University of Otago

Uncertainty, Precaution & Perception: Communicating the Risk of Heading in Football

Alexander William Gilbert

A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Communication

Centre for Science Communication, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand

July 2020

A. Gilbert

Abstract

Concerns regarding the short and long-term effects of sport-related head injury have grown in recent decades. The damage incurred by mild traumatic brain injury in the context of collision sports has thus received significant attention in both the media and in scientific research. Although this field of enquiry largely concerns the neurological impact of acute head injury (including concussion), a recent proliferation of research considering more subtle mechanisms of brain damage has also taken place. General observations from the study of “subconcussion” suggest a potential link between repeated head impacts of a subclinical nature and increased risk of neurodegeneration.

The act of intentionally playing the ball with one’s head, or “heading”, is an integral part of football gameplay, and has since been identified as a potential vehicle of subconcussive injury. Despite the lack of conclusive evidence for a link between the act of heading and increased risk of brain injury, both the United States and the United Kingdom have recently taken precautionary measures by introducing regulations on junior heading exposure. To date, no empirical study has investigated public perceptions of heading as a possible health risk.

The present study explored attitudes of the New Zealand public towards heading in an effort to gauge how the factors of scientific uncertainty and precautionary information influence heading- related risk perceptions. A sample of domestic players and parents of junior players (n= 89) was recruited to take part in an online survey. Participants were presented with information regarding subconcussion research and the notion of a link between heading and neurodegenerative disease.

Individuals presented with a disclosure of scientific uncertainty tended to report a higher level of risk attributable to heading compared to controls. Conversely, information regarding precautionary measures generally resulted in slightly lower risk perceptions than controls. Analysis revealed a medium-large interaction effect between the factors of uncertainty and precautionary information (p<.05), illustrating significantly higher risk perceptions when uncertainty was paired with a “strong” example of precaution (banning junior heading) compared to when uncertainty was associated with an example of weaker precaution (limiting junior heading exposure).

The findings show that information on precautionary heading guidelines may act to attenuate concerns regarding heading as a possible health risk. Moreover, acknowledgment of the uncertainty inherent to current heading research may combine with information on strong precautionary measures to prompt a more intuitive and perhaps pessimistic assessment of risk. These findings have significant implications for the communication of current and future heading guidelines to the public, as transparent risk communication demands honest appraisals of the scientific uncertainty and value-laden precautionary judgments that underpin regulatory decisions. Considering the potential health risk posed by heading, alongside the general health benefits of physical activity; the communication of heading guidelines must take care in promoting the safety of at-risk populations while also avoiding the risk of escalating public concerns and reducing participation in football.

i

A. Gilbert

Acknowledgments

Completing this thesis has been an incredible experience. I’ve been lucky enough to meet so many great people over the last two years, and the extent to which each one has aided me is difficult to put into words. But here goes nothing. Firstly, my thanks go to Associate Professor Jesse Bering, whose expertise and guidance not only made this project achievable, but also immensely enjoyable. The lessons I’ve learnt working with Jesse have been invaluable to my completion of this thesis, but they will become no less important throughout the rest of my life. Your knowledge, patience and encouragement as a supervisor are unequalled. Thank you for helping me find my feet. I truly believe there is no department quite like the Centre for Science Communication. The friendships and memories I have gained during my time here have had a huge impact on my life, and I’m certainly a better person for it. The entirety of the Centre’s staff has aided in this journey. In particular, I would like to thank Professor Nancy Longnecker and Dr Fabien Medvecky for always showing an interest in my project and offering much-needed advice. I’m incredibly grateful for the assistance of Sue Harvey, who was always willing to go the extra mile in service of my project. I also need to thank Anna Samuel, who has so adeptly taken up the unenviable role of replacing Sue at the front desk. Your help has not gone unnoticed. My gratitude towards Dr Thomas Swan cannot be overstated. Tom’s guidance in terms of study design and statistical analysis has been nothing short of crucial. When I had no idea what I was doing, Tom pointed me in the right direction. I’m equally grateful for having met Steve Ting. There is not enough room here to list the ways in which Steve has helped me. In short, I would like to thank him for making each day I spent at the Owheo building an absolute joy. I owe special thanks to Wynton Rufer, Dr Michael Lipton and Dawn Astle, each of whom graciously offered their time and stories for the purpose of this thesis. I also wish to thank Manon Knapen and Victoria Alogna. Both were willing to take me under their wing and share their knowledge when they had no obligation to do so. The challenge of completing a thesis has a unique way of highlighting the value of good friends, hence why I want to acknowledge Matthew, Sarah, Harry and Mackenzie. Your camaraderie has been indispensable. Lastly, I want to thank my family. This year has been difficult for all of us, and I have never been more grateful to have your support. I’ll never be able to say it enough, but thank you Mum and Dad for backing me every step of the way, just as I know you always will. Robin, Campbell and Lachlan, I’m proud to call you my brothers. And to my partner and best friend Renée, thank you for putting up with me through it all. Your willingness to do so says more than I ever could.

ii

A. Gilbert

List of Tables

Table 5-1: Eight message conditions presented to participants ...... 55 Table 5-2: Separate elements of message conditions ...... 56 Table 5-3: Perceived Susceptibility to Sport Injury (PSSI) scale ...... 63 Table 5-4: Adaptation of PSSI scale items ...... 64 Table 5-5: Adaptation of Sport Injury Anxiety Scale (SIAS) – Anxiety related to BPW subscale ...... 66 Table 6-1: Distribution of participants across 8 experimental message groups ...... 69 Table 6-2: Mean risk perception scores (RPS) for each experimental message condition ...... 70

List of Figures

Figure 6-1: Age and sex of participants...... 69 Figure 6-2: Interaction of Uncertainty and Precaution...... 71 Figure 6-3: Importance of football to personal identity ...... 73 Figure 6-4: Frequency of heading in training ...... 74 Figure 6-5: Frequency of heading during games ...... 74 Figure 6-6: Parents’ estimation of their child’s attitude towards heading ...... 75 Figure 6-7: Parents’ preference for junior heading regulation ...... 75 Figure 7-1: Effect of precautionary information on EMF risk perception ...... 79

iii

A. Gilbert

Table of contents

1 INTRODUCTION ...... 1

1.1 Heading in football: emerging concerns ...... 1

1.2 Heading regulation ...... 3 1.2.1 Critical reception ...... 4

1.3 Thesis structure ...... 7

2 SUBCONCUSSION LITERATURE ...... 9

2.1 Neurobiological findings...... 9 2.1.1 Neuroimaging ...... 9 2.1.2 Biochemical markers ...... 10

2.2 Neuropsychological findings...... 14 2.2.1 Confounding concussion & subconcussion ...... 16 2.2.2 Limited evidence of dysfunction ...... 18 2.2.3 Alternative studies of function ...... 20 2.2.4 Head injury symptoms ...... 21 2.2.5 Critical assessment ...... 22

2.3 Biomechanical findings...... 25 2.3.1 Instrumentation ...... 26 2.3.2 Recording threshold ...... 28 2.3.3 Concussion risk factors ...... 29 2.3.4 Anthropometrics & neck strength ...... 29 2.3.5 Heading technique ...... 31 2.3.6 Protective utility of headgear...... 32 2.3.7 Ball composition ...... 33 2.3.8 Impact location ...... 34 2.3.9 Heading scenario ...... 35 2.3.10 Heading frequency ...... 36

3 UNCERTAINTY ...... 38

3.1 Communicating uncertainty ...... 39

4 PRECAUTION ...... 43

4.1 The precautionary principle ...... 43 4.1.1 Criticism of the precautionary principle ...... 45 4.1.2 In favour of the precautionary principle ...... 48

4.2 Communicating precaution ...... 51

5 METHODOLOGY ...... 54

iv

A. Gilbert

5.1 Research questions ...... 54

5.2 Hypotheses ...... 54

5.3 Study design ...... 55

5.4 Independent variables ...... 57 5.4.1 Uncertainty ...... 57 5.4.2 Precautionary measures ...... 58

5.5 Participants ...... 59

5.6 Questionnaire design ...... 59 5.6.1 Demographic questions ...... 60 5.6.2 a priori risk ...... 61 5.6.3 Perceived risk of heading ...... 63 5.6.4 Perceptions of others ...... 66 5.6.5 Support for heading regulation ...... 67 5.6.6 Attention check and debrief ...... 67

6 RESULTS ...... 69

6.1 Quantitative analysis...... 69

6.2 Risk perception analysis ...... 69

6.3 Risk perception scores ...... 70 6.3.1 Simple main effects of Uncertainty ...... 72 6.3.2 Simple main effects of Precaution ...... 72

6.4 Participant characteristics ...... 73

7 DISCUSSION ...... 77

7.1 The uncertainty effect ...... 78

7.2 The precautionary effect ...... 79

7.3 Interaction of Uncertainty and Precaution ...... 80

7.4 Conclusions ...... 83 7.4.1 Implications for communicating heading regulations ...... 84

7.5 Limitations & future directions...... 85

REFERENCES ...... 87

APPENDIX ...... 104

CREATIVE COMPONENT ...... 118

v

A. Gilbert

Introduction 1.1 Heading in football: emerging concerns

In recent decades, the topic of sports-related head injury has become far more prominent in the public discourse and within the scientific community. However, the connection between neurodegenerative conditions and certain sporting activities has been apparent for nearly a century. The clinical symptoms of “punch drunk”, today recognised as chronic traumatic encephalopathy (CTE), were first described by Martland in 1928. The signs of neurodegeneration seen in boxers were thought to be the consequence of “single or repeated blows to the head or jaw” (p. 1103) and were described as being “Parkinsonian” in nature (Martland, 1928). CTE is a neurodegenerative disease that presents numerous symptoms, including memory impairment, depression and the ultimate onset of dementia (Baugh et al., 2012), although these symptoms may not appear until well after the infliction of trauma (McKee et al., 2009). While evidence implicates a link between CTE and repeated concussive head trauma (McKee et al., 2009), the current understanding of how repeated subconcussive impacts may contribute to CTE—or to neurodegeneration in general—is less conclusive (Mainwaring, Ferdinand-Pennock, Mylabathula & Alavie, 2018).

Bailes, Petraglia, Omalu, Nauman and Talavage (2013) define a subconcussive blow as, “a cranial impact that does not result in known or diagnosed concussion on clinical grounds” (p. 1236), noting that the repetitive, cumulative nature of these impacts is thought to harbour the deleterious effects of subconcussion. Head impacts such as these, as well as rapid acceleration- deceleration of the body and torso (Bailes et al., 2013), result in the collision of the semisolid brain and cerebral fluids with the skull, initiating what is known as the “slosh effect” (Smith et al., 2012).

In their review of the literature on subconcussion in sport, Mainwaring et al. (2018) analyse the use of 41 unique terms in describing the phenomenon of subconcussion across 56 papers, highlighting the potential inadequacy of the term itself: “defining subconcussion by what it is not does not tell us what it is” (p. 52). Mainwaring et al. (2018) argue that terms such as “subconcussion” and “subconcussive injuries” therefore cannot be operationally defined. In lieu of such amorphous terminology, Belanger, Vanderploeg, and McAllister (2016) and Mainwaring et al. (2018) suggest using alternative terms such as “head impact”, as there is a suggestion that “subconcussive” changes may in fact be indicative of undiagnosed concussive trauma (Forbes, Glutting & Kaminski, 2016). Although the notion of subconcussion might currently be considered an “elusive theoretical construct” (Belanger et al., 2016), the remainder of this thesis will continue

1

A. Gilbert referring to this phenomenon as “subconcussion”, as this represents the predominant terminology within the literature being discussed.

Most of the empirical (and anecdotal) focus of CTE research and sports-related head injury has traditionally centred on those sports for which head injuries are more likely to occur given the nature of the activities involved. For example, boxing has long been associated with progressive neurodegeneration due to the sport’s prevalence of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) (Gavett, Stern & McKee, 2011). Concussion incidence in American football has also drawn considerable attention, with the detrimental neuropsychiatric effects of repetitive mTBI (Didehbani, Cullum, Mansinghani, Conover & Hart, 2013; Guskiewicz et al., 2005) and evidence of increased CTE incidence (Mez et al., 2017) among former players generating widespread concern.

Although the link between repeated concussions and degenerative diseases such as CTE is not entirely clear (Gavett et al., 2011), public and academic interest in sports-related CTE and subconcussion has undoubtedly been fuelled by high-profile deaths of former professional athletes—and football is no exception. The untimely death of former professional footballer Jeff Astle, 59, for instance, garnered significant media attention, with the coroner ultimately attributing his accrued neurodegeneration to a career of heading a football (Eaton, 2002). Similar cases of footballers exhibiting neurodegenerative pathology have since arisen, including Ling et al.’s (2017) study of 14 retired players presenting with dementia. Four of the 6 individuals made available for post-mortem examination were diagnosed with CTE, with concomitant pathologies arising in all 6 (Ling et al., 2017). Since these men spent an average of 26 years playing the game, with the onset of cognitive impairment around age 63; the authors surmised that a probable link exists between prolonged exposure to repetitive head impacts and their observed pathologies (Ling et al., 2017).

The act of “heading” in football is a core component of a player’s repertoire, the goal of which is typically to initiate contact with the ball at or around the hairline in order to intentionally direct the flight of the ball (Kirkendall, Jordan & Garrett, 2001). Cases of neuropathology in former players have since highlighted the act of heading in football as perhaps an entirely unique mechanism of subconcussive injury. As noted earlier, the literature on sports-related head injury is largely concerned with high-impact sports such as American football and boxing, and typically focuses on their potential to inflict concussive impacts (e.g., Guskiewicz et al., 2005; Neselius et al., 2014; Papa, Ramia, Edwards, Johnson & Slobounov, 2015). However, a growing subset of research has begun investigating the nature of repeated, subconcussive impacts in sport (e.g., Davenport et al., 2016; Lipton et al., 2013; Stewart et al., 2018), and the act of intentionally

2

A. Gilbert heading a football—an event not replicated in any other sporting endeavour—presents a unique opportunity to investigate the acute and chronic effects of subconcussive exposure on neurological health (Maher, Hutchison, Cusimano, Comper & Schweizer, 2014).

1.2 Heading regulation

In November of 2015, the first case of widespread heading regulation was introduced when the U.S. Soccer Federation announced their “U.S. Soccer Concussion Initiative”. This set of guidelines was adopted as a means of addressing head injuries in the national game following a 2014 class- action lawsuit in which a group of parents cited nearly 50,000 concussions among high school players during the 2010 season (Yang & Baugh, 2016). Enforced by US Club Soccer, this initiative includes a set of guidelines recommending junior players abstain from heading the ball, stating that players aged 10 and under “shall not engage in heading, either in practises or in games.” (US Club Soccer, 2016). Furthermore, players aged between 11 and 13 are limited to a maximum of 30 minutes of heading per week during practise, given that this does not exceed 15 to 20 headers per week. There is no restriction on heading during games for players in these age groups.

Following the ground-breaking US announcement, the English Football Association (FA) clarified their own stance on junior heading regulation, citing insufficient evidence to follow the example of U.S. Soccer (Moore, 2015). However, evidence emerging from FA and PFA (Professional Footballers’ Association) funded research highlighting neurodegenerative disease in former professional players would ultimately prompt the United Kingdom’s own adoption of junior heading guidelines in February of 2020 (The FA, 2020). The FIELD (Football’s InfluencE on Lifelong health and Dementia risk) study is a retrospective cohort study of former professional Scottish players (Russell et al., 2019). Five of 7 former players presenting with dementia exhibited neuropathology consistent with CTE following post-mortem assessment, with similar findings among 4 former professional rugby players (Lee et al., 2019). Further, among the 1180 former football players and 3807 matched controls of the FIELD cohort to have died, neurodegenerative disease was listed as the primary cause of death for 1.7% of former players versus 0.5% of controls (Mackay et al., 2019).

Despite acknowledging that “there was no evidence in the FIELD study to suggest that heading the ball was the cause to the link with incidence of degenerative neurocognitive disease”, the FA introduced junior heading guidelines to “mitigate against any potential risks” heading poses to junior players (The FA, 2020). Unlike the US guidelines, the FA’s announcement does not pertain to junior heading during games, and instead recommends that heading should not be introduced

3

A. Gilbert to training until the age of 11. These guidelines also stipulate a graduated approach to heading in training, wherein the following frequencies are advised:

• Under-12 age group: maximum of 1 heading session per month (no more than 5 headers per session). • Under-13 age group: maximum of 1 heading session per week (no more than 5 headers per session). • Under-14 to under-16 age groups: maximum of 1 heading session per week (no more than 10 headers per session). • Under-18 age group: “Heading drills should be reduced as far as possible”.

1.2.1 Critical reception

To date, these announcements represent the only two instances of widespread heading regulation in football’s history, although there are indications that the Union of European Football Associations (UEFA) intends to introduce Europe-wide guidelines within 2020 (The FA, 2020), and individual clubs appear to be following suit (Hodson, 2020). As might be expected, public reception of these guidelines has been mixed (Schuppe, 2015; Topping, 2020).

Heading guidelines have received praise for restricting both the potential number of concussions and subconcussive hits incurred by junior players, a sentiment buoyed on by concerns regarding the adverse effects of brain trauma at a highly developmental age (Babikian, Merkley, Savage, Giza & Levin, 2015). Others argue that any form of heading regulation may be premature (Kontos et al., 2017), noting that heading is relatively rare in the junior game. Studies observing the number of headers performed by junior players report averages ranging from ~1 (Forbes et al., 2016; Janda, Bir & Cheney, 2002; Kaminski, Cousino & Glutting, 2013) to ~2 (McCuen et al., 2015) headers per player per game. Professional cohorts, on the other hand, have estimated players carry out between 50 and 2100 headers in a single season, with a median of 800 (Matser, Kessels, Jordan, Lezak & Troost, 1998). Notably, the number of headers incurred during training does not factor into Matser et al.’s (1998) figures. Some argue that the disparity in heading exposure between junior and professional players—apparent in both the number of headers per game and the number of games and training sessions carried out per season (Forbes et al., 2016)—should not be overlooked. Interestingly, the rarity of junior heading during gameplay is what influenced the FA to omit games from the UK’s heading guidelines and focus solely on reducing exposure

4

A. Gilbert during training. In this sense, the US and UK heading guidelines are incompatible with one another.

Some critics have noted the somewhat paradoxical nature of opting to regulate junior heading based on findings from professional players. The FIELD study has been influential in the introduction of the UK’s junior heading regulation; however, their findings indicate professional football players as a potentially high-risk population. Some argue the decision to reduce heading exposure for junior players is therefore not the logical conclusion to take from the current findings of the FIELD study (Malcolm, 2019). The researchers themselves note, “The relevance of our findings in elite athletes to recreational soccer participation is not known. These observations cannot be applied directly to recreational and amateur soccer players” (Mackay et al., 2019, p. 1807). Conversely, there appears to be no incentive to regulate heading exposure among the population identified as being at an inordinate risk of neurodegenerative disease (Malcolm, 2019).

In the context of safety in junior sport, the notion of heading regulation is by no means novel. In March of 2016, an open letter signed by 73 academics and public health professionals called for the removal of tackling from junior rugby (SportCIC, 2016). Citing the prevalence of “harmful contact”, the authors argue that rugby puts players under the age of 18 at a high risk of injury— including concussion—compared to those in other sports. This desire to increase protection for junior players persists across multiple high-contact sports. For example, most parents would support age restrictions on tackling in junior American football if this were an option (Chrisman et al., 2019).

Critics often warn of the risk that overregulation in junior sport could lead to increased sedentary behaviour (MacDonald & Myer, 2017; Molcho & Pickett, 2011), noting the universal health benefits conferred by physical activity (Blair & Morris, 2009; Hiles, Lamers, Milaneschi & Penninx, 2017; Lautenschlager et al., 2008). Indeed, results of the FIELD study show former players had lower all-cause mortality than controls, and were less likely to die from lung cancer or ischemic heart disease, leading the authors to suggest that a higher level of physical activity in former players was associated with lower levels of smoking and obesity than that of the general public (Mackay et al., 2019). Further criticisms suggest current heading guidelines are not stringent enough, and that given limitations on heading exposure are arbitrary and risk providing a false sense of protection (Schuppe, 2015). Moreover, Dr Dominic Malcolm, Reader in the Sociology of Sport at Loughborough University, voices concerns that heading guidelines act simply to appease worried parents: “This is not evidence-based policy but a desire to be seen to be doing something.” (as cited in Topping, 2020).

5

A. Gilbert

As will be discussed in more detail, the subject of heading in football as being a possibly deleterious mechanism of long-term brain injury represents an issue of scientific uncertainty. Despite indications that a career of prolonged heading exposure may underlie cumulative, subconcussive damage (Mackay et al., 2019), the clinical significance of heading is not well understood (O’Kane, 2016).

The protective measures introduced in the form of junior heading guidelines reflect a risk management approach akin to that of the “precautionary principle,” wherein the limited available evidence has prompted decision makers to err on the side of caution.

These aspects of uncertainty and precaution have significant implications for public health intervention, not the least of which pertains to how these concepts are communicated between researchers, decision makers and the general public. A 2016 survey from the US revealed most people agree with the notion that neurodegenerative disease stemming from repeated brain trauma in sport represents a serious public health issue (Dyck & Talty, 2016). This survey also showed that the same proportion of adults who consider tackling in American football unsafe for pre-high school children (~60%) also believe heading in football is unsafe for children in this age group.

To date, no empirical study has investigated the New Zealand public’s perception of heading in football as a possible health risk factor. The remainder of this thesis aims to provide a detailed description of the uncertainty and precaution inherent to the issue of heading as a potential health risk, and presents an exploratory analysis of the perceived risk that New Zealanders attribute to the intentional act of heading in football. Furthermore, this topic is employed as a means of investigating communicative health science outcomes influenced by uncertainty and precaution, with the aim of contributing to a wider understanding of effective science communication under such uncertain conditions.

