.83 (0.27) 1.05 (0.14) −5.61 b0.001 R Inferior frontal gyrus 486 51 15 2 0.72 (0.23) −0.81 (0.12) 5.34 b0.001 R Inferior frontal gyrus 459 43 4 27 −0.94 (0.26) 1.19 (0.16) −6.64 b0.001 L Middle occipital gyrus 405 −33 −90 −3 1.45 (0.22) 0.05 (0.29) 3.98 b0.002 R Superior parietal lobule 405 32 −59 47 −0.59 (0.29) 1.05 (0.16) −4.73 b0.001 R Inferior occipital gyrus 378 32 −88 −7 1.39 (0.18) −0.25 (0.20) 6.66 b0.001 L Middle frontal gyrus 378 −30 −753−0.98 (0.34) 1.11 (0.15) −4.68 b0.01 R Medial frontal gyrus 297 3 19 46 −0.38 (0.32) 1.05 (0.20) −3.52 b0.01 L Cuneus 243 −24 −78 22 −1.52 (0.28) 0.43 (0.22) −4.32 b0.01 R Middle temporal gyrus 216 63 −28 −2 0.97 (0.15) −0.01 (0.30) 2.49 0.03 L Angular gyrus 189 −44 −59 35 0.84 (0.11) −0.20 (0.18) 4.69 b0.01 R Cuneus 162 4 −90 25 1.27 (0.45) −1.25 (0.22) 4.45 b0.01 L Middle frontal gyrus 162 −36 15 30 −0.37 (0.37) 1.05 (0.18) −2.95 0.01 L Superior parietal lobule 162 −27 −55 41 −0.62 (0.26) 0.98 (0.13) −5.33 b0.001 L Precentral gyrus 162 −34 −278 59 −0.82 (0.37) 0.98 (0.19) −3.66 b0.01 L Inferior occipital gyrus 135 −25 −90 −9 1.52 (0.30) 0.24 (0.35) 2.48 0.03 R Inferior parietal lobule 135 62 −41 30 0.11 (0.29) −1.05 (0.22) 4.39 b0.001 L Precentral gyrus 135 −38 −16 51 −1.21 (0.44) 1.11 (0.22) −4.24 b0.001 temporoparietal cortex, and angular gyrus. More broadly, a potential- was present, as evidenced by the regions implicated in the choice ly meaningful pattern in terms of differential activation comprised type analyses, distinct neurocognitive networks also subserved deci- differences in lateralization, with the majority of regions with signif- sion making for the different rewards. These results both converge icantly greater activation for cigarette choices being in the left hemi- and contrast with an earlier study by McClure et al. (2007), which in- sphere and the majority of monetary reward regions being in the cluded a between-subjects comparison of delay discounting of fruit right hemisphere. This dissociation is most strikingly clear in the a juice and water (primary reinforcers) and money (a secondary rein- priori inferior frontal gyrus ROIs, where reciprocal differences in acti- forcer). Specifically, McClure et al. (2007) found areas of overlap be- vation and deactivation based on commodity are present. tween the commodities (e.g., supplementary motor area), but also Taken together, these findings suggest that commodity activation some distinct regions of activity that appeared to be commodity- patterns were not differences in degree, but in kind. That is, these specific (e.g., posterior cingulate) (McClure et al., 2007). In the cur- findings provide evidence that although domain-general activation rent study, the patterns of activity for overall activity attest to the

Fig. 5. Commodity type disjunction mask indicating regions of significant differences in activity compared to rest (pb0.005, minimum cluster size=5 voxels). The 10 largest regions of interest (ROIs) are illustrated with numbers in descending order of magnitude; the numbers correspond to the ROIs in Table 4, where neuroanatomical labels and specific coordinates are provided. All coordinates are presented in Talairach space and presented using radiological convention.