6

A. Gilbert

1.3 Thesis structure

This initial chapter has introduced the topic of subconcussion research, including a discussion of the concerns regarding heading in football as a mechanism of cumulative brain injury. Chapter one also describes two examples of heading regulation adopted in response to these growing anxieties, discussing a brief background of the nature and implementation of the recent US and UK junior heading guidelines. This introductory chapter closes with an acknowledgment of the uncertainty and precaution inherent to junior heading regulation, presenting an opportunity to explore the implications of communicating science under these conditions.

Chapter two provides a review of the subconcussion literature, including a discussion of the key findings that have contributed to our current understanding of subconcussion and of how participation in football may factor into the development of later-life neurodegeneration. The research of interest is separated into three distinct categories: neurobiological, neuropsychological and biomechanical findings. The findings of each field are discussed in terms of their significance to the subconcussion literature and how each has contributed to our understanding of heading as a potential risk. Most importantly, Chapter two aims to illustrate the degree of scientific uncertainty that currently pervades the football heading literature.

Chapter three discusses scientific uncertainty in general, illustrating different forms of uncertainty and how these come to permeate certain areas of scientific enquiry. This chapter discusses some of the implications that uncertainty presents for policy makers, followed by a particular focus on the significance of uncertainty to the practise of science communication, and a consideration of how the presentation of uncertainty can influence public perceptions of risk.

Chapter four introduces the precautionary principle as a decision-rule employed by policy makers faced with uncertain risks. Opening with a description of the precautionary principle’s history and variety of formulations, Chapter four continues with a consideration of the principle’s critical appraisal, highlighting the pros and cons identified by researchers in risk management. This chapter also discusses research into the communication of precautionary information and the effect this can have on risk perceptions.

In the fifth chapter, a description of the methods employed in undergoing this investigation is provided, including an explanation of the study design and resources used to manipulate the experimental factors of uncertainty and precaution.

Chapter six summarises the main findings of the research component of this thesis. Lastly, Chapter seven discusses the relevant theory explaining observed trends in risk perception and considers the broader implications of communicating uncertainty and precaution in the context

7

A. Gilbert of heading as a potential health risk. The final chapter concludes with a discussion of methodological limitations, suggestions and directions for further research in this area.

8

A. Gilbert

Subconcussion literature

2.1 Neurobiological findings 2.1.1 Neuroimaging

Experimental studies employing neuroimaging techniques are among the most important in the developing subconcussion literature (Davenport et al., 2016). While there is a substantial background of neuroimaging research into concussive head impacts, or mTBI (Shenton et al., 2012), studies exploring the relationship between repeated subconcussive head impacts and changes in the brain’s microstructure have begun to mount, even in patients without a history of concussion (Koerte, Ertl-Wagner, Reiser, Zafonte & Shenton, 2012; Lipton et al., 2013).

The repeated heading of a football as a mechanism for this subconcussive effect on brain anatomy was presented as early as 1989, following Sortland and Tysvaer’s (1989) examination of former professional players. Thirty-three former representatives of the Norwegian National Football Team underwent cerebral computational tomography (CT) scans which were observed for evidence of cerebral atrophy (i.e., widening of the cerebral cortical sulci and enlargement of the ventricular system). The authors concluded that one third of the examined players exhibited cerebral atrophy above normal values and considered this to be a result of “multiple small head injuries mainly connected with heading” (Sortland & Tysvaer, 1989, p. 48). However, similar studies have yielded conflicting results. For instance, in a study of 25 active players from the United States Men’s National Team training camp, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) findings did not support a relationship between reported heading frequency and increased brain structural change (Jordan, Green, Galanty, Mandelbaum & Jabour, 1996). There was also no significant increase in brain abnormalities compared to a control group of track athletes, with the authors concluding, “any evidence of encephalopathy in soccer players relates more to acute head injuries received playing soccer than from repetitive heading” (p. 205).

Notwithstanding their noted limitations (e.g., the lack of comparative CT scans from similar-aged Norwegian males; higher age of players considered experienced “headers” of the ball; see Sortland & Tysvaer, 1989), each of these studies illustrates a common difficulty in subconcussion research. Conventional CT and MRI techniques have proven to be insufficient tools for observing the subtle changes in brain structure typical of subconcussive impacts (Koerte, Lin, Willems et al., 2015), so the findings of these early studies should be considered with caution. Using T1- weighted MRI scanning—a more sensitive neuroimaging modality (Koerte, Lin, Willems et al., 2015)—evidence of greater cortical thinning has been observed in former professional players in comparison to non-contact athletes (Koerte et al., 2016). Kemp, Duff and Hampson’s (2016) 5-

9

A. Gilbert year longitudinal study of active amateur footballers (aged 18-19) utilised similar highly sensitive MRI scans. However, these yielded no evidence of increased atrophy at baseline or 5-year follow- up.

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) has proven to be a particularly effective tool in this area, as diffuse axonal injury tends to be the most commonly observed brain alteration in mTBI (Koerte, Lin, Willems et al., 2015). As a measure of water molecule diffusion in tissue, fractional anisotropy (FA) observed through DTI can provide information regarding the integrity of the brain’s white matter microstructure (Koerte, Lin, Willems et al., 2015). Amateur football players have exhibited a relationship between heading and lower FA within various brain areas, suggesting diffuse axonal damage (Lipton et al., 2013). This association was not explained by a history of diagnosed concussion and suggests white matter abnormalities may be a result of repeated subconcussive heading.

DTI findings also suggest that female players may be at a greater risk of these deleterious effects, with females exhibiting a stronger relationship between heading and lower FA, as well as a significantly higher volume of affected white matter (Rubin et al., 2018). Myer et al.’s (2018) longitudinal study of female high school football players also illustrates significant changes in white matter microstructure over the course of a regular season. Interestingly, this study also investigates the potential protective effect of jugular vein compression, finding that players who wore a compression collar for the duration of the season did not exhibit the same significant white matter changes (Rubin et al., 2018). Compression of the jugular vein aims to reduce outflow of blood from the brain, thereby reducing the brain’s overall compliance by filling the vascular “reserve volume” and providing some resistance to the slosh effect induced by head impacts (Smith et al., 2012).

Although evidence of white matter abnormalities may signal the importance of DTI studies to subconcussion research, consideration of the activities already linked to these changes—beyond heading a ball—must also be afforded. Studies observing white matter integrity in abusers of alcohol and other addictive substances reveal similar deficits in brain microstructure (Arnone, Abou-Saleh & Barrick, 2006), something that few studies investigating the effect of repeated head impacts have controlled for (Mainwaring et al., 2018).

2.1.2 Biochemical markers

Another emerging area of subconcussion research that shows promise is concerned with the biochemical markers related to neurological damage (Maher et al., 2014). Although neuroimaging

10

A. Gilbert techniques and neuropsychological evaluation provide valuable insight into the outcomes of sport-related head injury, these processes are often incapable of adequately detecting subtle effects such as diffuse axonal injury (Papa et al., 2015). The search for clinically reliable measures of monitoring head injury has intensified over the last 10 years, resulting in an influx of traumatic brain injury biomarker studies (Papa et al., 2015). To date, CTE is only diagnosable post-mortem, therefore, significant incentive has been placed on finding objective biomarkers of the neurodegenerative disease that may provide much earlier diagnosis and prognosis (Baugh et al., 2012).

CTE pathology is primarily characterised by neuronal loss and deposits of hyperphosphorylated tau protein, rendering tau a biological marker of great histological significance (Bailes et al., 2013). Alosco et al.’s (2017) study of 96 former NFL (National Football League (American football)) players investigates the efficacy of total blood plasma tau (t-tau) as an in vivo biomarker for CTE, finding that greater repetitive head impact exposure predicted higher levels of tau in blood plasma. There were no differences in the plasma tau levels of former NFL players compared to controls of the same age (40-69), however, suggesting injuries indicative of CTE were “below threshold” for most former players in the study (Alosco et al., 2017). Kawata et al. (2018) examine the acute changes in plasma tau levels in current collegiate-level American football players following training camps, finding no relationship between rate of subconcussive impacts and increased plasma tau. However, the absence of this relationship in these studies perhaps reflects the inadequacy of tau as a biochemical marker of subconcussive injury. As a biomarker of general cortical axonal damage, plasma tau does not allow for differentiation between distinct tauopathies (Alosco et al., 2017), and inconsistencies between plasma and CSF levels of tau are known to arise in the head trauma literature (Papa et al., 2015).

The role of neuroinflammation in mediating subconcussive injury is one of increasing academic interest. The neuroinflammation theory posits that axonal injury incurred by cumulative head impacts may result in chronic inflammation and a cascade of deleterious effects (Bailes et al., 2013). Signs of neuroinflammation and early tauopathy have been observed in animal model studies, such as in Tagge et al.’s (2018) rat model of concussion. Following induced mTBI, rats exhibited blood–brain barrier disruption, microgliosis, neuroinflammation, phosphorylated tauopathy, and electrophysiological dysfunction, all of which showed no correlation to neurobehavioural dysfunction (Tagge et al., 2018). Evidence of increased neuroinflammation has also been observed in a human cohort without any history of concussion, with Koerte, Lin, Muehlmann et al.’s (2015) study of 11 former professional footballers observing a relationship between heading frequency and increased markers of neuroinflammation compared to former non-contact sport controls.

11

A. Gilbert

Shultz, MacFabe, Foley, Taylor and Cain (2011) employed mild lateral fluid percussion to induce head injury in their rat model of subconcussion. Immunohistochemical analysis found a significant increase in microglia/macrophages in rats following subconcussive injury compared to controls. Microglia are integral actors in the neuroinflammatory response, and their increased levels are perhaps indicative of neuroinflammation following repetitive subconcussive events (Shultz et al., 2011). Increased levels of nerve growth factor (NGF) and brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF) in the blood serum of 17 professional football players following a controlled bout of heading also suggests a neuroprotective response to microtrauma (Bamac et al., 2011). While these neurotrophic factors are argued to be indicators of brain tissue damage (Hicks, Martin, Zhang & Seroogy, 1999), these results should be treated with caution, as NGF and BDNF are produced by other tissues in the body, and their blood serum levels may simply be a result of increased blood-brain barrier permeability (Bamac et al., 2011).

As in Myer et al.’s (2018) longitudinal study, Smith et al. (2012) also examine the effects of jugular compression in reducing traumatic axonal injury due to the slosh effect, although within the context of an animal model of concussion. A control group of rats without compression collars exhibited greater numbers of axons positive for amyloid precursor protein (APP) following concussion, compared to rats wearing a collar at the time of injury (Smith et al., 2012). Reports of head injury resulting in eventual Alzheimer’s disease pathology have been linked to increased APP expression (Spear, 1995), and abundant accumulation of amyloid plaque presents in approximately 50% of CTE patients (Blaylock & Maroon, 2011). It is important to note, however, that Smith et al. (2012) aimed to simulate concussion in their rat model, and it is unknown whether repetitive subconcussive injury presents the same risk of this “amyloid cascade” and subsequent onset of dementia (Spear, 1995).

Neurofilament light protein (NF-L) is a constituent of the cytoskeleton responsible for supporting the growth and function of large myelinated axons (Rubin et al., 2019), and represents a promising biomarker of acute neuroinflammation and microtrauma following repetitive head impacts (Mainwaring et al., 2018). Neselius et al.’s (2014) study of 30 seasoned amateur boxers saw an increase in CSF NF-L in 77% of participants following a bout, compared to that of non- boxing controls. However, neuropsychological evaluation exhibited no significant difference between boxers and controls. Rubin et al. (2019) builds upon the efficacy of NF-L as a biomarker with their study of 18 collegiate-level American football players. Analysis of blood samples taken pre- and post-training, alongside impact data recorded by mouthguard accelerometers, showed that subconcussive impact frequency and magnitude were predictors of increased plasma NF-L levels (Rubin et al., 2019). The potential neuroprotective effect of Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) presents an interesting avenue for further research, as the ingestion of DHA by collegiate

12

A. Gilbert

American football players over the course of a season appeared to attenuate serum NF-L levels compared to a placebo control group (Oliver et al., 2016). Similar biomarker results have been replicated in a controlled heading experiment, with collegiate-level footballers exhibiting raised serum NF-L after both 1 hour and at 22 days following an acute bout of heading (Wallace et al., 2018).

S-100β is a calcium-binding protein that reflects injury to glial cells (Zetterberg et al., 2007), and is the most commonly examined biochemical marker in the traumatic brain injury literature (Papa et al., 2015). Kawata et al. (2017) observed significant increases in plasma S-100β of collegiate American football players following repeated subconcussive impacts, with number of impacts, as well as the sum of peak linear head acceleration (PLA) and peak rotational head acceleration (PRA), significantly associated with these increases. S-100β is also the biomarker most commonly found to increase with exposure to heading (Maher et al., 2014), as was observed in Stålnacke, Ohlsson, Tegner and Sojka’s (2006) study of 44 female football players. Despite the correlation observed between heading and increased S-100β, similar findings in non-contact athletes without head trauma potentially signals the protein’s release from other cells like chondrocytes and adipocytes, calling into question S-100β’s specificity to the brain (Papa et al., 2015).

Mainwaring et al.’s (2018) review of the subconcussion literature highlights biomarker research as an area showing particular promise, emphasising the need for further exploratory and confirmatory studies. Maher et al. (2014) share this sentiment, acknowledging the great potential of both neuroimaging and biomarker studies for assessing concussive and subconcussive consequences, given the research continues to be fine-tuned. Biomarker research has not received a positive consensus, however, and evidence of no change in biomarkers including S- 100β, NF-L and tau following exposure to heading a football (Dorminy et al., 2015; Straume- Næsheim, Andersen, Jochum, Dvorak & Bahr, 2008; Zetterberg et al., 2007) illustrates the need for further refinement. Mayer et al.’s (2018) review of TBI biomarker research insists a relative paucity of biomarkers that are able to provide objective measures of head injury, especially as it pertains to the rapidly changing brains of children.

13

A. Gilbert

2.2 Neuropsychological findings

A prominent subfield of subconcussion research concerns the functional changes brought on by repeated mild head impacts. Most behaviours arise from complex interactions between structures in the brain, therefore, it holds that any disruption to these processes may result in measurable disturbances to functional brain activity (McCrory, Makdissi, Davis & Collie, 2005). Cognitive impairment is recognised as a potential short or long-term consequence of concussion (Gysland et al., 2012), as well as a major feature of CTE during latter stages of the disease (McCrory, Meeuwisse, Kutcher, Jordan & Gardner, 2013). A considerable portion of studies discussed here utilise cognitive tests that have proven useful for gauging neuropsychological deficit in concussed athletes (Echemendia, Putukian, Mackin, Julian & Shoss, 2001). The subject of heading in football provides a unique quandary, however, as the neuropsychological effects of subconcussion—independent of concussion—are not well understood (McCrory et al., 2005).

Tysvaer, Storli and Bachen’s (1989) investigation of former professional football players exhibits somewhat abnormal electroencephalography (EEG) readings compared to that of matched, non- athlete controls—findings that are also replicated in Tysvaer and Storli’s (1989) study of active professionals. Neuropsychological testing also exhibited impaired memory and lack of concentration in the players within these studies, among other symptoms of mTBI. However, the utility of abnormal EEG recordings as a marker of minor brain damage remains questionable, as these values lack specificity and such abnormalities are found in ~10% of the population (Spear, 1995). Similar findings have been observed in active professional (Matser et al., 1998) and amateur players (Matser, Kessels, Lezak, Jordan & Troost, 1999), with those involved in football faring worse than controls in cognitive tasks including planning and memory.

Tysvaer and Storli (1989) note that younger players were more likely to exhibit abnormal EEGs than older players, a finding the authors suggest could be explained by a combination of smaller, weaker necks and less experience of correct heading technique. Yet, as Rutherford, Stephens and Potter (2003) point out, such a finding is inconsistent with the notion that EEG abnormalities arise in proportion to one’s lifetime exposure to heading. Downs and Abwender (2002) build upon these early neuropsychological findings, illustrating poorer cognitive performance among US collegiate and professional players compared to collegiate swimmers. Contrary to Tysvaer and Sortli (1989), Downs and Abwender (2002) hypothesised that older players would exhibit poorer outcomes than their younger counterparts. The resulting cognitive deficit apparent in older players supported their hypothesis, leading to the proposal of a dose-response relationship between heading exposure and cognitive decline.

14

A. Gilbert

Further studies have revealed a link between estimated lifetime heading exposure and cognitive function, with higher estimates predicting poorer outcomes in neuropsychological batteries (Witol & Webbe, 2003); while both recent heading (in the last 7 days) and high lifetime frequency combine to make for poorer cognitive outcomes than other such combinations of heading recency and frequency (Webbe & Ochs, 2003).

Lipton et al.’s (2013) multimodal study of amateur football players found those with the highest annual frequency of heading exhibited both white matter structural abnormalities (reduced FA) and poorer performance on memory tasks than those within a “medium” and “low” heading frequency range. Heading frequency exhibited a non-linear relationship with these structural and functional changes, introducing the concept of a potential “heading threshold”, above which the risk of TBI increases significantly. This relationship prompted Lipton et al. (2013) to suggest that, “repair mechanisms may be unable to keep pace with the cumulative injury that occurs beyond this degree of exposure” (p. 854). Of note is the apparent disparity between the heading threshold for reduced FA and that of poorer cognitive function, with a higher exposure threshold predicting the latter (1800 headers per year). The authors assume this difference may relate to differential lead times, wherein pathological change (WM abnormalities) precedes the clinical manifestation of impaired cognitive function (memory deficit). Svaldi et al. (2017) similarly observe a potential non-linear relationship between head impact exposure and neurophysiological change, with asymptomatic female players exhibiting decreased cerebrovascular reactivity (CVR)—a known biomarker of mTBI—over the course of a season. Levitch et al. (2019) report impaired performance on the same cognitive battery employed in Lipton et al. (2013); however, this impairment was differentially associated between recent (reduced psychomotor speed) and long-term (poorer verbal memory) heading.

Early research such as the Norwegian (Tysvaer et al., 1989; Tysvaer & Storli, 1989) and Dutch (Matser et al., 1998; Matser et al., 1999) studies are widely referenced in the subconcussion literature, with their findings supporting a relationship between purposeful heading and cognitive decline (Kirkendall et al., 2001). However, Kirkendall et al.’s (2001) review of the early subconcussion literature notes the curiously low number of reported concussions in the Norwegian studies, citing difficulty in defining concussive episodes as a possible explanation. Meanwhile, Matser, Kessels and Troost (2001) illustrate differential consequences of concussion and purposeful heading, with each variable leading to impairment in separate domains of cognition. As will be discussed further, confounding concussion and subconcussion is just one methodological shortfall common to the neuropsychological literature.

15

A. Gilbert

2.2.1 Confounding concussion & subconcussion

The utility of neuropsychological evaluation for the early detection of concussion is well supported in the traumatic head injury literature (Neselius et al., 2014). Concerns regarding the neurological consequence of sport-related concussion have also been well documented in recent years, buoyed on by research into the increased risk of cognitive impairment resulting from multiple concussions throughout a professional career (Guskiewicz et al., 2005). A history of multiple sport-related concussions has been linked to increased severity of depressive symptoms in former NFL players (Didehbani et al., 2013), while multiple concussions predict higher rates of depression diagnosis in amateur football players (Hunter et al., 2018). Both animal models (Tagge et al., 2018) and human studies (Terwilliger, Pratson, Vaughan & Gioia, 2016) illustrate the increased susceptibility to further head injury that single concussive episodes impose, as well as the significantly increased burden of symptoms arising from continued head trauma.

Killam, Cautin and Santucci (2005) observe neuropsychological dysfunction in concussed contact-sport athletes compared to non-concussed and non-athlete controls. Participants experiencing concussion within the last 2 years were considered recently concussed, with these athletes exhibiting particular deficit in tasks of immediate memory recall. The authors posit that this alteration to cognitive functioning may resolve over time, as non-recently concussed athletes did not share this outcome, implying some features of cognitive impairment may be transitory in nature. Interestingly, contact sport athletes without a history of concussion still performed worse than non-athlete controls, suggesting the burden of repeated, subconcussive impacts may still result in cognitive sequelae (Killam et al., 2005).

Talavage et al. (2014) adopt a similar approach by comparing the cognitive performance of concussed and non-concussed high school American football players. While non-concussed players were initially intended as matched controls for the concussed group, the authors discovered a subset of these players who still exhibited neuropsychological dysfunction in the absence of clinically observed concussion. Gysland et al.’s (2012) findings seem to contradict that of both Killam et al. (2005) and Talavage et al. (2014), wherein neither prior concussion history nor exposure to subconcussive impacts related to neuropsychological outcomes following a season of collegiate American football. Bailes et al. (2013) questions the sensitivity of cognitive assessment tools such as the Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metric (ANAM) used by Gysland et al. (2012), however, suggesting these measures may be insufficient to detect subclinical neurological dysfunction in athletes. Pearce (2016) notes that both Talavage et al. (2014) and Gysland et al. (2012) only tested for cognitive impairment following an entire season

16

A. Gilbert of American football play, whereas the acute consequences of subconcussive impacts would ideally be measured within 1 to 10 days of exposure.

Ellemberg, Leclerc, Couture and Daigle (2007) observe prolonged cognitive deficit in female collegiate-level football players for at least 8 months following first-time concussion. Forbes et al. (2016), on the other hand, highlights no pre- to post-season difference in neuropsychological function between previously concussed and non-concussed female high school players. An important factor to note in the neuropsychological literature—and heading studies in general— is the potential role that subconcussive blows play in the facilitation of cognitive dysfunction, including the possible hindrance of recovery following previous insult (Bazarian et al., 2014). The average onset of most recent concussion in Forbes et al. (2016) occurred 27 months prior to investigation, suggesting this period of time is sufficient to see cognitive dysfunction resolve. Forbes et al.’s (2016) neuropsychological test scores might have reflected that of Ellemberg et al. (2007) had participants been examined 6-8 months following concussion. Another possible explanation for Forbes et al.’s (2016) findings is the low frequency of heading carried out by participants, averaging 26.5 headers per player throughout the entire season. Kaminski et al. (2013) similarly observed no cognitive deficit in female high school players following a season in which players averaged 1 header per game. Female collegiate football players have been shown to incur more than twice the exposure to heading than those at a high school level (McCuen et al., 2015), and a comparison of Ellemberg et al. (2007) and Forbes et al.’s (2016) reported mean headers reflects this trend. Therefore, one might expect to observe this difference between collegiate and high school players, especially if participants are afforded a far longer recovery time.

Head impacts that do not include intentional heading (e.g., head-to-head, elbow-to-head) are also shown to alter cognitive function in professional football players with and without clinically diagnosable concussion (Straume-Næsheim et al., 2009). Stewart et al. (2018) made the obverse finding of no cognitive deficit arising from unintentional head impacts, while intentional heading was significantly associated with reduced performance on psychomotor speed and attention tasks. Both intentional heading and unintentional head impacts have displayed independent associations with moderate to severe CNS symptoms in amateur football players (Stewart et al., 2017). Taken together, these studies further highlight the difficulties that arise when attempting to parse information regarding the consequences of heading alone from the multiple other factors contributing to a player’s total head impact burden.

Several reviews of the literature identify the confounding of concussion and subconcussion as a significant methodological limitation of neuropsychological studies (Kontos et al., 2017; Maher et al., 2014; Mainwaring et al., 2018; O’Kane, 2016; Tarnutzer, Straumann, Brugger & Fedderman-

17

A. Gilbert

Demont, 2017). Kirkendall et al. (2001) suggest difficulties in defining concussion may contribute to this confounding, and that this issue of semantics might account for the relatively low number of concussions reported in early studies such as Tysvaer and Lochen (1991). Attempting to isolate any cognitive deficit attributable to subconcussion appears difficult. Players tend not to recognise concussion symptoms, and these symptoms are commonly underreported—even when recognised (Straume-Næsheim et al., 2009). However, given the well-understood decremental effect of concussion on cognitive test performance (Harmon et al., 2013), the potential confounding of concussive and subconcussive effects is important to consider.

2.2.2 Limited evidence of dysfunction

Contrary to the findings of early Norwegian and Dutch studies, Straume-Næsheim, Andersen, Dvorak and Bahr’s (2005) much larger cohort of Norwegian professional footballers (n=271) reports no relationship between cognitive impairment and concussion history or heading frequency. Unlike Tysvaer and Lochen’s (1991) findings of mild to severe cognitive decline in 30 of 37 former professional players, Vann Jones, Breakey and Evans (2014) found impairment in only 10 of 92 former professional UK footballers studied (as assessed by completion of the “Test Your Memory” questionnaire). This rate of impairment falls in line with that expected of the general public, suggesting any cognitive deficit from heading in football may simply be transient.

Studies comparing different athletic groups likewise yield limited evidence to support the relationship between purposeful heading and cognitive impairment. Grouping football players into a “moderate-contact” group alongside softball and basketball, Tsushima et al. (2018) observe no deficit in neuropsychological function compared to “low-contact” sport athletes (cross- country, baseball, volleyball, water polo and tennis). “High-contact” sports did exhibit slightly poorer performance than the lower contact groups, this dysfunction being attributed to cumulative subconcussive impacts in the absence of sport-related concussion (Tsushima et al., 2018). Notably, athlete groups were determined by relative risk of concussion—hence the inclusion of football into a moderate-contact group. The authors admit to this approach qualifying as an “indirect assessment” of subconcussive head trauma, as impact exposure in the form of heading does not factor into this consideration. Guskiewicz, Marshall, Broglio, Cantu and Kirkendall (2002) observe no difference in cognitive battery scores between division 1 collegiate football players and controls consisting of non-football athletes and students, nor did heading frequency or prior concussion history influence neuropsychological outcomes. The same trends are observed between football, rugby, and non-contact athletes in adolescent (Stephens,

18

A. Gilbert

Rutherford, Potter & Fernie, 2005; Stephens, Rutherford, Potter & Fernie, 2010) and collegiate populations (Rutherford, Stephens, Fernie & Potter, 2009). The only significant interaction detected in these cross-sectional studies was a slight deficit in performing tasks of attention in players with a history of concussion, and this dysfunction was apparent across all sporting groups.

Several studies observe the neuropsychological effects of heading under controlled conditions. Gutierrez, Conte and Lightbourne’s (2014) study of female high school players reveals no decrease in cognitive battery scores following a series of 15 headers, although a negative relationship between neck strength and resultant head acceleration was noted. Koerte et al. (2017) likewise observe no deficit following controlled heading; rather, youth football players exhibited slightly improved sensorimotor function following training involving exposure to heading, an improvement the authors attribute to the “well-known immediate benefits of physical exercise” (p. 2393). However, while football players exhibited no reduction in sensorimotor task performance from pre- to post-season, they also lacked the improvement on tasks requiring inhibition that the control group displayed over the course of the study. This difference leads the authors to suggest that the potential cognitive benefits enabled by physical exercise may be offset by repetitive head impacts.

Rieder and Jansen (2011) observe no cognitive deficit attributable to heading when comparing a training group’s battery scores to that of matched non-heading controls. Jansen and Lehmann (2018) build upon this study by increasing exposure in the heading group to include 4 sessions of 100 headers each—aiming to better reflect Lipton et al.’s (2013) reported median of 432 headers per season for adult amateur players—although this increase still yields no evidence of cognitive dysfunction. Gallant, Drumheller and McKelvie (2017) compare the effect of “proper” and “improper” heading technique on serial reaction time task (SRTT) performance. To manipulate technique, previously inexperienced individuals were recruited and randomly assigned to either an experimental group, which received instruction on correct heading technique, or a control group, which did not. Following a short heading session, neither group exhibited decreased SRTT performance. These results are consistent with previous findings that heading during training sessions does not result in any acute cognitive deficit (Putukian, Echemendia & Mackin, 2000). That said, critics of this empirical method might question whether a 10-minute training session is sufficient to evoke “proper” heading technique in individuals lacking any prior experience.

19

A. Gilbert

2.2.3 Alternative studies of function

Studies investigating oculomotor function have identified near point of convergence (NPC) as a potential indicator of subconcussive trauma. NPC measures the nearest point at which an individual can maintain convergence (inward eye movement). This adduction is controlled by the oculomotor nerve, and an increase in NPC measurement (reflecting poorer oculomotor function) is well understood to be a primary indicator of mTBI (Kawata, Tierney, Phillips & Jeka, 2016). Increased NPC has now also been observed following exposure to subconcussive impacts in both American football (Kawata, Rubin, et al., 2016; Zonner et al., 2019) and football (Kawata et al., 2016). Kawata et al. (2016) observe increased NPC measures following 10 controlled headers compared to a non-heading control group, with this change in oculomotor function persisting for at least 24 hours. Kawata, Rubin, et al.’s (2016) investigation of American football players illustrates worsened NPC among those in a higher impact frequency group compared to those in a lower impact group. This study increased the time period for observation and found NPC changes in the high impact group persisted for 3 weeks following exposure to tackling before correcting to pre-season baseline. Zonner et al.’s (2019) similar findings support the notion that subconcussive impacts have at least a transient effect on oculomotor function, and indicate that a higher cumulative impact burden may deter intrinsic mechanisms of repair (Lipton et al., 2013).

Di Virgilio et al. (2016) utilise transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to discern corticomotor inhibition following controlled heading in a group of 19 amateur players. Increased corticomotor inhibition, along with worsened memory performance, was measured immediately following heading exposure. These results normalised after 24 hours, further suggesting the transient nature of functional deficits caused by heading. There is also evidence of a transient effect on postural stability following controlled heading (Haran, Tierney, Wright, Keshner & Silter, 2012; Hwang, Ma, Kawata, Tierney & Jeka, 2017), although evidence from similar controlled studies (Broglio, Guskiewicz, Sell & Broglio, 2004; Mangus, Wallmann & Leford, 2004) and season-long observational research (Kaminski, Wikstrom, Gutierrez & Glutting, 2007) contradicts these findings. Interestingly, despite observing no change in postural control, Broglio et al.’s (2004) controlled heading was carried out with a ball velocity of 55 mph—more than double that of Haran et al.’s (2012) study (25 mph). While these inconsistent findings suggest postural stability may have limited utility for subconcussion research, O’Kane (2016) notes that this difference may be due to Haran et al.’s (2012) use of a more sensitive postural stability measure in the form of an infrared motion analysis system.

20

A. Gilbert

2.2.4 Head injury symptoms

Investigations into the relationship between head impacts and reported symptoms have similarly found mixed results. Studies observing no relationship between heading frequency and cognitive outcomes nevertheless report a considerable portion of youth players complaining of headache symptoms following bouts of heading (Janda et al., 2002; Salinas, Webbe & Devore, 2009). Further studies report no change in cognitive performance or symptom disclosure following heading among high school and collegiate players (Kontos, Dolese, Elbin, Covassin & Warren, 2011; Putukian et al., 2000). This difference in symptom prevalence may be a result of children being more willing to report symptoms such as headaches (Salinas et al., 2009), although Janda et al. (2002) note there may be some difficulty in differentiating between reported headache resulting from “mild head injuries” and reported symptoms reflecting localised pain due to ball impact. There is some evidence to suggest that males and females also differentially report symptoms following concussive episodes and bouts of heading (Colvin et al., 2009; Jansen & Lehmann, 2018; Rieder & Jansen, 2011), with females being more likely to complain of symptoms than males, although this is not a consistent finding in the neuropsychological literature (Zuckerman et al., 2012).

Jordan et al.’s (1996) study of the U.S. Men’s National Soccer Team observes a significant relationship between head/neck injury symptoms and history of acute head injuries—although heading frequency did not correlate with reported symptoms or MRI results. Heading and unintentional head impacts are found to differentially influence self-reported CNS symptoms, with exposure to unintentional head impacts (e.g., head-to-head, head-to-goal post) largely explaining the occurrence of head/neck injury symptoms (Stewart et al., 2017). Beyond symptoms such as headaches and dizziness, unintentional head impacts during football play have also shown to correlate with worsened Patient Reported Outcomes (PRO’s), including anxiety, depression, sleep disturbance and sleep impairment (Hunter et al., 2018). An unexpected finding of Hunter et al.’s (2018) study is that heading frequency did not predict worsened PRO’s—rather, players in the 3rd quartile of intentional heading exposure displayed better outcomes. The authors suggest that heading frequency may reflect overall football activity, meaning players reporting greater heading frequency may prosper from the mental and physical health benefits of general physical activity (Hiles et al., 2017). The lack of PRO improvement in 4th quartile headers might suggest repetitive head impacts above a certain threshold offset the benefits of increased physical activity (Hunter et al., 2018).

21

A. Gilbert

2.2.5 Critical assessment

Although studies concerning brain function make up for the majority of subconcussion literature (Rodrigues, Lasmar & Caramelli, 2016), a common sentiment is that these studies largely fail to demonstrate a relationship between heading in football and neuropsychological decline (Belanger et al., 2016; Kontos et al., 2017; O’Kane, 2016; Rodrigues et al., 2016; Rutherford et al., 2003; Tarnutzer et al., 2017). While the majority of neuropsychological studies discover no connection between heading in football and some deficit in cognitive function, it is important to note that this entire subfield of research is hampered by methodological limitations (Rutherford et al., 2003).

A common issue highlighted by reviews of the neuropsychological literature is the use of inadequate control groups (Maher et al., 2014; Mainwaring et al., 2018; Tarnutzer et al., 2017). For instance, participants in Putukian et al. (2000) act as both headers of the ball and controls across two different sessions. Their results show no difference in cognitive function between the two groups; however, this does not account for potential differences between those included in the study and individuals of non-contact sport populations. Tarnutzer et al. (2017) note that studies reporting a relationship between heading and cognitive deficit were more likely to include inappropriate control groups, as well as insufficient control of type I error, than studies that failed to observe such a relationship. Rutherford et al. (2009) is one of the few neuropsychological studies that addresses the inflation of type I error that arises through testing multiple hypotheses, and their study observed no relationship between heading and cognitive deficit.

Because individuals are not randomly allocated to a sporting group, the comparison of individuals by participation and non-participation in different sports presents a risk of internal validity bias (Rutherford et al., 2003). For example, while differences in cognitive function between a group of football players and a group of non-football players may be due to the former’s exposure to heading; there is no guarantee that differences in test scores are not the result of other factors that vary between these “intact” groups. Therefore, the inclusion of controls matched on variables beyond just sport participation is essential for making inferences from the subconcussion literature (Mainwaring et al., 2018).

Consideration should be afforded to the time period in which participants engaged in heading, as retired players who were active during the 1960s and ‘70s were largely exposed to leather balls that could increase by 20% of their own mass when wet (Kirkendall et al., 2001). Anecdotal evidence of increased head injury symptoms such as headache and nausea appear to implicate the use of earlier leather footballs, e.g., “most players of that era freely admit that heading a wet, heavy ball could lead to the reported symptoms” (Kirkendall & Garret, 2001, p. 332). Evidence

22

A. Gilbert from early neuropsychological studies including players from the era of leather footballs should, therefore, be viewed with caution.

Difficulties in interpreting the neuropsychological literature are only compounded by the range of tools employed in the measuring of cognitive function. Tarnutzer et al.’s (2017) review of this literature notes the appropriate use of cognitive batteries that address the three key domains of memory, attention and executive functioning across most studies. However, the authors also observe more than 60 unique cognitive tests employed across the span of brain function studies. These tests often include those developed and validated specifically for use in sporting contexts, such as the Immediate Post-Concussion Assessment and Cognitive Test (ImPACT); Sideline Concussion Assessment Tool 3 (SCAT3), and ANAM cognitive batteries (Maher et al., 2014; Mainwaring et al., 2018). Although their reliability in “sideline” concussion assessment is well documented (Maher et al., 2014); McCrory et al. (2005) stress that these batteries are primarily used as screening tools for concussion and may lack the sensitivity to provide comprehensive evaluations of cognitive function. Tsushima et al. (2018) address concerns relating specifically to use of the ImPACT instrument. While their investigation of cognitive function between high, moderate and low contact groups yielded no relationship to subconcussive exposure, questions persist as to the utility of established concussion assessments in identifying more subtle cognitive sequelae.

Hunter et al.’s (2018) decision to investigate the relationship between head impact exposure and Patient Reported Outcomes, rather than cognitive battery performance, illustrates concerns surrounding brain function studies. The authors state that their “focus on PROs reflects growing recognition that standardized cognitive batteries may be insensitive to deficits of complex functioning that characterize mild TBIs” (p. 395).

Evidence of axonal injury in the absence of any measurable cognitive deficit (Neselius et al., 2014) highlights the potential inadequacy of neuropsychological testing as a single measure of brain injury. Mainwaring et al. (2018) suggest all studies which employ cognitive testing as the sole measurement of subconcussive change should be viewed under scrutiny, and a multimodal approach should instead be incentivised. Studies incorporating magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) alongside neuropsychological testing support this argument, as increased biomarkers of neuroinflammation are observed in relation to short-term (Poole et al., 2014) and long-term (Koerte, Lin, Willems et al., 2015) head impact exposure of non-concussed contact-sport athletes—despite no evidence of cognitive impairment eventuating. Meanwhile, further multimodal studies observe no change in brain structure or function related to heading exposure (Dorminy et al., 2015; Kemp et al., 2016). The variable findings of these studies highlights the need for multiple approaches towards characterising subconcussive change.

23

A. Gilbert

To date, neuropsychological studies in the subconcussion literature might be considered exploratory at best, with little evidence to support the relationship between observable cognitive impairment and heading in football (Belanger et al., 2016; Kontos et al., 2017; Mainwaring et al., 2018; O’Kane, 2016; Rodrigues et al., 2016; Rutherford et al., 2003; Tarnutzer et al., 2017). The limited evidence of functional change compared to that of structural change in the subconcussion literature leads one to question the injurious nature of heading as far as cognitive function is concerned (Rodrigues et al., 2016). As Neselius et al. (2014) notes, however, “Absence of clinical symptoms/cognitive impairment after concussion does not seem to be equivalent to absence of brain injury” (p. 7). The same may hold true for subconcussive injury, and it would appear essential that future studies do not rely solely on neuropsychological measures.

Cross-sectional studies may be inadequate to implicate heading as a specific cause of dysfunction (Rodrigues et al., 2016), and a significant onus has been placed on researchers in their production of longitudinal data to better understand this relationship (Kirkendall & Garrett, 2001). Nonetheless, studies investigating the potential threshold dose-response relationship between head impact exposure and cognitive impairment yield intriguing data (Lipton et al., 2013; Montenigro et al., 2017), and further research aimed at characterising these thresholds is warranted.

24

A. Gilbert

2.3 Biomechanical findings

Biomechanical studies make up a significant subset of research in the subconcussion literature, as these studies aim to provide data on the nature and quantity of head impacts in sport (Davenport et al., 2016). Heading a football is a complex and coordinated event, and the variety of scenarios in which heading may occur introduces multiple significant variables. However, the resulting mechanics of heading a football are always dictated by the relationship: force = mass x acceleration (Kirkendall et al., 2001; Kirkendall & Garret, 2001). Variables such as linear acceleration, rotational acceleration and impact location tend to be better predictors of concussion than simply the magnitude of the force (Belanger et al., 2016). Beyond just the velocity applied to the head and body during an impact of a given force, it is the change in velocity that more accurately reflects the energy transmitted to the player (Dashnaw, Petraglia & Bailes, 2012). Therefore, researchers typically focus on four primary mechanisms of neuropathologic change in acute head injury: linear acceleration, rotational acceleration, angular acceleration and carotid artery injury (Bunc, Ravnik & Velnar, 2017).

Although the relationship between biomechanical data and the onset of concussive injury—or subconcussive injury, for that matter—is far from straight forward (Belanger et al., 2016); the substantial background of biomechanical research in American football suggests greater head accelerations are associated with cumulative changes to the brain and acute head injury (Caccese & Kaminski, 2016). Most studies investigating the biomechanics of heading in football focus on linear acceleration and, to a lesser extent, rotational acceleration (Mainwaring et al., 2018). Mainwaring et al.’s (2018) review of the subconcussion literature notes that rotational impact data may in fact be underrepresented compared to that of linear acceleration, as the shearing and tensile strains caused by the rotating of the cerebrum around the brainstem is known to be a more frequent mechanism of concussion than linear accelerations (Guskiewicz & Mihalik, 2011).

Among the American football studies reviewed by Mainwaring et al. (2018), mean peak linear accelerations (PLA) were typically observed between 20g and 30g, although the highest PLA observed was up to 940.5g in Kawata, Rubin et al. (2016). Meanwhile, the range of linear head accelerations in heading studies was between 14.49g (Hwang et al., 2017) and 50.8g (Dorminy et al., 2015), illustrating the high variability in biomechanical data currently available. Mainwaring et al. (2018) partly attribute this wide range to differences in age and level of play of participants, although a variety of differences in methodologies and metrics used across heading studies render it difficult to make any generalised observations regarding the biomechanics of heading in football (Davenport et al., 2016; King, Hume, Gissane, Brughelli & Clark, 2016; Mainwaring et al., 2018).

25

A. Gilbert

An example of the particular difficulty posed by measuring head impacts in football is made patently obvious in Naunheim, Standeven, Richter and Lewis’s (2000) study comparing head accelerations across high school-level American football, ice hockey and football (soccer) players. Mounting the helmets of both the ice hockey and American football players with triaxial accelerometers, head impact data was recorded from games during the regular season. The football data, on the other hand, was obtained in a controlled condition by recording isolated headers. The results of this study found that the PLAs recorded while heading a football were up to 160% higher than those recorded during ice hockey play and up to 180% higher than those recorded during American football play. This marked increase in head acceleration during heading appears highly worrying, especially considering that 20% of the recorded headers exceeded 75g (current knowledge of a potential threshold for concussion ranges between 70–90g or higher (Mainwaring et al., 2018)). However, to record accelerations during heading, football players were also fitted with accelerometer-mounted American football helmets. While American football and ice hockey data were obtained over 3.9 and 6.5 hours of play, respectively (resulting in 158 and 161 recorded impacts), the data from heading was limited to just 25 impacts. Needless to say, the use of a helmet to record heading data is a drastic departure from the conditions of typical football play, and perhaps explains the abnormal data yielded by Naunheim et al.’s (2000) football cohort.

2.3.1 Instrumentation

It is perhaps unsurprising that the majority of human data on subconcussive impacts comes from studies of American football players, as the protective equipment ubiquitous to the sport, including mouthguards and helmets, readily accommodate accelerometer technology (Belanger et al., 2016). While advances in accelerometer technology have since made it easier to unobtrusively obtain head impact data from football players, the array of unique tools used across studies outlines the need for standardised measures (Mainwaring et al., 2018).

The majority of studies discussed here utilise some form of triaxial accelerometer to record impact data, often embedded in headbands or helmets for human subject trials or as part of simulated head impact models. An alternative to these singular accelerometers is the Head Impact Telemetry System (HITS) (Broglio et al., 2010; Davenport et al., 2014; Hanlon & Bir, 2010; Hanlon & Bir, 2012) consisting of six wireless accelerometers placed around the cranium for a more accurate measure of head acceleration (Davenport et al., 2014). Hanlon and Bir’s (2010) study of controlled heading appears to justify the use of HITS for quantifying the magnitude of linear head accelerations, as this measurement showed a high level of agreement with concurrent

26

A. Gilbert accelerometer data. Rotational head impact data, on the other hand, was consistently underreported by HITS compared to supplemental accelerometer measurements, calling into question the utility of HITS for heading studies.