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 8 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx

Table 5 brain activity differences in commodity-based decision making may Exploratory continuous analyses of brain activity in relation to the behavioral economic benefit from using multiple commodities (e.g., money, food, water, index of impulsivity (i.e., hyperbolic temporal discounting function, k). Panel A pre- cigarettes) or cross-commodity decision-making (drug today versus sents significant correlations (Pearson r) between regions of interest and level of impulsivity based on decision making performance. Panel B presents stepwise regres- money in the future; e.g., Bickel et al., 2011). Studies using designs sion of a model incorporating the regions implicated in zero-order correlations, result- such as these would have the potential for substantially clarifying ing in the exclusion of three. Asterisks reflect statistical significance pb0.05. Final commodity-specific and non-specific brain activity. Likewise, differ- 2 2 model overall R =0.62, adjusted R =0.54. Region coordinates are provided in Tables 2 ences in lateralization were also not predicted, but, the fact that and 3. choices for cigarette rewards were largely subserved by the left hemi- A sphere, which is associated with more hedonic and affectively in- Region k 2345formed decision making (Schluter et al., 2001; Demaree et al., 2005; Hecht, 2010), may reflect less deliberate consideration of exact num- 1. Ventral striatuma,d 0.58* – 2. Temporoparietal cortexa,c −0.58* −0.28 – bers of cigarettes and more consideration of the subjective appeal of 3. Medial frontal gyrusb,d 0.63* 0.80* 0.18 – smoking. This is necessarily conjecture, but represents an empirical 4. Middle temporal gyrusb,d 0.59* 0.56* −0.10 0.58* – question for future research. Finally, the absence of a behavioral com-

B modity effect was unexpected, however, previous studies have trans- lated dollars into packs of cigarettes (e.g., Baker et al., 2003), not Region B (SEM) β individual cigarettes, which may explain the difference. Delay dis- Medial frontal gyrusb,d 0.06* (0.02) 0.54* counting is typically subject to robust magnitude effects — greater im- a,c − − Temporoparietal cortex 0.05* (0.02) 0.48* pulsivity for smaller rewards and vice versa (e.g., MacKillop et al., a A priori region of interest. 2010). As such, the two different conversions result in much larger nu- b Functionally defined region of interest. meric values or much lower numeric values in the task. In the case of a c Activity during impulsive choices. d conversion to individual cigarettes, the magnitude effect of a large nu- Activity during restrained choices. meric value may have canceled out the commodity effect behaviorally, although the fMRI nonetheless revealed a distinct profile of activation. presence of commonalities in activity – a core discounting choice net- This commodity and magnitude effect interaction is speculative, but it work – but the large magnitude commodity differences suggest more is a plausible explanation for this divergence from previous studies. meaningful differences by commodity than suggested in the earlier In terms of limitations, the sample size was self-evidently suffi- study, at least with regard to an addictive drug reward and a monetary cient to detect an array of significant differences, but a larger sample reward. In addition to commodity differences, other methodological would be expected to bring these effects into sharper relief for the re- differences further reduce the comparability of the two studies and gions implicated and identify several additional ones. For example, in are the most probable explanation for differences in brain activation ob- the general discounting behavior analyses, the VS, left AIC, and precu- served. Differences in hardware, software, and paradigms commonly neus exhibited differences in magnitude that would be expected to be pose a challenge to comparability across fMRI studies, but clarifying dif- statistically significant in an only modestly larger sample. Moreover, ferences between domain-general and commodity-specific brain activ- the presence of notable differences in the commodity-based differ- ity should nonetheless be a priority in future studies. ence requires caution be applied to the general decision-making find- An exploratory objective was to simultaneously consider the var- ings. Some of these differences may have effectively canceled each ious regions implicated and determine unique associations between other out, meaning that the observed common