Broglio et al.’s (2010) prospective study of an American football cohort implemented HITS to record all head impacts during gameplay from 2005 through to 2008, with the aim of identifying biomechanical predictors of concussion. Analysis of the 54,247 impacts and 13 confirmed cases of concussion recorded over this time yielded predictive values consistent with previously recorded concussive thresholds, suggesting HITS may harbour some value in sideline concussion diagnosis. Mainwaring et al. (2018), however, point out the potential shortcomings of HITS when it comes to obtaining data in the field, noting variables such as helmet fit, perspiration effects and hair length as factors that may influence recordings, as well as a tendency for HITS to produce both false positive and false negative recordings.

The intraoral application of accelerometers has also been utilised in multiple studies of controlled subconcussive impacts (Dorminy et al., 2015; Kawata, Rubin et al., 2016; Kawata et al., 2017; Kawata et al., 2018; Tierney et al., 2008; Withnall, Shewchenko, Wonnacott & Dvorak, 2005), although the integration of these sensors within mouthguards or biteplates represents another departure from any standardised methodology, and may introduce a further source of measurement error (Mainwaring et al., 2018). Narimatsu et al.’s (2015) finding that intentionally clenching down on a mouthguard decreases resultant head accelerations during heading perhaps increases concerns over the use of intraoral accelerometers. During controlled heading, Narimatsu et al. (2015) observed that male high school football players were able to mitigate resultant head accelerations when instructed to clench their masseter muscles—an effect that increased upon masseter-clenching with a mouthguard in place. This decrease in head acceleration was accompanied with a significant increase in both masseter and sternocleidomastoid muscle activity, suggesting that clenching—both with and without a mouthguard—acts to stabilise and increase the effective-mass of the head-neck-trunk segment in opposition to applied forces (Narimatsu et al. 2015). The mitigating effect of clenching may introduce a point of dispute regarding the use of intraoral accelerometers for subconcussion research, but perhaps more importantly, it might also represent a protective intervention worthy of further investigation.

Further alternatives for measuring head accelerations include motion capture analysis (Dezman, Ledet & Kerr, 2013; Mansell, Tierney, Sitler, Swanik & Stearne, 2005), in-ear sensors (Sandmo, McIntosh, Andersen, Koerte & Bahr, 2019), accelerometers within footballs (Stone et al., 2018)

27

A. Gilbert and biofidelic models of head impact (Shewchenko, Withnall, Keown, Gittens & Dvorak, 2005a, 2005b, 2005c; Taha & Hassan, 2016). Bauer, Thomas, Cauraugh, Kaminski and Hass (2001), as well as Broglio, Ju, Broglio and Sell (2003), differ to the majority of studies investigating heading mechanics, as both measure impact force rather than head acceleration.

Several studies investigate head accelerations using the more low-profile xPatch accelerometer (McCuen et al., 2015; Press & Rowson, 2017; Reynolds et al., 2017a, 2017b). Attached to the mastoid process behind either the left or right ear, the xPatch does not require attachment to equipment such as helmets or mouthguards, offering an attractive alternative for studies of heading during games and practice. Each using the xPatch to quantify head accelerations incurred by female collegiate-level football players, McCuen et al., (2015), Press and Rowson (2017) and Reynolds et al. (2017b) report means of 3.5, 1.69 and 5.7 impacts per practise, respectively. Coupling their recorded xPatch data with video analysis of all games and trainings, Press and Rowson (2017) found that the xPatch recorded significantly more impacts than were later confirmed via the captured footage. The authors note instances of jumping, running and players touching the xPatch as events falsely triggering impact recordings, illustrating the device’s tendency to produce type I errors.

2.3.2 Recording threshold

While false-positive errors may account for some of the variance seen in heading frequency among female collegiate practices above, Reynolds et al. (2017b) illustrate the difference that a given recording threshold can have on one’s findings. Originally recording all head impacts above a threshold of 10g, the authors show that the resulting mean value of 5.7 impacts per practice is reduced to 4.0 when the data is adjusted to reflect a recording threshold that disregards all impacts under 20g, as was employed by McCuen et al. (2015). Using the same 20g threshold, the resulting mean values of 4.0 (Reynolds et al., 2017b) and 3.5 (McCuen et al., 2015) impacts per practice show a greater level of agreement.

King et al.’s (2016) review of head impact research supports the universal implementation of a 10g threshold across all contact-sport studies, as this cut-off point enables researchers to distinguish between negligible events and genuine impacts (e.g., voluntary head movements vs. intentional heading). This argument is further sustained by illustrating the extent to which varying impact thresholds manipulate the data of a previous study of New Zealand rugby union players. Setting an impact threshold of 14.4g, the resulting data set excluded up to 42% of impacts that were recorded above the lower, 10g, threshold. Because any injurious effect of subconcussive

28

A. Gilbert impacts is likely cumulative (Bunc et al., 2017), a potential cumulative threshold of subconcussive events leading to concussive injury should remain an important consideration (King et al., 2016).

2.3.3 Concussion risk factors

Studies of concussion incidence in football suggest that female and youth players may be at a higher risk of concussive injury during the act of heading than older, male populations (O’Kane, 2016). Among collegiate sport in the United States, girls’ football has been found to have the third- highest rate of concussion, behind girls’ ice hockey and American football (Hootman, Dick & Agel, 2007). Likewise, football was found to have the joint-highest rate of concussion with American football among high school athletes, with girls faring worse than boys when playing the same sport (Gessel, Fields, Collins, Dick & Comstock, 2007). In the same study, Gessel et al. (2007) found that 18% of girls attributed their concussion to head-to-ball contact, while only 8% of boys said the same.

At an adult professional level, female football players are 2.4 times more likely to suffer concussion than men (O’Kane, 2016), while concussion rates in junior/youth football appear to be higher among younger players (11-12 years) than older players (13-14 years) (O’Kane et al., 2014). Player-to-player contact is consistently cited as the most frequent mechanism of concussion at all levels of play, regardless of age or gender (Maher et al., 2014), however, female players tend to have a higher rate of concussion resulting from ball contact alone (O’Kane, 2016). Although the literature regarding head injury in youth football is relatively limited (O’Kane, 2016), findings such as decreasing head acceleration with increasing age (Kalichová & Lukášek, 2019) suggest younger players may be at a greater risk of subconcussive burden than older players.

The increased risk of concussive injury among youth and female populations represents a significant line of enquiry for biomechanical research (O’Kane et al., 2014). Several studies aim to better understand the variables which predict greater head accelerations during impacts, and among the most frequently postulated of these variables are the size and strength of the head- neck complex (Caccese & Kaminski, 2016).

2.3.4 Anthropometrics & neck strength

Previous studies have suggested greater neck strength predicts lower head accelerations during purposeful heading (Bretzin, Mansell, Tierney & McDevitt, 2017; Caccese et al., 2018; Gutierrez

29

A. Gilbert et al., 2014). Measures of neck strength have been found to vary widely across healthy young adults, however, the considerable advantage in neck strength in all directions that males have over females is well established (Catenaccio et al., 2017). Caccese et al. (2017) measured the isometric neck strength of 100 football players aged 12 to 24 who each carried out 12 controlled headers. Hand-held dynamometers were used to gauge the strength of the sternocleidomastoid and upper trapezius muscles (in kg) of each participant, and these measurements were added together to provide a mean value of isometric neck strength. Analysis of the resulting head impact data found that 13.3% of the variance in peak linear head acceleration, and 17.2% of the variance in peak rotational acceleration, was explained by differences in neck strength. Similarly, Bretzin et al.’s (2017) study of controlled heading found that lower neck strength was significantly related to greater head impact kinematics, noting the particular relationship between lower neck flexor and extensor strength and greater head accelerations in female participants. Neck strengthening has been suggested as a potential protective intervention for players exhibiting low neck strength, with the hope that increased strength will aid in mitigating injurious head accelerations (Gutierrez et al., 2014).

Following an 8-week cervical resistance training program, Mansell et al. (2005) assessed the effect of increased isometric neck strength in 36 collegiate-level football players on head acceleration incurred from an external force applicator. Despite a mean 15% increase in neck flexor strength, including a 22.5% increase in neck extensor strength of females in particular, increased neck strength exhibited no significant effect on resultant angular head accelerations, suggesting neck-strengthening regimes alone are not sufficient to mitigate head accelerations. Dezman et al. (2013) similarly observe no evidence of either neck flexor or extensor strength significantly reducing accelerations from heading a football. Interestingly, this study did discover a relationship between symmetrical neck strength and reduced head acceleration, with the average difference in flexor and extensor strength predicting greater angular and linear head accelerations. The protective benefit of symmetrical neck strength offers a potential explanation to Mansell et al.’s (2005) findings, as the increased flexion and extension strength described in Mansell et al. (2005) were not accompanied by matched increases in strength in the opposite direction.

Anthropometric measures of head and neck size offer an alternative means of assessing a player’s potential ability to withstand forces incurred during heading, with factors such as neck girth and head-neck segment mass predicting the magnitude of head accelerations (Bretzin et al., 2017; Caccese et al., 2017). These findings engender the notion that potential high-risk populations, such as junior/youth players, should undergo anthropometric screening before heading a football is allowed (Caccese & Kaminski, 2017). Others suggest there is only a modest correlation between

30

A. Gilbert head/neck anthropometrics and measures of neck strength, questioning the suitability of anthropometric screening (Catenaccio et al., 2017). While there is evidence that both neck strength and head-neck segment anthropometrics may have protective utility when it comes to minimising head accelerations during heading, neck strength presents a factor that is at least modifiable, unlike that of head and neck size (Caccese et al., 2017).

2.3.5 Heading technique

Despite some positive indications, there remains a paucity of evidence to suggest the efficacy of neck strengthening regimes for mitigating injurious head accelerations (Bailes et al., 2013). Several studies focus instead on the stability of the head-neck segment during impact, observing the level of muscle activation and its contribution to this stability (Caccese, Lamond, Buckley & Kaminski, 2016). Neck muscle activation is an integral aspect of intentional heading in football, with increased muscle activity being observed before, during and after contact across standing and jumping headers, often measured by sternocleidomastoid and trapezius muscle activity (Bauer et al., 2001).

Shewchenko et al. (2005b) present a biofidelic 50th percentile human model of heading, validated by kinematic data obtained in a controlled laboratory condition with active football players. The numerical model is used to investigate the effect that neck muscle activity and changes in heading technique have on kinematic head response. Increased neck muscle activity—characterised by “pre-tensing” prior to impact—resulted in modest reductions (up to 7%) in linear acceleration at the head’s centre of gravity, with the authors attributing this reduction to greater coupling between the mass of the head and its supporting structure. However, the same increase in muscle activity also saw a significant increase in the head’s angular acceleration (up to 48%), suggesting the benefits of increased muscle activation are far outweighed by the consequences. The stiffer head-neck complex achieved by increased muscular tensing may see the head rebounding over a shorter time period, resulting in greater shear stress to the head’s centre of mass (Shewchenko et al., 2005b).

Peak angular accelerations experienced by female high school football players were similar to those experienced by collegiate players in McCuen et al. (2015), despite the younger cohort experiencing significantly lower linear accelerations on average. The authors note that the relationship between age and linear acceleration is to be expected, as those playing at a higher level are likely to head balls kicked with greater force. Higher angular accelerations, on the other hand, are suggested to be the result of poorer technique and neck strength, resulting in high school players receiving more “glancing blows”.

31

A. Gilbert

Clark et al.’s (2017) study of eye discipline during heading suggest this aspect of heading technique may add to the increased risk of concussion faced by female players. Retrospective analysis of images obtained from Google found that, of the 139 male high school players identified, 79% had their eyes closed during a header, and 90% of females identified had their eyes closed (n=154). Defining eye discipline as “the ability to keep the eyes engaged in sporting activity with high risk” (p. 10), the authors insist that training players to keep their eyes open, especially those considered to be at a high risk of head injury, can reduce the incidence of concussion during contests for the ball resulting from head clashes and poorly executed headers.

Just as neck strength represents a variable of heading with the potential to be manipulated, heading technique has been identified as a potential avenue for mitigating head impact burden. Quintero et al. (2019) provide an example of effective intervention in the form of behavioural skills training (BST), exhibiting considerable heading improvement in a group of previously untrained girls aged 9 to 12. However, this study would have benefitted by incorporating metrics of head impact data in order to reveal what effect improved technique ultimately had on resultant head accelerations. While Caccese et al. (2018) observe the predictive value of anthropometric and neck strength measurements in relation to head acceleration, heading technique as assessed by motion capture analysis did not share this utility.

2.3.6 Protective utility of headgear

Numerous studies have investigated the protective utility of headgear for use during football play. Broglio et al. (2003) find headgear reduced the peak resultant force recorded by a force-plate during simulated head impacts. Naunheim et al. (2003) conduct a similar study, although this included more appropriate conditions such as a headform fitted with a triaxial accelerometer. Simulated heading at 3 ball speeds revealed no change in head acceleration between the control and headgear conditions. A combination of both human and headform trials also found no evidence of headgear mitigating kinematics during simulated heading (Withnall et al., 2005), although acceleration from simulated head-to-head contact was reduced by 33% when headgear was introduced. The authors suggest this difference is likely due to the stiffness of the components involved in the impact. During a clash of heads, headgear deforms at the site of contact and results in some dissipation of energy. However, the deformation of a ball during the act of heading is 10 times greater than that of the headgear’s nominal thickness (Withnall et al., 2005), so even if the headgear in question is soft enough to deform completely, the ball still represents the “sacrificial weak layer” of the collision and ultimately dictates the extent of energy dissipated. Therefore, with the inclusion of headgear, we would expect to see little-to-no

32

A. Gilbert difference in head acceleration during heading (also observed in finite element (FE) analysis of simulated heading (Taha & Hassan, 2016)), although there is some evidence of its protective utility during a clash of heads, especially in reducing the risk of laceration (O’Kane, 2016).

Delaney, Al-Kashmiri, Drummond, Correa and Delaney’s (2008) retrospective survey of adolescent football players reveals a considerable number of concussions reported during the 2006 season. Approximately 27% of the participants involved reported wearing headgear during that season, and subsequent analysis of survey results found that the two variables putting players at the highest risk of concussion were being female and not wearing headgear. However, this survey asked players to retrospectively report on concussion symptoms/incidence for the entire 2006 season, and self-report data spanning this length of time is liable to include considerable recall error (Lipton et al., 2018).

Not only is there a lack of evidence to support the protective value of headgear in football, findings have emerged that suggest wearing headgear may increase female players’ risk of experiencing injurious head accelerations. Tierney et al. (2008) observe increased head impact kinematics among female players wearing headgear compared to non-headgear controls. This unexpected increase in head response is perhaps attributed to a greater sense of safety afforded to players wearing protective headgear, resulting in more aggressive play and subsequently increased head kinematics (O’Kane, 2016; Tierney et al., 2008).

2.3.7 Ball composition

Elements of a football’s composition have also been discussed in terms of reducing head impact burden (Babbs, 2001; Kirkendall et al., 2001; Kirkendall & Garrett, 2001; O’Kane, 2016; Shewchenko et al., 2005c). With the mass and force of the ball playing significant roles in head impact interaction, important consideration must be given to the time period in which participants of retrospective studies played the game, as the leather ball used up until the mid- 1970s would absorb considerable amounts of water compared to modern synthetic balls, increasing by up to 20% in mass when wet (Kirkendall et al., 2001).

Shewchenko et al. (2005c) posit the alteration of ball characteristics as a potential protective intervention, using subject trials and numerical modelling to observe the effect of reducing ball mass and pressure on head and neck kinematics. A 35% decrease in ball mass resulted in an equivalent reduction in head response, with similar reductions to shear stress and neck axial response. Meanwhile, achieving these reductions in head response by manipulating ball pressure required pressure reductions of at least 50%. Current guidelines require junior players to use

33

A. Gilbert smaller-sized balls, and the importance of not overinflating these balls also seems apparent (O’Kane, 2016).

Babbs (2001) suggests reducing ball pressure would not only greatly improve player safety, but would also increase “playability” and “touch” of the football due to the increased contact time and area afforded by underinflation. Consideration is given to the negative response this notion would likely receive, with the author insisting we should argue against “hard ball macho.” Shewchenko et al. (2005c), on the other hand, suggest that any safety benefits achieved by adapting ball characteristics should be carefully weighed against reductions in playability. As their analysis found that pressure reductions below 25% resulted in very little change to head response (<5%), it seems highly unlikely that reducing ball pressure could be justified as a protective intervention in the adult game.

2.3.8 Impact location

The location of head impacts is another variable of sports-related brain injury known to predict concussive episodes (Belanger et al., 2016; Broglio et al., 2010). Breedlove et al.’s (2012) study of neurophysiological and neuropsychological change in a cohort of high school American football players revealed that participants who were clinically asymptomatic, yet exhibited underlying functional change (as measured by fMRI), had received a large number of high-magnitude impacts to the top-front portion of the head. The left and right middle frontal gyrus (MFG) is recognised as a brain region of interest, as strains to the MFG in the sagittal plane—such as those resulting from top-front impacts—are known to be particularly large (Breedlove et al., 2012). Players presenting with physiological change, regardless of concussion diagnosis over the two-season study period, also received significantly more impacts to the side of the head. The high frequency of impacts to the top-front and side of the head also correspond with strains to the basal ganglia and limbic circuits—anatomical regions involved in emotional processing—early damage to which is suggested to underlie the highly aggressive episodes of CTE patients in later life (McKee et al., 2009). Impacts not resulting in concussive injury can still include significant forces to structures of the deep midbrain and brainstem, implying subconcussive events may be a significant mechanism of injury independent of symptoms (Dashnaw et al., 2012).

Location of impact during heading was correlated to differences in head acceleration in Hanlon and Bir (2012). The study of female under-14 players observes increased linear accelerations during impacts to the side of the head, while increased angular accelerations were correlated to impacts on both the front and side of the head. As pointed out by Caccese and Kaminski (2016), however, only 47 head impacts were recorded across the 24 participants in this study, limiting

34

A. Gilbert the generalisability of these findings. Although relatively small populations are not uncommon in the subconcussion literature regarding heading, the exploratory findings of studies such as Hanlon and Bir (2012) may still provide useful insight into potential methods of reducing subconcussive burden (Caccese & Kaminski, 2016).

Conventional wisdom might suggest that rostral regions of the brain such as the prefrontal cortex are of particular concern regarding the subconcussive consequences of heading, as these impacts typically involve head-to-ball contact at or around the forehead. Indicating abnormalities in temporo-occipital white matter, however, Lipton et al. (2013) suggest that regions of interest in a football heading context might instead reflect the phenomenon of contrecoup injury, wherein damage is greater in regions opposite the site of impact.

2.3.9 Heading scenario

Harriss, Johnson, Walton and Dickey’s (2019) study of 36 female youth players recorded 434 purposeful headers and found that impacts to the top of the head—owing to improper technique—resulted in higher rotational velocities than impacts properly executed with the front of the head. However, heading scenario, rather than head impact location, was found to have a significant effect on linear head accelerations. Headers received on the top of the head from a goalkeeper’s punt resulted in significantly higher linear accelerations compared to when these heading scenarios were executed with the front of the head, perhaps putting further onus on correct heading technique. Caccese et al. (2016) found headers performed from punts and goal kicks resulted in the highest linear and rotational head accelerations in female collegiate players, likely due to the high velocity of the ball during these scenarios. While 19% of the 380 impacts observed arose from punts and goal kicks, these events yielded 27% of the cumulative head acceleration experienced, suggesting the potential protective benefit of eliminating these types of headers from the female collegiate game. Harriss et al. (2019), on the other hand, do not share the belief that reducing heading from goal kicks and punts would be appropriate for female youth players, as their data showed these events to account for only 12% of heading exposure. However, the authors do not provide a value of cumulative head acceleration across this cohort, and it would be of interest to note whether the total acceleration experienced from goal kicks and punts saw the same ~50% disproportion as Caccese et al. (2016).

35

A. Gilbert

2.3.10 Heading frequency

While the outcomes of subconcussive events rely on a complex interplay between several variables, including impact magnitude, location, head-neck-torso anthropometrics, etc., head impact frequency may reflect the greatest risk of subconcussive burden (Bailes et al., 2013; Dashnaw et al., 2012). Reynolds et al. (2017a) found that collegiate football players experience relatively low head accelerations on average compared to collegiate and high school American football and even collegiate lacrosse players. Despite experiencing low-magnitude head accelerations, collegiate football’s high rate of head impacts through purposeful heading resulted in a cumulative impact burden similar to that of high school American football (as measured by the sum of peak resultant linear (PRLA) and rotational (PRRA) accelerations per game/practice). Similarly, Lamond, Caccese, Buckley, Glutting and Kaminski (2018) observe heading frequency to be a greater factor of cumulative burden than impact magnitude in female collegiate football players. Although current heading guidelines aim to reduce the head impact burden for players up to an early high school age, female players at a collegiate level experience more than double the exposure to heading than those at a high school level (McCuen et al., 2015). At a collegiate level, the cumulative burden of head impacts in men’s football also relies heavily on impact frequency, with games providing the majority of this burden compared to the low frequency of heading in training (Reynolds et al., 2017b).

Although summing resultant head accelerations across a season provides an important look into the distribution of impact magnitudes, this method does not account for the magnitude of each isolated event. As the relationship between resultant head accelerations and the risk of concussive injury is non-linear, Davenport et al. (2016) endorse an alternative metric for head impact burden in the form of risk-weighted cumulative exposure (RWE). RWE results in a higher weight attributed to impacts of a higher magnitude, thereby addressing the interplay of frequency and magnitude that resultant acceleration-summing alone does not. Davenport et al. (2014) observe a strong correlation between RWE in high school American football players and changes in DTI measures. The relationship between RWE values and changes in white matter integrity indicates the utility of RWE in identifying potentially injurious head impact burdens in the absence of concussive symptoms. This method of quantifying head impacts is yet to be employed in the heading literature and could provide a useful interpretation of cumulative burdens incurred by different populations.

Methods such as accelerometer instrumentation and video analysis are frequently employed in the biomechanical subconcussion literature. However, these techniques are expensive, labour- intensive and difficult to utilise across large samples and longitudinal studies (Catenaccio et al.,

36

A. Gilbert

2016). With increasing interest in heading frequency, self-report methods have been endorsed as a valid alternative to these tools (Catenaccio et al., 2016; Lipton et al., 2018). Harriss, Walton and Dickey (2018) suggest recall bias renders self-report data inappropriate for quantifying heading exposure, with their cohort of female youth players overestimating actual exposure by 51%. Catenaccio et al. (2016) note a sex-difference related to self-reporting of heading exposure, with females performing worse than males. However, female reports still yielded moderate agreement with exposure measured by direct observation, while male reports showed excellent agreement, validating the HeadCount survey as an accurate measure of heading exposure over a 2-week period. Lipton et al. (2018) also demonstrate the HeadCount-2w survey as a valid self-report measure in adult amateur players, with a reduced sex-difference and excellent agreement with daily self-report data. Use of the HeadCount-2w survey has shown a correlation between heading exposure in the highest quartile and more severe CNS symptoms (Stewart et al., 2017), as well as poorer psychomotor speed (Levitch et al., 2019). Validated surveys such as HeadCount-2w may represent a potential avenue for identifying players who fall into the highest range of heading exposure and represent a high-risk population for subconcussive injury.

37

A. Gilbert

Uncertainty

The inherent complexity of any scenario gives rise to a certain degree of scientific uncertainty (van Asselt & Vos, 2006). This uncertainty can be produced by a variety of factors, including—but not limited to—differences in measurement techniques; extreme variability of the world outside of laboratory conditions; and sheer excess of information (Lemons, Shrader-Frechette & Cranor, 1997). Very rarely are decisions in life made under certainty; rather, decisions predominantly include a degree of uncertainty ranging from risk to ignorance (Resnik, 2003).

Keohane, Lane and Oppenheimer (2014) note that scientific uncertainty often represents one of two major forms: parametric uncertainty and structural uncertainty. Parametric uncertainty arises when the underlying processes of a phenomenon are well understood, but a significant margin of error is present when attempting to estimate outcomes. Structural uncertainty, on the other hand, denotes a phenomenon from which uncertainty arises because the underlying causal processes are not well understood. When the uncertainty in question is such that numerical probabilities cannot be specified, an issue of structural uncertainty might also be considered ambiguous (Keohane et al., 2014).

In order to distinguish probability from uncertainty, Wiener and Rogers (2002) use the example of being struck by lightning compared with brain tumours arising from cell phone exposure. While both scenarios may be considered low-probability risks, the latter could be considered far more uncertain, as we have insufficient knowledge of the cause and effect relationship between cell phone use and brain tumours to state whether that risk even exists. The issue of brain injury secondary to heading a football occupies similar ground. Not only is it currently difficult to label heading as a vehicle of subconcussive injury, but we are also faced with considerable uncertainty as to the relationship between brain injury and subconcussive exposure in the first place.

Scientific uncertainty frequently pervades issues of environmental and human health policy (Lemons et al., 1997). According to decision theorists, when we have insufficient evidence to estimate the probabilities of various outcomes, specific strategies for dealing with conditions of ignorance must be adopted over established probabilistic approaches such as the utility maximisation strategy (Resnik, 2003). As a result, alternative approaches to risk management such as the precautionary principle are often discussed in the context of environmental and public health interventions. Decisions such as these must always consider the dynamic nature of uncertainty. While an issue of scientific uncertainty may appear to have long-term implications, short-term strategies are often favourable due to the chance that this uncertainty may be rapidly resolved (Gollier & Triech, 2003).

38

A. Gilbert

As discussed in Chapter 2 of this thesis, the current status of research into the effects of subconcussion are highly uncertain. Nevertheless, increasing societal concerns regarding the neurological consequence of heading in football have prompted governing bodies of the sport in the United States and the United Kingdom to adopt precautionary measures to reduce heading exposure in junior players. The resulting guidelines for junior heading represent decision making under scientific uncertainty. The issue of subconcussion as it relates to heading in football could be considered somewhat ambiguous, as our limited understanding of this phenomena currently precludes any ability to estimate one’s probability of incurring neurodegenerative sequelae as a result of participating in football.

Fischhoff and Davis (2014) identify three broad categories into which decisions may fall: decisions about action thresholds, decisions with fixed options and decisions about potential options. The first of these categories is of particular interest here, as the introduction of junior heading guidelines in the US and UK are the first examples of football organisations providing an affirmative answer to the question: ‘is it time to act?’ These decisions are often triggered by emerging scientific evidence, although they are also invariably influenced by value judgments. Fischhoff and Davis (2014) highlight the tension between these aspects of risk management: “People will judge science’s value by the apparent wisdom of recommendations based on it” (p. 13665). Accordingly, fair judgment hinges on the understanding of both the uncertainty underlying the science and of the decision-rule that invokes action.

In the case of junior heading regulation, the appropriateness of current guidelines must be weighed in consideration of scientific uncertainty and precautionary action. However, decision makers and members of the public cannot disentangle the relative influence of uncertainty and precaution on their own, unless these aspects are conveyed effectively. For example, if a call for regulation is unnecessary, this could be the result of ambiguous evidence (from which mistakes could understandably arise), or an overly cautious decision-rule invoking unnecessary action, even if the science is strong (Fischhoff & Davis, 2014). With an understanding of one of these dimensions, individuals may be able to infer the nature of the other, but poor communication of the uncertainty surrounding science and value judgments will inevitably undermine this ability.

3.1 Communicating uncertainty

When the outcomes of a given activity are unknown, uncertainty is widely understood to be a key factor in the process of decision-making (Windschitl & Wells, 1996). Therefore, the way in which

39

A. Gilbert this uncertainty is communicated to stakeholders, decision makers and the public at large represents an important ethical consideration (Keohane et al., 2014).

The presentation of quantitative uncertainty (represented as a numerical probability) is shown to have a significant effect on lay-people’s decision making (Padilla et al., 2015). However, as previously discussed, not all issues of uncertainty yield reliable probability distributions. This dilemma of ambiguity poses additional challenges to the communication of uncertain science, as verbal representations of uncertainty—compared to numerical representations—can undergo greater manipulation in terms of context and framing (Windschitl & Wells, 1996). Communicating with loaded terms such as “likely” or “probable” also opens the key message to a variety of interpretations (Wallsten, Budescu, Rapoport, Zwick & Forsyth, 1986).

Research into the psychology of uncertainty indicates that people do not follow principles of probability theory when considering uncertain risks. Instead, intuitive heuristics drive estimations of risk that vary significantly between individuals (Slovic, 2000). Knowledge of scientific uncertainty can lead to decreased satisfaction when making decisions and may increase the burden of anxiety when facing decisions with “high stakes” (Politi, Clark, Ombao, Dizon & Elwyn, 2011). Moreover, there are concerns that disclosing scientific uncertainty may increase public confusion and concern, as people often desire assurances of safety or unsafety (Johnson & Slovic, 1995).

MacGregor, Slovic and Morgan (1994) illustrate the difficulties inherent to communicating situations made complex by scientific uncertainty. Their results show that even careful dissemination of uncertain risk information regarding electromagnetic field (EMF) exposure can increase societal concerns. On the other hand, Wiedemann and Schütz (2005), as well as Wiedemann, Thalmann, Grutsch and Schütz (2006), exhibit no effect on risk perception attributable to disclosing the uncertainty of EMF exposure evidence. At the same time, these studies show information regarding precautionary measures taken to reduce EMF exposure can increase perceived risk. The lack of an effect on risk perception resulting from an uncertainty factor is unexpected, as “it is this uncertainty that actually provides the reason for applying the precautionary principle” (Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005, p. 404).

Johnson and Slovic (1995) find that disclosing uncertainty can increase concerns regarding a potential hazard, although this effect was small and inconsistent in their study. Meanwhile, attitudes towards risk in general appeared to be a more reliable predictor of participants’ responses to stories involving uncertainty. A large portion of evidence suggests that any effect uncertainty has on risk perception is not uniform, and risk perceptions manipulated by uncertainty are heavily dependent on the specific hazard being discussed (Frewer et al., 2003). A

40

A. Gilbert finding that appears to hold for uncertainty in general is that decision-makers have an aversion to choices with imprecise parameters (Broomell & Kane, 2017). The comparative ignorance hypothesis provides an explanation for this trend, stating that individuals will experience uncertainty aversion if they are able to compare an uncertain event to an event exhibiting greater certainty. There is also a large body of evidence illustrating the effect that judgmental biases have on an individual’s risk perception (Kunreuther, 2002). For instance, it is well understood that when thinking about risks, people rely on certain heuristics which serve to simplify the process of decision-making (Sunstein, 2005).

Disclosing scientific uncertainty has also been shown to reduce risk perceptions (Pepper et al., 2019). Pepper et al.’s (2019) inclusion of uncertainty information regarding the short and long- term effects of electronic vaping products (EVPs) saw a decrease in the perceived risk of EVPs compared to those who did not receive uncertainty information. Although the reasons for uncertainty lowering risk perception are not well understood in this case, such an effect is nevertheless a significant observation, as risk perceptions are known to influence future behavioural intentions (Brewer et al., 2007).

A central problem to any scientific issue, but especially those of considerable complexity and uncertainty, is the need for decision-makers and the general public to understand that science can rarely produce absolute answers (Ghazoul & McAllister, 2003). Policy makers and the public at large tend to expect precise outcomes from scientific research, something largely influenced by the need to make decisions based on a corpus of uncertain knowledge (van Asselt & Vos, 2006). However, if scientists overstate the reliability of their findings, ensuing disputes and conflicting evidence may lead the public to lose trust in scientific advice. It is therefore imperative that scientists facilitate an understanding of the uncertainties inherent to any conclusions based on their research (Ghazoul & McAllister, 2003).

Scientific experts tend to believe that the general public struggles to conceptualise uncertainties related to risk management, and that disclosing uncertainties is likely to increase distrust in science and increase risk perceptions regarding uncertain hazards (Frewer et al., 2003). Regardless of these concerns, Keohane et al. (2014) maintain that the communication of uncertain information to decision-makers and the public should follow a principled approach. Among the five principles posited by Keohane et al. (2014) is specification of uncertainty about conclusions. Together with the principle of honesty, these facets of communication under uncertainty are considered non-negotiable, while the remaining principles of precision, audience relevance and process transparency allow some room for potential trade-offs, depending on the aim of the communication. Frewer et al. (2003) share the opinion that addressing uncertainties is an essential part of the risk management process. They argue, however, that the public is not

41

A. Gilbert only capable of understanding uncertainties, but people actively want to know about these aspects of the scientific process.

Frewer et al. (2003) argue that hesitation to share information regarding uncertainty reflects a misguided adherence to the deficit model of science communication, in that it may “reinforce the desired social distance between expert and nonexpert groups” (p. 83). However, there is evidence to suggest that individuals who do not initially grasp the concept of uncertainty are able to recognise its significance when it is presented simply (Johnson & Slovic, 1995).

Ultimately, the absence of any definitive guidance for communicating uncertainty is perhaps not surprising. Spiegelhalter, Pearson and Short (2011) suggest it is therefore important to gather information on communicative outcomes from a variety of contexts. There is currently no understanding of how the public interprets the uncertainty of subconcussion research, nor is there any empirical evidence as to the communicative outcomes relating to the uncertainty of heading regulation. Belanger et al. (2016) warn of the considerable implications for society when labelling subconcussive blows as a mechanism of “brain injury”. Caution in publicising this relationship appears to be justified, and consideration of the discussion above further supports the necessity of disclosing the underlying uncertainty of heading regulation to the public.

42

A. Gilbert

Precaution

4.1 The precautionary principle

Certain realms of scientific enquiry deal with considerable uncertainty regarding risk factors, providing a significant challenge to those in areas such as environmental and public health policy (Lemons et al., 1997). As is the case in both environmental and public health, scientific experiments may or may not confirm or disprove a given hypothesis, often leading to further research. Policymakers and regulators, on the other hand, must often reach decisions in light of the available evidence, regardless of the degree of certainty that such evidence may yield. This scenario is otherwise known as the “regulator’s dilemma” (Bodansky, 1991), wherein delaying regulatory action in the hope that new information will arise and ease some uncertainty is an interim decision in itself.

The precautionary principle is an alternative to traditional risk assessment which champions a “better safe than sorry” perspective (Bodansky, 1991). Where established risk assessment methods might forestall regulatory action in the wake of scientific uncertainty, the precautionary principle incentivises the adoption of proactive measures before vulnerable parties might incur harm (Raffensperger, Schettler & Myers, 2000). Early evidence of the precautionary principle is seen in the German concept of Vorsorgeprinzip—or ‘foresight principle’—relating to the government’s civic duty to prevent environmental harm (Harris & Holm, 2002; Resnik, 2003). The principle began emerging in European environmental policy during the 1970s (Foster, Vecchia & Repacholi, 2000; Sandin, Peterson, Hansson, Rudén & Juthe, 2002), before being formerly endorsed for the first time in 1987 by the second international North Sea conference (Bodansky, 1991).

The precautionary principle has since been invoked across a wide range of environmental and public health issues characterised by scientific uncertainty. Indeed, uncertainty is widely accepted to be the essence of the precautionary principle (van Asselt & Vos, 2006), as this decision-rule is employed as a means of addressing potential hazards for which we have insufficient data to provide a probability of outcomes (Bodansky, 1991). When a distribution of probability cannot be ascertained, the precautionary principle impels regulators to instead invoke a notion of plausibility (Resnik, 2003).

Environmental policy is a realm of considerable scientific uncertainty; therefore, it is not surprising that the precautionary principle originated, and left an indelible mark, in this area (Bodansky, 1991). Application of the principle has since expanded to include risks to public

43

A. Gilbert health, although examples of precautionary approaches to hazards far precede the adoption of the precautionary principle. Dr John Snow’s recommendation of removing the handle from the Broad Street water pump in 1854 is one such example (Foster et al., 2000). Although there was limited evidence that water contamination was correlated to London’s cholera epidemic at the time, Snow’s assertion was later proved to be correct, and the precautionary measures he suggested were of great benefit to public health (Harremoës, Gee, MacGarvin, Stirling & Keys, 2001).

There is currently no authoritative definition of the precautionary principle (Lemons et al., 1997), and differential applications of the principle across a variety of domains invites a wide range of interpretation. Sandin (1999) identifies 19 unique formulations of the precautionary principle existing before the turn of the millennium, and their analysis reveals several aspects shared by each one. In an attempt to operationalise the principle, Sandin (1999) organises four dimensions of the precautionary into a referential “if-clause” as follows:

“If there is (1) a threat, which is (2) uncertain, then (3) some kind of action (4) is mandatory” (p. 891).

Each of these four domains are found to vary across different formulations in terms of strength and precision. For example, the strength of a formulation (the degree of precaution advised) is shown to be influenced by the uncertainty (2) domain, as seen in the following statement from the 1997 Wingspread Conference:

“When an activity raises threats to the environment or human health, precautionary measures should be taken, even if some cause-and-effect relationships are not fully established scientifically.” (as cited in Sandin, 1999, p. 891).

The uncertainty dimension identified above—“some cause-and-effect relationships are not fully established scientifically”—renders this a relatively strong formulation of the precautionary principle, as it accepts very little in the way of uncertainty as a reason to preclude preventative action. The following statement from the 1992 Rio Declaration is an example of how strength and precision may differ between formulations:

44

A. Gilbert

“In order to protect the environment, the precautionary approach shall be widely applied by States according to their capabilities. Where there are threats of serious or irreversible damage, lack of full scientific certainty shall not be used as a reason for postponing cost-effective measures to prevent environmental degradation” (as cited in Sandin, 1999, p. 903).

This formulation is ultimately weaker than that of the Wingspread Conference, as it highlights a preference for cost-benefit analysis (Goldstein & Carruth, 2004). This aspect also makes the Rio Declaration’s formulation more precise, as the identification of cost-effective measures provides some inclination as to what actions regulators should take. Nevertheless, Sandin (1999) clarifies that every formulation of the precautionary principle includes a “strikingly imprecise” phrasing of the uncertainty dimension. Wiener and Rogers (2002) identify 3 types of precautionary principle formulation which differ by strength. However, the authors ultimately share Sandin’s (1999) observation that even the weakest formulation lacks the precision to sufficiently inform action.

4.1.1 Criticism of the precautionary principle

Use of the precautionary principle presents a number of contentious issues, not the least of which is the variability of its interpretation (Foster et al., 2000). Bodansky (1999) articulates this widely shared criticism by claiming the precautionary principle is too vague to inform regulatory action. Despite representing a general approach to uncertain risks, the precautionary principle fails to answer two integral questions: “When is it appropriate to apply the precautionary principle? And what types of precautionary actions are warranted and at what price?” (Bodansky, 1999, p. 5). While different formulations may (or may not) provide these answers within specific contexts, it remains difficult to define a single precautionary principle.

The strongest criticism of the precautionary principle pertains to its focus on potential negative consequences, with the replacement of traditional risk assessment viewed by some as an imprecise alternative akin to “pure pessimism” (Peterson, 2007). Harris and Holm (2002) view the precautionary principle as a “fundamental threat” to technological progress, as it requires an approach to science involving uncertain risks that is “ultra-conservative and irrationally cautious” (p. 357). Opponents not only believe the principle’s regulatory stance is overly risk- averse, but also consider it to be an unscientific method of decision-making (Resnik, 2003). While some critics of the precautionary principle view certain formulations as hidden attempts at trade-

45

A. Gilbert protectionism (Foster et al., 2002; Goldstein & Carruth, 2004; Raffensperger et al., 2000), others consider the broader implications of precaution, such as financial loss, foregone benefits and countervailing risks.

As discussed earlier, the presence of scientific uncertainty is what typifies the precautionary principle. This interplay between uncertainty and precaution can give rise to significant issues, including the apparent contradictory nature of certain formulations. Van Asselt and Vos (2006) label this the “uncertainty paradox”, wherein regulators are ultimately impelled to provide some degree of certainty that something is indeed a risk. The precautionary principle deals in terms of plausibility rather than the distinct concept of probability (Resnik, 2003). This is a point of necessity, as the uncertainty inherent to issues of precaution defies any possibility of calculating the probability that a given hazard might arise (Peterson, 2006).

The precautionary principle is often criticised for altering the traditional approach to burden of proof. Weak versions of the principle reduce the amount of evidence necessary to implement precautionary measures against a risk (Bodansky, 1999), whereas the strongest versions call to reverse the burden of proof entirely, requiring proponents of an activity to provide evidence that it is completely safe before undertaking it (Wiener & Rogers, 2002). The concept of providing “definitive” evidence of an activity’s safety could be viewed as a fallacy, however, as very few decisions (if any) are made under conditions of certainty (Resnik, 2003). At the very least, such a removal from regular burdens and standards of proof would likely prove too demanding to fulfil, in which case the risk of over-regulation becomes a worrying prospect (Wiener & Rogers, 2002).

Among the more salient criticisms of the precautionary principle is the potential for countervailing risks to take shape. Bodansky (1999), for instance, remarks that, “the precautionary principle seems to suggest that the choice is between risk and caution, but often the choice is between one risk and another” (p. 43). While precautionary measures may be implemented to remove or reduce a primary risk, this may directly or indirectly increase risk- taking behaviour of other kinds (Sandin et al., 2002). For example, banning certain substances may represent a direct increase in countervailing risks due to the subsequent adoption of less- well known alternatives. Bodansky (1999) illustrates the examples of DDT and chlorofluorocarbons (CFCs), both of which were at first thought to be environmentally benign. The banning of DDT resulted in farmers adopting more acutely toxic alternatives, while the environmental impact of refrigerants developed to replace CFCs were no better understood than were CFCs themselves at first (Bodansky, 1999).

Certain risks may indirectly increase countervailing risks in a variety of ways, including economic stresses, foregone benefits and stifled innovation (Sandin et al., 2002). For example, the United

46

A. Gilbert

States has strict regulations regarding drug development compared to other parts of the world. While this exhibits a precautionary approach to the uncertain risks inherent to introducing novel drugs to the public, this also reflects an acceptance of the risk that the benefits from yet- unavailable pharmaceuticals may be withheld from patients in need (Bodansky, 1991). Concerns of this nature extend to all areas of the regulatory space, with opponents of the precautionary principle stressing the impracticality of viewing any and all endeavours with suspicion. Goldstein and Carruth (2004, p. 498) point out that “it is a truism that the more precautionary is a society, the more likely it will have erroneously engaged in costly and unnecessary preventive actions.”

In addition to potential countervailing risks, adopting strong versions of the precautionary principle might also paralyse the development of new technologies (Foster et al., 2002). In this respect, Sandin et al. (2002) insist that it would be necessary to apply the precautionary principle to the measures that the principle itself prescribes. Sunstein (2005) also considers strong versions of the precautionary principle to be paralysing, as risks exist on all sides of uncertainty: “it condemns the very steps that it requires” (p. 14). In this sense, holding precautionary measures to the standards of the precautionary principle would be an insurmountable task. Wiener and Rogers (2002) argue that uncertain risks do not imply the need for action. Rather, the imminent risk of unforeseen, countervailing risks begs a measured assessment of the appropriate actions one might take. The incentive to respond to uncertain risk only then arises if a given response reflects a justifiable ratio of benefits and costs (Resnik, 2003).