regions effectively brain activity and impulsive behavior. In terms of neural activity, met a higher threshold of being present in spite of commodity differ- although a number of regions were individually correlated with be- ences. The relatively small number of items per category meant that havioral performance, two regions in particular were substantially further fractionation of the stimuli was not viable and future studies and uniquely associated with level of impulsivity, the medial frontal will be necessary to more comprehensively characterize regions gyrus and temporoparietal cortex, and the relative balance between responsible for domain-general and commodity-specific aspects of the two predicted over half the variation in behavioral performance. discounting. Given the high overall levels of discounting observed, Specifically, medial frontal gyrus activity was positively associated this also suggests that the items used in this study, which came with discounting, whereas temporoparietal cortex activity was nega- from the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (Kirby et al., 1999), may tively associated with discounting. This opposing directionality is not be optimally sensitive for characterizing variation in discounting broadly consistent with the competing systems hypothesis, in which in neuroimaging research. Rather, stimuli from a more traditional motivational drive and regulatory inhibition jointly determine the delay discounting task that include a broader array of amounts and behavioral outcome (McClure et al., 2004; Bickel et al., 2007). delays might be necessary to provide a high-resolution characteriza- tion (e.g., Bickel et al., 2009). Another methodological point that is 4.2. Considerations and limitations relevant is that the order of stimuli was counterbalanced in general (i.e., money–cigarettes–money-cigarettes), but not by participant, so As a preliminary study in this area, it is important to note that it is possible that order had an effect on the findings. Finally, it should there are also a number of interpretive considerations and limita- be noted that the protocol did not comprehensively assess concurrent tions. For example, decision making for monetary rewards was asso- drug use and psychiatric symptoms among the participants, which ciated with significantly higher activity in the VS compared to could have influences on brain activity or behavior and could not be cigarette rewards, which was surprising, as the VS is believed to evaluated. play an important role in processing incentive salience (Voorn et al., 2004) and would be expected to be more sensitive to a consumable 4.3. Applications drug reward. However, it is also possible that monetary rewards may in fact have been more salient to the participants because they Recognizing the preceding considerations, the current study none- were of relatively low income and, having just smoked a cigarette, theless provides proof-of-concept that a neuroeconomic approach were satiated in terms of nicotine. This hypothesis is testable in future has promise for studying nicotine dependence. As such, there are sev- studies manipulating commodity deprivation. Another consideration eral clear directions for the future. Although this study provides initial is that money and cigarettes may have some identification overlap evidence of the neurocognitive substrates underlying discounting and (e.g., money is directly exchanged for cigarettes) and clarifying notable commodity differences, more comprehensive studies will be

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx 9 essential for fully characterizing discounting in smokers, both for addicts: the commodity and its temporal location determine discounting rate. Psy- chopharmacology (Berl) 217 (2), 177–187. monetary and cigarette rewards, and in comparison to non-smokers Boettiger, C.A., Mitchell, J.M., Tavares, V.C., Robertson, M., Joslyn, G., D'Esposito, M., or occasional smokers. However, an important factor is that smokers Fields, H.L., 2007. Immediate reward bias in humans: fronto-parietal networks and non-smokers differ significantly in terms of both vascular and and a role for the catechol-O-methyltransferase 158Val/Val genotype. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 14383–14391. brain health (e.g., Paul et al., 2008), which could affect BOLD response Boonn, A., 2007. State Cigarette Prices, Taxes, and Costs Per Pack. Campaign for and should be controlled for to the extent possible. Given the sample Tobacco-Free Kids, Washington, DC. size, the current study could not examine differences between males Carter, R.M., Meyer, J.R., Huettel, S.A., 2010. Functional neuroimaging of intertemporal and females, but, as sex differences have been implicated in the rela- choice models: a review. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology and Economics 27–45. tionship between discounting and smoking (Jones et al., 2009), that is Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2003. Cigarette smoking-attributable mor- another direction for future work. Similarly, the role of withdrawal bidity — United States, 2000. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Review 52, 842–844. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2008. Smoking-attributable mortality, (and by association, craving), which has previously been shown to — – fi years of potential life lost, and productivity losses United States, 2000 2004. signi cantly increase discounting (e.g., Field et al., 2006), is another Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Review 57, 1226–1228. promising target for future studies. Finally, impulsive discounting is Coffey, S.F., Gudleski, G.D., Saladin, M.E., Brady, K.T., 2003. Impulsivity and rapid a risk factor for smoking cessation treatment failure (Krishnan-Sarin discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 11, 18–25. et al., 2007; Yoon et al., 2007; MacKillop and Kahler, 2009; Sheffer Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic et al., 2012) and the current study suggests a neuroeconomic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research 29, 162–173. approach may clarify the brain regions responsible for treatment Demaree, H.A., Everhart, D.E., Youngstrom, E.A., Harrison, D.W., 2005. Brain lateraliza- tion of emotional processing: historical roots and a future incorporating “domi- outcome. nance”. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 4, 3–20. Desmurget, M., Reilly, K.T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese, C., Sirigu, A., 2009. Move- 4.4. Conclusions ment intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 324, 811–813. Epstein, L.H., Richards, J.B., Saad, F.G., Paluch, R.A., Roemmich, J.N., Lerman, C., 2003. Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. Experimental The current study applied a neuroeconomic approach to under- and Clinical Psychopharmacology 11, 131–138. stand impulsive delay discounting in individuals with nicotine depen- Field, M., Santarcangelo, M., Sumnall, H., Goudie, A., Cole, J., 2006. Delay discounting dence. The brain regions associated with discounting converged with and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: the effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology 186, 255–263. a number of previous studies and, in some cases, were closely associ- Heatherton, T.F., Kozlowski, L.T., Frecker, R.C., Fagerstrom, K.O., 1991. The Fagerstrom ated with behavioral performance, but the study also revealed a qual- Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Question- – itatively different pattern of brain activity for discounting of cigarette naire. British Journal of Addiction 86, 1119 1127. Hecht, D., 2010. Depression and the hyperactive right-hemisphere. Neuroscience rewards. Although a number of considerations and limitations apply, Research 68, 77–87. these findings suggest the high promise of applying a neuroeconomic Hoffman, W.F., Schwartz, D.L., Huckans, M.S., McFarland, B.H., Meiri, G., Stevens, A.A., approach to understand the etiology, maintenance, and treatment of Mitchell, S.H., 2008. Cortical activation during delay discounting in abstinent meth- amphetamine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology 201 (2), 183–193 Dec. nicotine dependence. Jones, B.A., Landes, R.D., Yi, R., Bickel, W.K., 2009. Temporal horizon: modulation by smoking status and gender. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 104 (Suppl. 1), S87–S93. Acknowledgments Kable, J.W., Glimcher, P.W., 2007. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience 10, 1625–1633. Kirby, K.N., Petry, N.M., Bickel, W.K., 1999. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates This research was partially supported by grants and resources from for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental the Russell Sage Foundation (JM, LS, SD), the National Institutes of Psychology. General 128, 78–87. Krishnan-Sarin, S., Reynolds, B., Duhig, A.M., Smith, A., Liss, T., McFetridge, A., Cavallo, Health (K23 AA016936 [JM], P30 DA027827 [JM], R21 DA027331 D.A., Carroll, K.M., Potenza, M.N., 2007. Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment [SPD], R24 GM61374 [SPD]), the National Science Foundation outcome in a smoking cessation program for adolescent smokers. Drug and Alco- (0521432), Butler Hospital, the John and Mary Franklin Foundation hol Dependence 88, 79–82. [MTA], the Brown University Magnetic Resonance Imaging Facility, Lawrence, N.S., Ross, T.J., Stein, E.A., 2002. Cognitive mechanisms of nicotine on visual attention. Neuron 36, 539–548. and the Brown University Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies. Lazar, N.A., 2008. The Statistical Analysis of Functional MRI Data. Springer. MacKillop, J., Kahler, C.W., 2009. Delayed reward discounting predicts treatment Appendix A. Supplementary data response for heavy drinkers receiving smoking cessation treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 104, 197–203. MacKillop, J., Miranda Jr., R., Monti, P.M., Ray, L.A., Murphy, J.G., Rohsenow, D.J., Supplementary data to this article can be found online at doi:10. McGeary, J.E., Swift, R.M., Tidey, J.W., Gwaltney, C.J., 2010. Alcohol demand, 1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003. delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 119, 106–114. MacKillop, J., Amlung, M.T., Few, L.R., Ray, L.A., Sweet, L.H., Munafò, M.R., 2011. Delayed References reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology 216, 305–321. Audrain-McGovern, J., Rodriguez, D., Epstein, L.H., Cuevas, J., Rodgers, K., Wileyto, E.P., Madden, G.J., Petry, N.M., Badger, G.J., Bickel, W.K., 1997. Impulsive and self-control choices 2009. Does delay discounting play an etiological role in smoking or is it a conse- in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and quence of smoking? Drug and Alcohol Dependence 103, 99–106. monetary rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 5, 256–262. Baker, F., Johnson, M.W., Bickel, W.K., 2003. Delay discounting in current and never- Mazur, J.E., 1987. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, Commons, M.L., Mazur, J.E., Nevin, J.A., Rachlin, H. (Eds.), The Effect of Delay and and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112, 382–392. of Intervening Event on Reinforcement Value: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior. Ballard, K., Knutson, B., 2009. Dissociable neural representations of future reward mag- Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 55–73. nitude and delay during temporal discounting. NeuroImage 45, 143–150. McClure, S.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., Cohen, J.D., 2004. Separate neural systems Bechara, A., 2005. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507. drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience 8 (11), 1458–1463 Nov. McClure, S.M., Ericson, K.M., Laibson, D.I., Loewenstein, G., Cohen, J.D., 2007. Time Bickel, W.K., Vuchinich, R.E., 2003. Reframing Health Behavior Change with Behavioral discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 5796–5804. Economics. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah NJ. Mitchell, S.H., 1999. Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Bickel, W.K., Odum, A.L., Madden, G.J., 1999. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay dis- Psychopharmacology 146, 455–464. counting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology 146, 447–454. Mokdad, A.H., Marks, J.S., Stroup, D.F., Gerberding, J.L., 2004. Actual causes of death in Bickel, W.K., Miller, M.L., Yi, R., Kowal, B.P., Lindquist, D.M., Pitcock, J.A., 2007. Behavior- the United States, 2000. JAMA 291, 1238–1245. al and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal Monterosso, J.R., Luo, S., 2010. An argument against dual valuation system com- discounting processes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 90 (Suppl. 1), S85–S91. petition: cognitive capacities supporting future orientation mediate rather than Bickel, W.K., Pitcock, J.A., Yi, R., Angtuaco, E.J., 2009. Congruence of BOLD response compete with visceral motivations. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology and Eco- across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses. nomics 3, 1–14. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 8839–8846. Monterosso, J.R., Ainslie, G., Xu, J., Cordova, X., Domier, C.P., London, E.D., 2007. Fronto- Bickel, W.K., Landes, R.D., Christensen, D.R., Jackson, L., Jones, B.A., Kurth-Nelson, Z., parietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects Redish, A.D., 2011. Single- and cross-commodity discounting among cocaine performing a delay discounting task. Human Brain Mapping 28 (5), 383–393 May.