Assumptions regarding threshold values for environmental and health risks are commonplace in situations lacking empirical evidence (Lemons et al., 1997). Sandin et al. (2002) posit that incorporation of the de minimis principle may remedy some formulations of the precautionary principle, as it provides a probability threshold below which regulators are not impelled to take action. For example, the de minimis principle is often employed in contexts such as radiation exposure, setting a level of acceptable risk below that which would cause a 10^-6 increase in one’s average annual probability of fatality (Shrader-Frechette, 1993). However, the uncertainty inherent to environmental and public health risks poses a quandary for risk assessors utilising a de minimis inference, as it assumes that cumulative exposure below a given level is acceptable: “From a public policy and ethical standpoint, such a claim is problematic because de minimis standards are not able to provide equal protection to all people due to individual differences in sensitivities” (Lemons et al., 1997, p. 223). Shrader-Frechette (1993) shares the sentiment that the de minimis principle’s emphasis on average risk levels does not account for the needs of more vulnerable individuals: “If risk assessors make the de minimis inference and claim that average exposure data or average levels of risk are harmless or acceptable, then they err” (p. 138).

47

A. Gilbert

4.1.2 In favour of the precautionary principle

Although a notable concern regarding the precautionary principle is the sheer variability in its interpretation, proponents of the principle assert that this is an issue that at least can—and should be—remedied (Sandin, 1999). Certain agreements appear to exist between the two sides of the precautionary debate, including the stance that the precautionary principle should only be applied when reliable quantitative information is lacking (Peterson, 2006). Another widely shared belief is that strong versions of the precautionary principle should be rejected (Harris & Holm, 2002; Hughes, 2006). Hughes (2006) insists that a large portion of criticism towards the precautionary principle focusses solely on stronger formulations that appear irredeemable. In this respect, interpretation of the principle remains paramount to the precautionary debate.

The “speed bump” formed by precaution is viewed by detractors as a significant hindrance within the regulatory setting. However, this feature also receives significant praise (Raffensperger et al., 2000). In the views of many, the precautionary principle should play a prominent role in public policy, as important decisions are frequently obscured by scientific uncertainty. While we might incur unforeseen risks in the process of prevention, failure to do so in an environmental and public health context may result in devastating consequences (Raffensperger et al., 2000; Resnik, 2003). Goldstein (2001) argues for the utility of the precautionary principle, noting that it shares many attributes of good public health practice, including an emphasis on primary prevention and recognition of the fact that unwanted consequences of human behaviour are not uncommon. While traditional risk assessment typically works within the bounds of secondary prevention, the precautionary principle allows for some possibility of preventing hazards before they arise (Goldstein & Carruth, 2004).

Harremoës et al. (2001) highlight numerous case studies of public health impacts that may have been averted had early considerations of potential harm been made, including UV radiation, CFCs and asbestos. These cases invariably exhibit some degree of ignorance, insofar as the relevant risk assessments of each hazard included some degree of certainty that undesirable outcomes remained outside the scope of their implementation. The notion of creating a hole in the ozone layer was altogether unanticipated when the CFC industry began – some 40 years prior to early warning signs in the 1970s. Yet it was not until 1987 that The Montreal Protocol was signed as a global appreciation for the dangers chemicals such as CFCs pose to the ozone layer (Harremoës et al., 2001).

Goldstein (2001) addresses significant examples of public health interventions that ultimately resulted in negative consequences, further compounding the notion of countervailing risk. Goldstein (2001) argues, however, that a precautionary approach might have avoided or at least

48

A. Gilbert mitigated these outcomes, stressing the importance of a multidisciplinary, multimodal approach to potential hazards. Goldstein (2001) goes on to concede that sufficient risk-benefit analyses may not justify the prevention of certain activities, even if knowledge of the full array of outcomes were well understood. For example, the case of arsenic-contamination due to the drilling of wells for drinking-water in Bangladesh and West Bengal, India, was unanticipated, despite prior evidence that this was at least a plausible hazard. Nevertheless, running the risk of possible arsenic-contamination could be argued as a justifiable cost, considering the undoubted benefits of a cleaner water supply over the sewage-contaminated water that initially prompted drilling (Goldstein, 2001).

Detractors often label the precautionary principle “unscientific”, which is perhaps an undue evaluation. Although it may be true to consider the principle unscientific in the weak sense (as decisions stemming from precaution are not ultimately based on quantitative measures), it does not hold to consider it unscientific in the strong sense (Sandin et al., 2002). We typically classify theories, methods or hypotheses as being either scientific or unscientific. However, the precautionary principle is none of these things; rather, it is a normative principle for making practical decisions under conditions of scientific uncertainty (Resnik, 2003). This consideration is true for any decision-rule, and something is only considered unscientific in a strong sense if it actively contradicts science (e.g., creationism vs. evolution) (Sandin et al., 2002). Resnik (2003) argues that: “Methods of practical decision-making can be regarded as scientific insofar as they are rational” (p. 330). Inversely, irrational methods of decision-making might also then be considered unscientific (e.g., reading tea leaves) (Resnik, 2003). Therefore, we might consider the precautionary principle rational (and scientific) if it instructs us to take reasonable precautions to deal with plausible threats—something that may be guided with reference to established epistemological norms (Resnik, 2003). A rational decision-maker applying the precautionary principal will (ideally) still use the same type of scientific evidence as any other rational decision- maker working under the confines of “full” scientific evidence; the former simply requires less evidence before taking action.

Peterson (2007) argues against the utility of the precautionary principle as a decision-rule, on the basis that it ultimately contradicts other, more fundamental principles of rationality (Peterson, 2006). Contrary to Resnik’s (2003) view that the precautionary principle be considered a practical principle—instructing us to make decisions based on empirical evidence (or a lack thereof)—Peterson (2007) asserts that the precautionary principle would instead best be considered an epistemic principle rather than a decision-rule—instructing us of what we ought to believe. This view appears to be justified in the sense that all formulations of the precautionary principle confront the trade-off between type I and type II errors (Wiener & Rogers, 2002).

49

A. Gilbert

In pure science, the occurrence of type I (false-positive) errors are largely considered to be more problematic than type II (false-negative) errors (Sandin et al., 2002), owing to the instrumental nature of scientific discoveries in informing further research, wherein a type I error will likely lead to many more errors in future (Peterson, 2007). In terms of environmental and public health risks, however, one could argue that the inverse is true, as incurring false-negative errors may have catastrophic consequences (Peterson, 2007; Sandin et al., 2002). Risking false-negative errors is akin to adopting an “innocent until proven guilty” stance, while risking false-positive errors is more in-line with a “guilty until proven innocent” approach to uncertain risks (Wiener & Rogers, 2002). This appears to be the main point of difference between traditional risk assessment and the precautionary principle: the former tries to determine how much harm we are willing to tolerate, whereas the latter asks how much harm we can avoid (Raffensperger et al., 2000). Proponents of the precautionary principle argue that regulatory decisions regarding environmental and public health issues should err on the side of caution when the potential to wrongly label an activity harmless exists. A common sentiment among proponents of the precautionary principle suggests incurring false-positive errors (to wrongly label an activity as harmful) tends only to result in financial loss—although this clearly does not account for a full picture of false-positive consequences (Wiener & Rogers, 2002).

The preference of one type of error over another represents a ubiquitous facet of the scientific process: a value judgment. Many critics fault the use of the precautionary principle as it relies on value judgments that are not necessarily grounded in scientific data. However, the notion of “scientific proof” is more value-laden than many might presume (Sandin et al., 2002). The decision to limit the chance of incurring false-positive errors represents the value which scientists ascribe to the correct discovery of relationships in the natural world. As Lemons et al. (1997) argue, value-laden inferences will only become more common to scientific methods that are pervaded by uncertainty, as decisions are being made in the absence of data. Even with data at hand, quantitative risk assessment still involves normative assumptions about what type of risk we should be concerned about, and how much risk we should be willing to avoid (Resnik, 2003). Peterson’s (2007) main criticism of the precautionary principle is that it expresses value judgments that few would accept when weighed against other, more important value judgments; however, the reliance on value judgments, Peterson stresses, should not in itself be criticised, as these lie at the core of any decision-rule.

50

A. Gilbert

4.2 Communicating precaution

Schütz, Wiedemann and Clauberg (2007) note two key questions that have largely gone unanswered in the precautionary literature: “do precautionary measures really deliver improved protection [?]” and, “do people feel safer when they know that precautionary measures are in place to protect their health?” (p. 612). The first of these questions appears difficult—if not impossible—to answer, as the success of a precautionary measure results in the absence of a hazard that may never have been present in the first place (Wiener & Rogers, 2002). The second question, on the other hand, appears to be answerable, although the literature that attempts to do so is limited.

Decision-makers typically employ precautionary measures as a means of protecting the public from potential dangers. In some cases, a precautionary approach may also reflect an attempt to reduce the public’s fears and anxieties regarding an uncertain hazard, yet, evidence from studies of risk perception suggest precautionary measures often fail to achieve the latter (Wiedemann et al., 2013).

Public debate surrounding the potential health risks of electromagnetic fields arising from cellular phones and base stations was the catalyst of widespread concern throughout the early 2000s (Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005). Despite the scientific uncertainty underpinning the notion of EMF health risks (owing to the inability of experts to completely exclude the possibility of adverse health effects), the precautionary principle was employed in many countries as a means of reducing public anxieties. Several studies have used this example as a means of investigating the effect that knowledge of precautionary measures has on risk perception, many of them observing a positive relationship (Barnett, Timotijevic, Shepherd & Senior, 2007; Barnett, Timotijevic, Vassallo & Shepherd, 2008; Nielsen et al., 2010; Schütz et al., 2007; Timotijevic & Barnett, 2006; Wiedemann et al., 2006; Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005).

Wiedemann and Schütz (2005) and Wiedemann et al. (2006) manipulate the inclusion of precautionary information within a document detailing EMF risk assessment. The authors observe an increase in participants’ perceived risk of EMF’s when examples of precautionary measures intended to reduce risk of exposure were included in the document. Wiedemann et al. (2013) note the same relationship between precautionary information and EMF risk perception, although they also observe considerable variation between participants from different nations. Further evidence suggests that—in the context of EMF risk, at least—learning about precautionary measures introduced to reassure the public often has the opposite effect (Barnett et al., 2007; Nielsen et al., 2010). Although these effects are often small, perhaps leading one to believe they have limited practical relevance, Wiedemann and Schütz (2005) note, “no matter

51

A. Gilbert how small the effects are, they are contrary to the expectations of policy makers who hope to calm public fears about EMFs by implementing precautionary measures” (p. 404).

These findings suggest that precautionary measures may act as a cue that an uncertain risk is, in fact, genuine, thereby increasing one’s perception of that risk (Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005; Wiedemann et al., 2013). This effect could also be strengthened if an individual has existing concerns regarding a given risk, wherein the implementation of precautionary measures acts to confirm one’s fears (Timotijevic & Barnett, 2006). The potential for precautionary action to raise “unfounded fears” based on limited evidence is often cited by opponents of the precautionary principle (Raffensperger et al., 2000). Evidence that an individual’s level of concern regarding an activity predicts behavioural intentions (Barnett et al., 2008) further highlights the implications of employing precautionary measures that increase risk perceptions.

Wiedemann et al. (2013) refer to the utilisation theory of Easterbrook as a potential explanation for this increase in perceived risk. According to this theory, emotional cues encountered in a piece of information tend to override any others, making these cues the dominant influence of cognition underlying behaviour (Easterbrook, 1959). In the context of EMF risk, Wiedemann et al. (2013) suggest that individuals may interpret precautionary measures with a “there is no smoke without fire” mentality, and the dominance of the emotion arising from this cue may amplify risk perception despite other cues insisting safety. Past research exhibits the effect that messages intended to arouse certain emotions have on risk perception, with reactions of fear demonstrating a notable increase (Lerner & Keltner, 2000).

An explanation presented to justify decreasing risk perceptions due to precaution comes in the form of the Trust, Confidence and Cooperation (TCC) model (Earle, 2010). According to this model, an individual’s level of “trust” relates to judgments of an agent’s morality, whereas their level of “confidence” is relevant to judgments of an agent’s behaviour. Siegrist, Cvetkovich and Roth (2000) describe social trust as, “the willingness to rely on those who have the responsibility for making decisions and taking actions related to the management of technology, the environment, medicine, or other realms of public health and safety” (p. 354). If an individual feels they share certain values with an agent, this judgment of morality can lead to an increased sense of social trust (Earle, 2010). Likewise, knowledge of an agent’s behaviour—including constraints on future activity—can act to increase an individual’s confidence in their own safety (Earle, 2010). An increase in trust and/or confidence in risk management may signal that a risk is being managed effectively, thereby reducing measures of risk perception (Wiedemann et al., 2013). Moreover, individuals exhibiting trust in decision-makers have been shown to perceive less risk and greater benefit regarding their actions than those who exhibit distrust (Siegrist et al., 2000).

52

A. Gilbert

Wiedemann et al. (2006) stress that the antithetical outcomes of communicating precaution should not prompt the abandonment of protective measures. Rather, “they may serve to warn risk managers that using precautionary measures merely as a means for reassuring the public will probably fail” (p. 370). There is currently no understanding of how communicating the precautionary measures adopted in the context of heading regulation affects one’s perceived risk of heading. While the junior heading guidelines discussed in Chapter 1 of this thesis were introduced to the US and UK as a means of reducing head injury risk, they might also be considered attempts at reducing societal concerns, especially considering the litigation from which the U.S. Soccer heading guidelines originated (Yang & Baugh, 2016). It is also important to note that questions persist as to the role that public risk perceptions should play in the adoption of precautionary measures. Opponents of the precautionary principle insist that accurate risk assessment should not rely on the public’s perception of risk, as perceived risk is far more susceptible to manipulation than assessed risk (Goldstein & Carruth, 2004). Furthermore, there are concerns that precautionary measures introduced to mitigate perceived risks will undermine established protocols grounded in science (Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005).

The World Health Organization’s background document, “Electromagnetic Fields and Public Health Cautionary Policies” (2000), notes that precautionary regulations should be adopted, but, “only under the condition that scientific assessments of risk and science-based exposure limits should not be undermined by the adoption of arbitrary cautionary approaches” (p. 4). Although current junior heading guidelines might be considered arbitrary in the sense that no established science-based exposure limit to heading exists, Wiedemann and Schütz (2005) add that the risk of spreading unnecessary concern should also factor into the decision to introduce precautionary policy. Referring to the Constitution of the World Health Organization (1948), wherein health is defined as, “a state of complete physical, mental and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” (p. 1), the potential for precautionary measures to increase concerns and impede well-being also deserves consideration.

53

A. Gilbert

Methodology

5.1 Research questions

The preceding chapters of this thesis have investigated the mixed findings and general uncertainty of subconcussion research, as well as theory relating to factors influencing risk perception involving uncertainty and precaution. This review of the literature highlighted several points of interest, including the heretofore-unexplored question of how those within New Zealand domestic football perceive the act of heading as a potential health risk, and how the communication of uncertainty and precaution influences this perception. Consideration of this literature leads to the formulation of the following research questions:

1. How does the disclosure of scientific uncertainty regarding subconcussion research influence one’s perceived risk of heading in football? 2. How does information of precautionary measures adopted by junior heading guidelines influence one’s perceived risk of heading in football?

5.2 Hypotheses

The literature concerning the communication of uncertainty and precaution, as discussed in Chapters 3 and 4, respectively, allow for the formulation of the following pairs of opposing hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1a (H1a): The disclosure of scientific uncertainty regarding subconcussion research will lead to either an increase or decrease in risk perception compared to the non-disclosure of scientific uncertainty.

Hypothesis 1b (H1b): The disclosure of scientific uncertainty will have no effect on risk perception.

Hypothesis 2a (H2a): Information about precautionary heading regulation will be viewed as a cue that heading poses a genuine health risk, and risk perceptions will be increased compared to that of the non- precaution message condition.

Hypothesis 2b (H2b): Information about precautionary heading regulation will increase trust/confidence in risk management, thereby reducing risk perceptions.

54

A. Gilbert

The remainder of this chapter provides a description of the methods employed to test the validity of these hypotheses.

5.3 Study design

This study followed a 4x2 factorial design, in which participants were randomly presented with 1 of 8 textual messages regarding subconcussion research during their completion of an online questionnaire. Table 5-1 below outlines the 8 message conditions and their manipulation of uncertainty and precautionary information, while Table 5-2 displays the textual elements included in each condition.

Table 5-1: Eight message conditions presented to participants

Control Control Control Control + + + + No precaution Junior ban Heading Heading regulation minimisation

Uncertainty Uncertainty Uncertainty Uncertainty + + + + No precaution Junior ban Heading Heading regulation minimisation

55

A. Gilbert Table 5-2: Separate elements of message conditions

Sport-related concussion is an area of growing interest, with the harmful effects of concussive head injuries receiving increased attention. However, many researchers now believe that sub-concussive events (those that do not result in concussion) may also contribute to a future decline in brain health.

Football (soccer) presents a unique scenario for sub-concussion research, as the act of intentionally using the head to make contact with the ball is not replicated in any other sport. Although it is possible to sustain a concussion from heading a football, the majority of headings do not result in concussion symptoms, and are therefore considered sub- concussive.

When a player heads a football, the force of impact initiates movement of the brain within the surrounding fluid of the skull. This movement can result in what is known as the “slosh effect”. While sub-concussive impacts do not result in any observable symptoms, it is possible that the rapid stretching of nerve cells during this “sloshing” can result in damage to the brain. Given the repetitive nature of heading, both in training and during games, damage incurred over the course of a career might be substantial.

To understand these effects, scientific studies have been conducted to investigate any changes to the brain following sub-concussive impacts:

• Brain imaging techniques have shown greater amounts of heading relate to greater evidence of brain injury, even in players with no history of concussion

• Blood samples following controlled sessions of heading have shown increased markers related to brain injury

• Contact-sport athletes, including footballers, perform worse on tests of memory, planning and attention following sub-concussive events than non-contact athletes

*Despite these studies, there remains significant uncertainty regarding sub-concussion, and further research is needed to fully understand the potential relationship between sub- concussive impacts and brain damage.

While the studies suggest that the brain may sustain damage during events such as heading, it is not yet known whether this damage is temporary or permanent. Furthermore, as this is a developing field, many of the techniques and tools used in sub-concussion research differ between disciplines, making it difficult to draw general conclusions. Indeed, the results of cognitive tests can be quite varied, and tend to suggest no relationship between sub- concussive impacts and a decline in brain health.

*In consideration of the developing brains of young players, and in light of evidence that lower neck size and strength relate to greater head acceleration during heading, the U.S. Soccer Federation adopted a precautionary approach by introducing a ban on all heading during games and training for players aged 10 and under in 2016.

56

A. Gilbert

*In 2016, considering the developmental state of junior players’ brains, the U.S. Soccer Federation adopted a precautionary approach by placing strict regulations on heading during games for players aged 10 and under. The new regulations state that any intentional act of playing the ball with the head is to be penalised with the opposition side being awarded an indirect free kick.

*The U.S. Soccer Federation has adopted a precautionary approach by limiting the exposure of heading for junior players aged 11 to 13. In consideration of the developing brains of young players, the Federation’s 2016 mandate limits heading to 30 minutes per week during practice, with each player not to exceed a total of 20 headers each week. Key: * Section added to uncertainty conditions * Section added to junior ban conditions * Section added to heading regulation conditions * Section added to heading minimisation conditions

5.4 Independent variables

This study investigated the effects of two independent variables on perceived risk of heading in football:

• uncertainty of scientific information • information regarding health-related precautionary measures

These variables were manipulated by creating two “uncertainty” conditions and four “precaution” conditions.

5.4.1 Uncertainty

The variable of scientific uncertainty was divided into two levels. Participants assigned to the “uncertainty” group were randomly presented with one of four messages which included a statement regarding the unsettled nature of subconcussion research. Participants assigned to the “control” group received one of the remaining four messages without this statement on scientific uncertainty.

The base message included in each condition was intended to provide participants with a brief overview of subconcussion research as it pertains to heading in football, including a description of the “slosh effect” (as discussed in Chapter 1) as a mechanism of brain damage, and references

57

A. Gilbert to specific findings that support the notion of a relationship between heading and neurodegeneration. The control message was constructed using as little ambiguous language as possible, with the aim of maintaining a significant difference between the control and uncertainty conditions. All participants were exposed to identical information as was presented in this base message, therefore, message manipulation was solely the result of the inclusion/exclusion of information regarding uncertainty and/or precaution.

The inclusion of the uncertainty statement immediately following the control message intends to draw the attention of participants within the uncertainty group towards the current lack of consensus regarding subconcussion. The uncertainty statement is juxtaposed with the control message information to highlight the genuine necessity for further research, as well as some of the contradictory findings that do not support a relationship between subconcussion and neurological damage. Wiedemann and Schütz (2005) found that their manipulation of uncertainty information had no significant effect on any of their dependent variables, including participants’ perceived risk of electromagnetic fields. While this lack of an effect was unexpected, the authors suggested that this might have been because their manipulation was “simply not strong enough.” (p. 404). Considering this assumption, the uncertainty information prepared for this study was made to be more extensive than the single sentence utilised in Wiedemann and Schütz (2005).

5.4.2 Precautionary measures

The variable of precautionary measures was divided into four conditions:

• No precaution (control) • Junior ban • Heading regulation • Heading minimisation

While the control message condition does not contain any supplementary information, the remaining three conditions include short examples of health-related precautionary measures adopted by the U.S. Soccer Federation’s junior heading guidelines. The “Junior ban” condition describes the nationwide ban on all heading for children aged 10 and under; the “Heading regulation” condition describes the penalising of heading during games; and the “Heading minimisation” condition describes the limited exposure to heading allowed for players aged 11 to 13.

58

A. Gilbert

It is important to note that the UK junior heading guidelines had not been adopted at the time of this study, therefore examples of heading regulation were limited to that of the US junior guidelines.

5.5 Participants

This study was designed for New Zealand residents aged 18 and over. Participants were required to be domestic football players (having played for a domestic club within the last 12 months), or the parent of a child (aged 18 or under) who plays for a domestic football club. Participants fitting both criteria were also eligible to take part in this study.

Due to their similar 4x2 factorial design, the results of Wiedemann and Schütz (2005) were used as a reference for determining an appropriate sample size for observing an effect on risk perception due to precautionary information (the effect size for precaution was chosen over that of uncertainty as the latter did not yield a statistically significant effect; Wiedemann and Schütz, 2005). Power analysis carried out using the GPower 3.1 statistical power software generated a minimum required sample size of 180 participants.

Participants were recruited for this study via a snowball sampling technique. Invitations to complete and share the online survey were initially sent to administrators of each of the seven New Zealand Football Federations, including: Northern Football Federation, Auckland Football Federation, WaiBOP Football, Central Football, Capital Football, Mainland Football and Football South. Email invitations with links to access the online survey were sent to domestic clubs across New Zealand, and these were shared on several Facebook pages used by clubs to communicate with their affiliated members.

5.6 Questionnaire design

Participants of this study completed a questionnaire hosted on the online survey programme Qualtrics. At the beginning of this questionnaire, participants were required to provide relevant demographic information, including their age, gender, parenthood and footballing background, followed by a 10-item a priori risk assessment in order to gauge general attitudes towards risk. Participants were then randomly presented with one of the eight message conditions outlined above. After reading this message, participants responded to several items assessing their perception of risk related to heading in football.

59

A. Gilbert

The following sections describe each aspect of the questionnaire in more detail. A transcript of the questions presented to participants can be found in the appendix of this thesis.

5.6.1 Demographic questions

Upon entering the survey, participants were presented with an information sheet and consent form regarding the questionnaire to follow. The title of the survey, “Attitudes Towards Football in NZ”, as well as the aim, “to assess the attitudes of those who play amateur football (soccer) in New Zealand toward several aspects of the current game”, were included in the introductory information sheet in order to hide the true aim and hypotheses of the project. Entering the survey with the knowledge that the actual values of interest relate specifically to risk perception of heading has the potential to result in response bias, as participants may feel motivated to answer questions in a manner they believe is desired by the researcher, regardless of their true beliefs (Podaskoff, MacKenzie, Lee & Podaskoff, 2003). Providing a cover story to separate the measurement of criterion and predictor variables reduces the likelihood of a participant adopting an acquiescence bias (yea/nay-saying), and helps to prevent the formation of inaccurate data relationships fostered by this response editing.

The opening question of the survey identifies participants as football players, parents of players, or a combination of the two. This question also serves as an initial screen for participant suitability, as selecting “None of the above” in response to this question navigates participants out of the survey. All remaining participants continue with the survey by stating their age, citizenship, gender, highest level of education, and ethnicity. Any respondent who entered an age below 18 was excluded from final analyses. The survey programme Qualtrics also screened for the geographical location of each respondent’s IP address, with an address appearing from outside of New Zealand resulting in that respondent being navigated out of the survey.

Participants who identified as a football player were then required to provide relevant information regarding their background in the sport, including the number of years played and typical playing position. These aimed to provide a measure of overall experience, as well as an indication of a participant’s likely exposure to heading (e.g., a goalkeeper is unlikely to header a ball as often as a defender, if at all). Self-reported exposure to heading was also prompted in the following two questions. Players were asked to estimate how frequently they carried out five different activities in training and during games. In both instances, heading was embedded within four other common football activities in order to maintain concealment of the survey’s aim.

60

A. Gilbert

For participants who identified as parents, a block of items regarding their child/children’s participation in football followed the general demographic questions. Child age and number of years playing football were recorded. Parents were then required to rate (to the best of their knowledge) their child’s attitudes towards five aspects of football on a scale from 1 = dislikes a great deal, to 7 = likes a great deal. Like the questions presented to players described above, heading was embedded within four other activities so as not to reveal the context of the study. Responses to these scales aimed to provide a measure of pre-existing reluctance towards heading. Parents that identified as having more than one child who plays football were asked to rate the attitudes of both their eldest and youngest children towards these activities.

A question on the importance of football to his or her personal identity aimed to provide a measure of each participant’s identification with the sport. This rating was measured on a scale from 1 = Not at all important, to 7 = Extremely important, and was asked of all participants, regardless of player/parent group.

5.6.2 a priori risk

A measure of participant’s general attitudes towards risk was obtained by asking participants to complete a 10-item a priori risk questionnaire. This measure was included as a means of controlling for participants’ general attitudes towards risk as a potential confounder to perceived risk of heading in football. Participants were required to state their level of agreement with 10 statements on a scale from 1 = Strongly disagree, to 5 = Strongly agree. Only 3 of these items were intended for analysis, and these were randomly embedded within 7 incongruous items as a means of concealing risk perception as one of the study’s true dependent variables.

The 10 statements included the following:

• I attempt to recycle my waste whenever possible • I find myself irritable if I have not had any caffeine • I tend to support local businesses even if it costs me more money • I spend more time watching television/movies than I do reading • I try to get at least 8 hours of sleep each night • I would consider myself more physically active than the average person • I am comfortable approaching new people • I would take a risk even if it meant I might get hurt* • I am at a high risk of injuring myself when playing sport*

61

A. Gilbert

• I would consider sunbathing without sunscreen to be risky*

*Three risk attitude items of interest.

Each of the 3 risk items highlighted above were taken from validated questionnaires concerning different aspects of risk attitude. The adapted items were employed as a means of assessing participants’ general risk-taking propensity; perceived susceptibility to injury in general; and general perception of risk as it pertains to health and safety. Responses to each of these 3 items were combined to form an aggregate general risk-attitude score for each participant, while responses to the remaining 7 items were disregarded.

The first of these items was taken from Zhang, Red, Ling, Patel and Sereno’s (2019) General Risk Propensity Scale (GRiPS), a validated self-report questionnaire that provides a measure of risk- taking attitudes in a general context rather than a domain-specific one. The item, “I would take a risk even if it meant I might get hurt”, was chosen from the shortened 8-item version of the GRiPS, as these items showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92). The chosen item also best fit the context of risk-taking related to physical injury.

The item, “I am at a high risk of injuring myself when playing sport”, was adapted from Stephan, Deroche, Brewer, Caudroit and Le Scanff’s (2009) study on perceived susceptibility to sport- related injury. An athlete’s perceived susceptibility to sporting injury plays an important role in their decision to take preventative measures in the face of future risk (Stephan et al., 2009). As this sense of vulnerability may correlate to one’s perceived risk of (and intention to avoid) heading in football, this item was included to provide a measure of participants’ general perception of susceptibly to sporting injury.

The final item of interest within the a priori risk scale, “I would consider sunbathing without sunscreen to be risky”, was adapted from Blais and Weber’s (2006) revised Domain-Specific Risk- Taking (DOSPERT) scale. This scale was developed in order to measure attitudes regarding risk- taking and risk perception across five distinct domains: Ethical, Financial, Health/Safety, Recreational and Social. For the measurement of risk perception using the DOSPERT scale, participants were required to assess the level of risk that certain situations and behaviours would pose to their health/safety. For this project, item #23 of the DOSPERT scale was adapted from “Sunbathing without sunscreen” to “I would consider sunbathing without sunscreen to be risky” in order to prompt an assessment of risk perception, as well as to fit the established format and scale of the remaining a priori risk items.

62

A. Gilbert

5.6.3 Perceived risk of heading

After completing the a priori risk scale, participants were randomly presented with one of the 8 messages outlined earlier. Participants were instructed to carefully read their allocated message and were only able to navigate to the next section of the survey after approximately one minute.

Upon reading the message regarding subconcussion and heading in football, participants answered a set of four items adapted from the Perceived Susceptibility of Sports Injury scale (PSSI), a validated scale of sport-related risk perception taken from Gnacinski, Arvinen-Barrow, Brewer and Meyer’s (2017). The average scores taken across the four-item scales outlined in Tables 5-3 and 5-4 below provide a measure of perceived risk of heading-related injury. This scale was selected for its proven factorial validity and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87), as well as its applicability to the context of heading in football.

Table 5-3: Perceived Susceptibility to Sport Injury (PSSI) scale Item Original wording Original Scoring no. 1. What do you believe is the chance that you will 1 = very low chance get an injury during your sport season? 5 = very high chance

2. How susceptible do you feel you are to get an 1 = not at all susceptible injury during your sport season? 5 = very susceptible

3. What do you believe is the chance that you will 1 = less than a 10% chance get an injury during your sport season in terms 5 = 100% chance of percentages?

4. What do you believe your chances are of getting 1 = a lot lower an injury during your sport season compared 5 = a lot higher with other players in your league?

63

A. Gilbert

Table 5-4: Adaptation of PSSI scale items Item Adapted wording for players Adapted scoring no. 1. What do you believe is the chance that heading 1 = very low chance a football will damage your brain during your 5 = very high chance sporting career?

2. How susceptible do you feel you are to heading- 1 = not at all susceptible related brain damage during your sporting 5 = very susceptible career?

3. What do you believe is the chance that heading 1 = less than 20% chance a football will damage your brain during your 2 = 20% to 40% chance sporting career in terms of percentages? 3 = 40% to 60% chance 4 = 60% to 80% chance 5 = more than 80% chance

4. What do you believe your chances are of getting 1 = a lot lower an injury during your sport season compared 5 = a lot higher with other players in your league?

Item Adapted wording for parents Adapted scoring no. 1. What do you believe is the chance that heading 1 = very low chance a football will damage your child's brain while 5 = very high chance they play the sport?

2. How susceptible do you feel your child is to 1 = not at all susceptible heading-related brain damage while playing 5 = very susceptible football?

3. What do you believe is the chance that heading 1 = less than 20% chance a football will damage your child's brain while 2 = 20% to 40% chance they play the sport in terms of percentages? 3 = 40% to 60% chance 4 = 60% to 80% chance 5 = more than 80% chance

4. What do you believe your child's chances are of 1 = a lot lower damaging their brain from heading a football 5 = a lot higher compared with other players in their league?

64

A. Gilbert

The PSSI items were adapted into two sets, one of which was worded specifically for players, while the other was worded specifically for parents. While players answered these questions in terms of their perceived personal risk associated with heading a football, parents responded to questions regarding their perceived risk of heading to the neurological health of their children. Those identifying as both a player and a parent were presented with both sets of questions, one after the other.

In both sets of adapted questions, items 1, 2 and 4 retained the 1 to 5 scales used for the original PSSI items. The percentage scale used for item 3, however, was changed for both the player and parent cohorts. The original PSSI scale measured answers to the question, “What do you believe is the chance that you will get an injury during your sport season in terms of percentages?”, on a scale from 1 = less than a 10% chance, to 5 = 100% chance. It was noted that the intervals presented in this scale are not equal, nor is the upper limit of “100% chance” an appropriate value considering the lower limit of “less than a 10% chance”. As a result, this scale was amended to include equal intervals of 20%, terminating at an upper limit of “more than 80% chance”. This made item 3 easier for participants to understand and ensured that data obtained from the adapted scales was interval and fit for analysis.

In order to fit the specific context of head injury in football—and injury incurred by heading in particular—the original wording of the PSSI items were changed from the style of “What do you believe is the chance that you will get an injury during your sport season?” to that of “What do you believe is the chance that heading a football will damage your brain during your sporting career?” To incorporate the notion of subconcussive damage as a long-term health risk, the hypothetical timeframe of these questions was changed from a single “sport season” to “sporting career” for players, and to “while they play [football]” for parents.

An initial adaptation of the PSSI items included wording that followed the style of: “What do you believe is the chance that you will develop a head injury from heading a football during your sporting career?” Following a test survey with this format of the PSSI scale, it became clear that the wording of items needed to be less ambiguous regarding the hypothetical mechanism of proposed head injury. Feedback from test respondents indicated that these questions could be perceived as regarding any head injury sustained in the act of heading a football. One respondent cited head-to-head and elbow-to-head impacts as factoring into his consideration of the PSSI items: “I know that, seven out of ten times, I touch the other player or they touch me around the head when competing for a header”. With the aim of specifically measuring participants’ perceived risk of head-to-ball impacts as a mechanism of subconcussive injury, items were amended by replacing references to “head injury” (which were shown to evoke the notion of acute injury/concussion) with references reflecting a more chronic process of “brain damage”. In order

65

A. Gilbert to reflect the notion of subconcussive brain damage due to heading, as discussed in the preceding experimental message, items such as “What do you believe is the chance that you will develop a head injury from heading a football during your sporting career?” were changed to “What do you believe is the chance that heading a football will damage your brain during your sporting career?”

5.6.4 Perceptions of others

For participants in the “players” cohort, a set of three items aimed to measure the perceived social consequences of expressing concern for heading in football. These items were adapted from a subscale of Rex and Metzler’s (2016) Sport Injury Anxiety Scale (SIAS) and reworded to fit the specific context of heading. As a subscale of the SIAS, this set of items was originally constructed to measure “anxiety related to others’ perceptions of me (relabelled as anxiety related to being perceived as weak [BPW])” as an independent factor of sports injury anxiety. This subscale was selected as a means of assessing this social dynamic as a potential factor of participants’ reported risk perception of heading. Due to the high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.90) and hypothetical sports injury context of the SIAS subscale, the items outlined in Table 5-5 below were deemed suitable for the means of this survey.

Table 5-5: Adaptation of Sport Injury Anxiety Scale (SIAS) – Anxiety related to BPW subscale Item Original wording no. 1. When I am injured, some people think I am just being a baby.

2. When I am injured, some people think I am just being lazy.

3. When I am injured, some people think I am faking it.

Item Adapted wording no. 1. If I expressed any concern for heading in football, my teammates would think I was just being weak.

2. If I expressed any concern for heading in football, my teammates would think I was just being lazy.

66

A. Gilbert

3. If I expressed any concern for heading a football, my teammates would think I was faking it.

Prior to reading each item, participants were asked to select the extent to which they agreed or disagreed with each statement on a scale from 1 to 7 (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). An average rating across the three items was compiled for each participant and represented a value of anxiety related to the perceptions of others.

5.6.5 Support for heading regulation

Parents of junior players were asked the following question: “On a scale from 1 to 5, to what extent would you prefer that your child played in a league that regulated heading over one that did not, if this were an option?”, with 1 corresponding to “Not at all”, and 5 corresponding to “Definitely yes”. This question was included to observe any preference that parents within this cohort might have for junior heading regulation in New Zealand. It is understood that no such data is currently available, at least not within the context of parents involved in New Zealand football. It is therefore of interest to observe the proportion of parents who would/would not support the implementation of heading regulation—akin to that of the US and UK heading guidelines—as far as their own children are concerned.

5.6.6 Attention check and debrief

In order to observe whether participants did or did not retain information from their respective message conditions, the following message was presented at the completion of the survey:

“Earlier in the survey you read an article about some health problems that might arise from heading a football. Do you remember how the article described the movement of the brain within the skull? If you do, click the answer below that describes what you remember. If you do not, click "I don't remember.”

Please be honest! There is no penalty for failing to remember, but we need to know in order to use your data.”

67

A. Gilbert

Participants were instructed to select from “Flushing”, “Sloshing”, “Lashing” and “I don’t remember”, with any answer beside “Sloshing” being considered a failed attention check. This result was recorded in order to observe any difference in survey responses attributable to success or failure, with the notion that a failed attention check represented inadequate processing of the subconcussion message presented earlier in the survey.

A debriefing message formally concluded the survey, notifying participants of the study’s true aim. A request that participants did not discuss the true nature of the study with prospective respondents was included in this message.

68

A. Gilbert

Results

6.1 Quantitative analysis

The descriptive statistics and quantitative analysis discussed in this chapter were generated using Microsoft Excel and IBM’s SPSS Statistics 25.0 software.

This study initially consisted of 116 participants. However, following exclusion of those who failed the survey’s post-questionnaire attention check, the final cohort incorporated into the General Linear Model discussed below consisted of 89 participants in total.

6.2 Risk perception analysis

Female Male 15%

85%

Figure 6-1: Age and sex of participants, n= 89.

Table 6-1: Distribution of participants across 8 experimental message groups

Factor Condition N Uncertainty Control (No Uncertainty) 46 Uncertainty 43 Control (No Precaution) 24 Precaution Junior ban 21 Heading regulation 20 Heading minimisation 24 Total 89

69

A. Gilbert

6.3 Risk perception scores

As discussed in Chapter 5, each participant answered a set of four questions following exposure to a textual message regarding subconcussion research. Each question was recorded on a 5-point scale, and the sum of these four responses was collated to provide a risk perception score (RPS). These scores were recorded for each participant and included as the dependent variable of a Two- Way ANOVA comparing mean RPS across the 8 factorial groups arising from the 2 Uncertainty and 4 Precaution conditions. Table 6-2 below outlines mean risk perception scores by factorial group.

Table 6-2: Mean risk perception scores (RPS) for each experimental message condition Uncertainty Precaution Mean RPS Control 10.62 Control Junior ban 8.00 Heading regulation 7.46 Heading minimisation 9.46 Control 11.27 Uncertainty Junior ban 12.80 Heading regulation 9.78 Heading minimisation 8.54

The resulting data suggest risk perception scores were slightly elevated under Uncertainty compared to the Control condition. Two-Way ANOVA yielded a statistically significant main effect of Uncertainty on mean RPS, F(1,81) = 6.5, p = .013 (medium effect size, η2 = 0.075).

The Precaution factor similarly appeared to influence participants’ risk perception scores, with RPS seemingly reduced under conditions including precautionary information compared to control conditions. The ANOVA also generated a statistically significant main effect of Precaution on mean RPS, F(3,81) = 2.8, p = .048 (medium effect size, η2 = 0.092).

However, it is important to note that these main effects are qualified by a statistically significant interaction effect between the factors of Uncertainty and Precaution, F(3,81) = 3.3, p = .023. This interaction exhibits a medium-large effect size (η2 = 0.110).

70

A. Gilbert

***

Figure 6-2: Bar graph representing mean risk perception scores across 8 factorial groups. The x-axis represents the 4 Precaution message conditions and the difference between these conditions depending on the inclusion/exclusion of Uncertainty information. ***= p value≤.001 following pairwise comparison of simple main effects.

Visual interpretation of Figure 6-2 above supports the notion that there is a significant interaction effect between the two factors of Uncertainty and Precaution. As a result, this data suggests that an individual’s perceived risk of heading in football depends on the combination of uncertainty and precautionary information.

Because a statistically significant interaction effect was observed between the two experimental factors, the main effects of Uncertainty and Precaution as noted above may be misleading. Rather, analysis of the simple main effects of these factors should be considered in order to interpret the nature of their interaction.

Although Two-Way ANOVA illustrates a statistically significant interaction effect between the Uncertainty and Precaution factors, this result does not provide information as to where this interaction occurs. In order to better understand this interaction effect, this ANOVA was followed by a pairwise comparison of simple main effects, the aim of which is to describe the effect of one independent variable at a given level of another.

71

A. Gilbert

6.3.1 Simple main effects of Uncertainty

Pairwise comparisons adjusted for Least Significant Difference (LSD) indicate that subjects in the “Junior ban” Precaution group who were also presented with Uncertainty information recorded risk perception scores 4.80 points higher than those in the Junior ban group who did not receive Uncertainty information (p = .001, 95% CI of the difference = 2.06 to 7.54). None of the remaining Precaution groups exhibited a statistically significant simple main effect of Uncertainty on RPS.

6.3.2 Simple main effects of Precaution

Among participants who received Uncertainty information, those in the Junior ban group recorded risk perception scores 3.02 points higher than those in the Heading regulation group (p = .040, 95% CI of the difference = .14 to 5.90) and 4.26 points higher than those in the Heading minimisation group (p = .002, 95% CI of the difference = 1.63 to 6.90). Those who were not presented with Precaution information (Control) also recorded higher mean RPS than those in the Heading minimisation group when Uncertainty was present (p = .037).

Of those who were not presented with Uncertainty information (Control); participants who were also under the Control condition for Precaution (i.e., Control + Control) recorded higher RPS than those in the Junior ban group (Mean difference = 2.62, p = .046, 95% CI of the difference .05 to 5.18) and those in the Heading regulation group (Mean difference = 3.16, p = .016, 95% CI of the difference .59 to 5.73)).

Separate ANOVAs were conducted between the 8 factorial groups and other scale variables including age, a-priori risk perception, and perceptions of others. None of these tests resulted in any statistically significant effects, supporting the above assertion that differences in mean risk perception scores between these groups was the result of experimental manipulation and not random effects.

72

A. Gilbert

6.4 Participant characteristics

The following results concern additional data obtained from participants in order to understand their footballing background, including general attitudes towards heading and the notion of junior heading regulation. Barring Figure 6-3 below, the remaining figures presented in this chapter include participants who otherwise failed the post-message attention check and were excluded from the ANOVA described above. This data is not contingent on experimental manipulation and therefore includes responses from 116 participants.

Figure 6-3: Participants were asked to provide a rating for the following question: “On a scale from 1 to 7, how important is your involvement in football to your personal identity?” n= 89. (Note: 0 participants selected “3” in response to this question, hence it does not appear on this graph).