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003 10 J. MacKillop et al. / Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging xxx (2012) xxx–xxx

Moratti, S., Rubio, G., Campo, P., Keil, A., Ortiz, T., 2008. Hypofunction of right tempor- Sheffer, C., MacKillop, J., McGeary, J., Landes, R., Carter, L., Yi, R., Jones, B., Christensen, oparietal cortex during emotional arousal in depression. Archives of General C., Stitzer, M., Jackson, L., Bickel, W.K., 2012. Delay discounting, locus of control, Psychiatry 65, 532–541. and cognitive impulsivity independently predict tobacco dependence treatment Naqvi, N.H., Bechara, A., 2009. The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends in outcomes in a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic group of smokers. American Neurosciences 32, 56–67. Journal on Addictions 21 (3), 221–232. Paul, R.H., Grieve, S.M., Niaura, R., David, S.P., Laidlaw, D.H., Cohen, R., Sweet, L., Taylor, Sweet, L.H., Mulligan, R.C., Finnerty, C.E., Jerskey, B.A., David, S.P., Cohen, R.A., Niaura, G., Clark, R.C., Pogun, S., Gordon, E., 2008. Chronic cigarette smoking and the micro- R.S., 2010. Effects of nicotine withdrawal on verbal working memory and associat- structural integrity of white matter in healthy adults: a diffusion tensor imaging ed brain response. Psychiatry Research 183, 69–74. study. Nicotine & Tobacco Research 10, 137–147. Sweitzer, M.M., Donny, E.C., Dierker, L.C., Flory, J.D., Manuck, S.B., 2008. Delay discount- Petry, N.M., 2001. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, ing and smoking: association with the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology 154, 243–250. but not cigarettes smoked per day. Nicotine & Tobacco Research 10, 1571–1575. Ray, R., Schnoll, R.A., Lerman, C., 2009. Nicotine dependence: biology, behavior, and Voorn, P., Vanderschuren, L.J., Groenewegen, H.J., Robbins, T.W., Pennartz, C.M., 2004. treatment. Annual Review of Medicine 60, 247–260. Putting a spin on the dorsal–ventral divide of the striatum. Trends in Neurosci- Reynolds, B., 2006a. A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to ences 27, 468–474. drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology 17, 651–667. Vuchinich, R.E., Heather, N., 2003. Choice, Behavioural Economics and Addiction. Reynolds, B., 2006b. The Experiential Discounting Task is sensitive to cigarette- Pergamon/Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, The , p. 438. smoking status and correlates with a measure of delay discounting. Behavioural Ward, B.D., 2000. Simultaneous Inference for FMRI Data. Biophysics Research Insitute, Pharmacology 17, 133–142. Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee Wisconsin. Reynolds, B., Richards, J.B., Horn, K., Karraker, K., 2004. Delay discounting and probabil- Ward, B.D., 2006. Deconvolution Analysis of FMRI Time Series Data. Biophysics ity discounting as related to cigarette smoking status in adults. Behavioural Pro- Research Institute, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin. cesses 65, 35–42. Wipfli, H., Samet, J.M., 2009a. Global economic and health benefits of tobacco control: Rushworth, M.F., Walton, M.E., Kennerley, S.W., Bannerman, D.M., 2004. Action sets part 1. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 86, 263–271. and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 410–417. Wipfli, H., Samet, J.M., 2009b. Global economic and health benefits of tobacco control: Saad, Z.S., Glen, D.R., Chen, G., Beauchamp, M.S., Desai, R., Cox, R.W., 2009. A new meth- part 2. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 86, 272–280. od for improving functional-to-structural MRI alignment using local Pearson corre- World Health Organization, 2008. World Health Organization Statistics Report — 2008. lation. NeuroImage 44, 839–848. World Health Organization Press, Geneva, Switzerland. Schluter, N.D., Krams, M., Rushworth, M.F., Passingham, R.E., 2001. Cerebral dominance Yoon, J.H., Higgins, S.T., Heil, S.H., Sugarbaker, R.J., Thomas, C.S., Badger, G.J., 2007. for action in the human brain: the selection of actions. Neuropsychologia 39, Delay discounting predicts postpartum relapse to cigarette smoking among 105–113. pregnant women. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 15, 176–186.

Please cite this article as: MacKillop, J., et al., The neuroeconomics of nicotine dependence: A preliminary study of delay discounting of mon- etaryand cigarette rewards..., Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging (2012), doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.003