As might be expected, the majority of participants responded to this question with 5, 6 or 7, illustrating moderate-to-strong agreement with the idea that involvement in football plays a prominent role in these participants’ lives. Almost half (49%) of the participants included in final analyses considered football “Extremely important” to their personal identity, while only 7% responded with 1, 2 or 3—illustrating a moderate-to-strong disinclination to consider football of much personal importance.

73

A. Gilbert

Figure 6-4: Players’ estimations of how frequently drills involving heading are carried out during training, n= 87.

Figure 6-4 above illustrates that the frequency of heading drills during training is not consistent across players within this study. The most frequently selected option on the scale provided was 4, with 26% of players reporting that drills involving heading occur in “About half” of all training sessions.

Figure 6-5: Players’ estimated number of headers carried out per game, n= 87.

Reported frequency of heading during games suggests players are more likely to carry out a low number of headers per game than they are a high number. Most players estimated their range of heading frequency between 0 and 5 (39%) or 6 and 10 (31%) headers per game. Figure 6-5 above shows that relatively few players estimate a high number of headers per game—only 5% of players in this study report that an average game may involve carrying out 21 or more headers.

74

A. Gilbert

Figure 6-6: Parents’ estimation of their child’s attitude towards heading in football, n= 50.

Parents were asked to estimate their own child’s attitude towards 5 aspects of playing football, including their attitude towards heading. For parents with multiple children under the age of 18, Figure 6-6 above includes estimates of their youngest child’s attitude. Nearly half of parents (48%) responded with either 5, 6 or 7, indicating some degree of enjoyment that their child ascribes to the act of heading. Twenty-eight percent of parents, on the other hand, responded to this question with 1, 2 or 3, suggesting they have reason to believe their child dislikes heading in football. Almost as many parents (24%) indicate that their child harbours no strong feeling in either direction regarding heading.

Figure 6-7: Parents of junior players (n= 50) were asked, “On a scale from 1 to 5, to what extent would you prefer that your child played in a league that regulated heading over one that did not, if this were an option?”

75

A. Gilbert

Figure 6-7 above illustrates the proportion of parents who would/would not prefer the option of heading regulation in New Zealand football. Fifty percent of parents responded with either 1 or 2 on the 5-point scale provided, indicating that half of this cohort are against the introduction of heading regulation. Indeed, the majority of those showing this disinclination were strongly against the idea, with 38% of all parents reporting they would “Not at all” prefer that their own child played in a league that regulated heading exposure.

Meanwhile, 38% of parents responded with either 4 or 5, indicating preference for the option of junior heading regulation. Nearly a quarter (24%) of parents indicated that they would “Definitely” prefer that their child played in a league which regulated their exposure to heading. Although parents in this cohort tend to prefer that their child does not play in a setting that regulates heading exposure, a significant proportion appear open to, if not highly interested in, the concept of heading regulation in New Zealand.

76

A. Gilbert

Discussion

FIFA’s most recent “Big Count” survey estimated a total of 265 million global players in 2006, a 10% increase from the same survey 6 years earlier (FIFA Communications Division, 2007). If such a trend has maintained to this day, the number of individuals now playing football would be well over 300 million—or approximately 4% of the world’s population. Also according to FIFA’s survey, the US Soccer Federation has more youth players registered (~3.9 million) than any other organisation, while the English FA has fewer registered youth players than only France, South Africa, Brazil and Germany (FIFA Communications Division, 2007). NZ Football’s 2017 figures include nearly 60,000 registered junior players (NZ Football, 2018), while Sport New Zealand’s 2018 report outlines 19% of young people (aged 5-17) having participated in football within a week of their survey (Sport New Zealand, 2019). This proportion is higher than that of any other sporting code, illustrating the considerable popularity of football among junior/youth players in New Zealand.

Current heading guidelines represent significant regulatory decisions, with the US and UK examples already applying to millions of junior players. While no such guidelines yet exist in New Zealand, their introduction would similarly have a far-reaching effect. To date, no empirical research has investigated the New Zealand public’s perceptions of heading in football as a potential health risk. This study provides initial findings in this area and highlights several considerations for communicating junior heading regulation.

The goal of implementing heading guidelines may be viewed as two-fold: (1) to protect junior players in light of evidence linking football participation and increased risk of concussion and neurodegenerative disease, and; (2) to reduce societal concerns surrounding head injury in football by introducing precautionary measures. The aim of this thesis is not to assess the suitability of heading guidelines as a means of achieving the first goal (1) above. Rather, the scope of this thesis entails communicative outcomes within this area of public health intervention, with a specific focus on the influence of uncertainty and precautionary information in achieving the second goal (2) of heading regulation.

Chapter 5 outlines the two pairs of competing hypotheses which this study aimed to test. The initial findings of statistically significant main effects of Uncertainty information (p<.05) and Precautionary information (p<.05) on risk perception scores are consistent with H1a, wherein uncertainty information would have some effect on perceived risk; and H2b, wherein precautionary information would result in reduced risk perceptions due to increased trust/confidence in risk management. However, the medium-large (η2 = 0.110) interaction effect

77

A. Gilbert

(p<.05) between Uncertainty and Precaution which qualified these main effects represents the main finding of this study and demands a more nuanced interpretation of these factors.

7.1 The uncertainty effect

The distribution of mean RPS across the 8 factorial groups in this study appears to support the notion that disclosing scientific uncertainty does have an effect on one’s perceived risk of heading in football, prompting rejection of the null hypothesis, H1b. Although mean RPS tended to increase under conditions of uncertainty, this was not the case for all 4 Precaution conditions.

The most notable simple main effect observed following pairwise comparisons relates to the disclosure of uncertainty and the acknowledgment of a junior heading ban. Of the participants who were presented with information on US Soccer’s ban on heading for junior players, those who also read about scientific uncertainty recorded significantly higher risk perception scores than those to whom uncertainty was not disclosed (p = .001). Meanwhile, the disclosure of scientific uncertainty did not have such a significant effect across the remaining message conditions addressing precaution.

The observation of an effect on risk perception attributable to scientific uncertainty contrasts with past studies of similar design. Wiedemann and Schütz (2005) and Wiedemann et al. (2006) report no effect on participant’s EMF risk perception due to uncertainty information. Pepper et al. (2019) does observe an uncertainty effect, although this effect resulted in decreased risk perceptions regarding the use of electronic vaping products. The authors suggest that this decrease may be the result of deeper message processing, or due to the expectation of a “scary” public health message (in the vein of anti-smoking campaigns) offset by the acknowledgment of the risk at hand being uncertain.

Increased risk perception in the case of those receiving “junior ban” information is consistent with past findings which suggest the disclosure of uncertainty can increase concerns regarding a potential risk (Johnson & Slovic, 1995; MacGregor et al., 1994). However, the uncertainty- dependent increase in risk perception observed in this study is inconsistent across all factorial conditions and appears to depend also on the nature of precautionary information being discussed.

78

A. Gilbert

7.2 The precautionary effect

The interaction between the two factors of this study is further highlighted by simple main effects observed due to precautionary information. When scientific uncertainty was disclosed, participants who received information on a junior heading ban recorded higher risk perceptions than those who received alternative precautionary information in the form of “heading regulation” (p=.040) and “heading minimisation” (p=.002). Likewise, the absence of precautionary information altogether resulted in increased risk perception compared to the condition including information on precautions aimed at minimising heading exposure (p=.037).

When scientific uncertainty was not disclosed, participants who also received no information regarding precautionary measures recorded higher risk perception scores than those exposed to information regarding a junior heading ban (p=.046) and the methods used to ensure heading does not occur during games (Heading regulation) (p=.016).

a).

b).

Figure 7-1: a): Wiedemann & Schütz (2005), b): Wiedemann et al. (2006). Both graphs represent differences in risk perception across conditions of precautionary information.

The findings relating to precautionary information’s effect on risk perception in this study are consistent with prior research insomuch that this effect does not appear to be very large. The two bar graphs presented in Figure 7-1 above illustrate the main effect of precautionary information on participants’ perceived risk of EMF exposure. Notably, both (a) (Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005) and (b) (Wiedemann et al., 2006) observe a general increase in risk perception among conditions outlining precautionary measures taken to protect against EMF exposure compared to a condition omitting such information. While this effect is noted as being small, the authors of both

79

A. Gilbert studies argue that these findings maintain practical relevance as the effect occurs in the opposite direction to what regulators might expect and/or desire.

This study’s general observation of decreased risk perception among participants exposed to precautionary information might suggest that knowledge of heading guidelines acts to mitigate one’s concerns regarding heading as a potential health risk. An explanation for this effect might follow the TCC model of risk management as described in Chapter 4, wherein learning of precautionary measures could increase one’s trust and/or confidence in regulatory decisions. However, the notion that precautionary information acts to temper concerns regarding heading in football is called into question by the significantly higher risk perceptions observed among participants in the “Uncertainty” + “Junior ban” factorial group. As a result, the findings presented here suggest that the ability of precautionary information to reduce risk perception depends on (1) the nature of precautionary measures employed and (2) the disclosure of scientific uncertainty.

7.3 Interaction of Uncertainty and Precaution

Contrary to the findings of past risk perception studies (Barnett et al., 2007; Wiedemann & Schütz, 2005), this thesis presents a statistically significant interaction effect between the factors of uncertainty and precautionary information (p<.05). The presence of an interaction effect within this cohort renders it difficult to speak in terms of uncertainty or precaution alone. Instead, it appears that one’s perceived risk of heading in football due to one of these factors also depends on the nature of the accompanying factor.

This interaction is most notable when comparing responses from those in the “Junior ban” and “Heading minimisation” groups. While there is little difference in risk perception due to uncertainty for those receiving Heading minimisation information, this difference is pronounced among those who instead received Junior ban information. Past findings suggest the presence of uncertainty might act to increase an individual’s existing concerns surrounding precautionary measures. As noted by Barnett et al. (2008), the uncertainty of a given risk represents a key rationale for adopting precautionary measures. In this sense, one might justifiably expect any appreciation for precautionary advice to increase in the presence of uncertainty. Despite this assumption, Barnett et al. (2008) observe the opposite effect in the context of possible health risks relating to mobile-phone use, with individuals being less likely to consider precaution “good governance” as perception of the underlying uncertainty increases. Similarly, Barnett et al. (2007) note that individuals who express the most concern regarding the uncertainty of mobile-phone

80

A. Gilbert health risk also show the greatest concern associated with relevant precautionary measures. The authors highlight the irony of this failure to reassure concerned individuals, noting that, “an important part of the justificatory discourse around the provision of precautionary advice is that it is in response to public concerns” (Barnett et al., 2007, p. 249).

Although the relationship between uncertainty and precaution might explain the contrasting responses within the Junior ban group, this does not account for the lack of an uncertainty effect within the heading minimisation group. A possible answer to the waning effect of uncertainty across each level of the Precaution factor might therefore relate to the strength of each protective measure being described.

Although each level of the Precaution factor (not including the Control condition) was derived from the same example of US heading guidelines, the framing of each one reflects regulatory decisions of varying strength. The extracts below highlight the strength (i.e., the degree of precaution expressed; see Sandin, 1999) by each level of Precaution in this study.

Junior ban phrasing:

In consideration of the developing brains of young players, and in light of evidence that lower neck size and strength relate to greater head acceleration during heading, the U.S. Soccer Federation adopted a precautionary approach by introducing a ban on all heading during games and training for players aged 10 and under in 2016.

Heading regulation phrasing:

In 2016, considering the developmental state of junior players’ brains, the U.S. Soccer Federation adopted a precautionary approach by placing strict regulations on heading during games for players aged 10 and under. The new regulations state that any intentional act of playing the ball with the head is to be penalised with the opposition side being awarded an indirect free kick.

Heading minimisation phrasing:

The U.S. Soccer Federation has adopted a precautionary approach by limiting the exposure of heading for junior players aged 11 to 13. In consideration of the developing brains of young players, the Federation’s 2016 mandate limits heading to 30 minutes per week during practice, with each player not to exceed a total of 20 headers each week.

81

A. Gilbert

As described in the first condition above, the ban on heading for players aged 10 and under could be interpreted as a stronger case of precaution than the remaining two levels. While the “Heading regulation” phrase refers to “strict regulations on heading during games”, it does not address a complete ban on all heading for players of this age group. The “Heading minimisation” phrase might be considered weaker still, as this refers to minimising—rather than banning—exposure to heading and describes limits for 11 to 13-year olds.

While the introduction of precautionary measures might be interpreted by laypeople as a warning for potential danger (Wiedemann et al., 2006), evidence from the current study suggests this is dependent on the strength of precaution advised/enacted. At the same time, there is a possibility that greater scientific uncertainty drives more imaginative appraisals of potential risks (MacGregor et al., 1994). This notion is consistent with findings that information intended to be reassuring—coupled with a disclosure of uncertainty—generates deeper message processing than under conditions of certainty (Nabi, 2002).

In their investigation of emotional influences on judgment, including risk perception, Lerner and Keltner (2000) identify three central appraisal themes relating to fear: uncertainty, unpleasantness and situational control. Their findings indicate that fear predicts pessimistic assessments of risk, and the uncertainty of that risk plays a central role in this emotional appraisal. With this notion in mind, the data presented here suggests that uncertainty might play a similar role in the context of junior heading regulation, wherein the disclosure of scientific uncertainty might prompt deeper message processing and subsequent appraisals of fear, especially among already fearful individuals. Importantly, the absence of an uncertainty effect among those in the “Heading minimisation” group suggests that such an increase in risk perception also hinges on the strength of precaution being discussed. Within this cohort, it appears that the more conservative the proposed regulation (e.g., limiting heading as opposed to banning it), the less influence uncertainty has on perceived risk.

Considering potential risks requires processing complex information, and this process is often simplified by relying on certain heuristics (Sunstein, 2005). The “availability heuristic” is one such bias that may influence an individual’s perceived risk relative to heading in football. When making judgments about risk, individuals are more likely to consider a risk significant if it is cognitively available, i.e., if they can readily imagine the risk coming to fruition (Sunstein, 2005). Because events that are likely and occur frequently are easy to recall, the availability heuristic provides a helpful tool for quickly assessing the probability that one might be in danger. However, these judgements are often influenced by factors such as emotional saliency and recency (Slovic, 2000).

82

A. Gilbert

This influence can be direct, owing to past experience of a similar event, or it can be indirect, wherein highly publicised events can give certain risks more weight (Sunstein, 2005). Indeed, individuals tend to underestimate the actual risk of underreported dangers, while significantly overestimating risks that receive greater media attention (Kuran & Sunstein, 1999).

MacGregor et al. (1994) note a possible availability effect in their study of EMF risk perception. After briefly mentioning arguments that EMF exposure is linked to chronic depression, MacGregor et al. (1994) observed a sharp increase in participants agreeing that a link between the two exists. This increase was despite careful emphasis that “there is so little evidence about these effects that, at this point, such arguments are really just speculation” (p. 827).

Head injury in sport receives considerable media attention, and it is therefore possible that this highly publicised issue increases the salience, and therefore availability, of risk attributed to heading in football. Simply discussing a potential link between heading exposure and neurodegenerative disease may have triggered a similar effect within participants in this study. When statistical knowledge is lacking/unavailable, people are likely to judge a risk based on its availability (Sunstein, 2005). The disclosure of uncertainty within this cohort might therefore prompt greater reliance on intuitive heuristics when estimating the risk of neurodegenerative disease due to heading. As noted by Johnson and Slovic (1995), “descriptions of uncertainty in risk estimates may undercut any illusion of safety” (p. 486). While knowledge of precautionary measures may provide some individuals with reassurance, disclosing the uncertainty that evoked such precaution could evoke an intuitive—and sometimes pessimistic—estimate of risk.

7.4 Conclusions

This study provides initial, albeit tentative findings in the context of risk perceptions regarding heading in football. Participants generally reported that heading in football poses a small-to- moderate health risk, although experimental manipulation produced some notable differences. The most significant finding presented here pertains to an interaction effect observed between uncertainty and precautionary information. Within this cohort, acknowledging precautionary measures (in the form of heading guidelines) tended to reduce perceived risk of heading compared to controls. Disclosing scientific uncertainty generally acted in the opposite direction, resulting in higher risk perception scores—especially when coupled with examples of strong precaution. Interestingly, however, this uncertainty effect was not apparent when combined with an example of weaker precaution. This trend leads to the conclusion that risk perceptions are

83

A. Gilbert contingent on both (1) the presence of uncertainty information, and; (2) the strength of precautionary measures discussed.

7.4.1 Implications for communicating heading regulations

Ensuring that all relevant information is available to the public, including information regarding uncertainty, is critical to ensure risk management remains transparent (Frewer et al., 2003). However, as observed in this study and others, simply disclosing the existence of uncertainty can impact the way an individual perceives risk. Similarly, the level of precaution being advised/implemented appears to impact this perception. As a result, the communication of present and future heading guidelines to the public should involve careful consideration of how these factors might cause undue concern.

The adoption of heading guidelines represents a precautionary approach to uncertain science. On the topic of precaution, Sunstein (2005, p. 63) notes that “Public alarm, even if ill informed, is itself a harm”, highlighting the often-overlooked potential for cautious policies to increase societal concerns. Recent findings implicating football participation as a possible neurological health risk (Lee et al., 2019; Mackay et al., 2019) and the precautionary measures employed to reduce junior heading might understandably raise concerns among relevant parties. However, the general benefits of sport participation and physical activity in reducing health risks such as cardiovascular disease, stroke, diabetes and obesity are beyond doubt (McKee et al., 2014; Taioli, 2007; Teramoto & Bungum, 2010).

With physical inactivity representing the fourth highest risk factor for global mortality (World Health Organization, 2010), ensuring that risk communication does not act to increase public alarm and drive individuals away from sport participation appears prudent. At the same time, the public must be made aware of evidence that certain activities pose some risk of harm. As noted by Schütz et al. (2007), the challenge therefore appears to be explaining precautionary measures in a manner that justifies their implementation to the public and acts as a cue of increased safety, rather than as confirmation that a serious risk exists. Part of this explanation should include distinguishing between proven and unproven risks (Wiedemann et al., 2006), as well as an acknowledgment of the value judgments underpinning regulatory decisions (Lemons et al., 1997).

Considerations such as these add to the ever-present onus on the public to increase its science literacy. In order to be an effective partner in policy decisions, citizens must remain informed and aware of relevant research (MacGregor et al., 1994). However, an understanding of all elements

84

A. Gilbert inherent to decision making—including uncertainty and value-laden judgments—should be cultivated among the public to ensure the intention of precautionary measures can be recognised and appreciated (Lemons et al., 1997). Although people tend to acknowledge the value of precaution, this does not necessarily coincide with an opinion that precaution represents good governance (Barnett et al., 2008).

7.5 Limitations & future directions

This thesis presents an initial enquiry into the domain of risk perception regarding heading in football. Although interesting data emerged, it is important to note that these are subject to several limitations. The most notable of these is the limited sample size, with this study’s population (n= 89) unable to reach the desired total of 180 participants. Due to the relatively small population being sampled (players and parents within New Zealand football), a snowball sampling technique was necessary in order to reach the desired participants. Although this technique was considered appropriate given the exploratory nature of the study, this method of participant recruitment can be difficult and time-consuming. Without achieving the minimum required sample size yielded by prior power analysis (n= 180), further studies including more participants are required to determine whether the effects observed here are truly representative of the wider New Zealand football population.

Another important note regarding recruitment pertains to the global COVID-19 pandemic arising in early 2020. It would not be inconceivable for the exceptional circumstances surrounding the pandemic and New Zealand’s subsequent lockdown to influence one’s general attitude towards risk, uncertainty and precaution—even if this was only a temporary influence. Very few participants were recruited during this period, however, therefore it seems unlikely that this had much bearing on this study’s overall data. Indeed, it was the lack of recruitment over this period that had the greatest impact on this study’s findings, as New Zealand’s lockdown status during Alert Level 4 coincided with the onset of the domestic football season in March of 2020. Plans to attend local game days and tournaments for recruitment were unfortunately disrupted, and several clubs expressing interest in the online survey were unable to finalise registrations over this time, meaning the contact details for most players and parents were not made available until it was necessary for final analyses to be completed.

The findings presented here relate to the effects of uncertainty and precautionary information, although there are many other factors that may influence perceived risk related to heading in football. Low risk perceptions may be the result of increased trust/confidence in risk

85

A. Gilbert management (Earle, 2010), thus it would be of interest to assess whether experimental manipulation influences one’s level of trust/confidence in scientific processes underpinning regulatory decisions. An individual’s reported trust and confidence might also reflect a general propensity to trust strangers and general confidence about the future, respectively (Earle, 2010). Future studies controlling for these traits may shed more light on variations in risk perception within this population.

Wiedemann et al. (2017) suggest traditional risk perception measurements might reflect inaccurate estimations of public concern, as individuals with enduring concerns exhibit attitudes that are more emotionally and morally influenced. Typical risk perception surveys may not be able to distinguish between individuals to whom concerns are more relevant and enduring, and those who do not share such strong attitudes. Indeed, people make sense of risk management strategies in diverse ways (Timotijevic & Barnett, 2006), it would therefore be appropriate to investigate factors beyond uncertainty and precautionary information in order to glean a more comprehensive understanding of how the public perceive the risk of heading in football.

Players and parents within football were selected for this study to ensure that the message regarding subconcussion research and heading regulation retained relevance. Responses indicating that most participants consider football to be of great personal importance confirms that the subject matter was highly relevant to this cohort. Future studies would benefit from observing risk perceptions across different populations, as it would be of interest to observe how the perceptions of people affiliated with football differ from those of the general public. It is understood that regulatory decisions such as junior heading regulations are largely driven by concerns expressed by relevant parties. While this study did not observe a significant difference in risk perception between players and parents, future research should further explore the risk attitudes of those to whom football heading research and heading regulations are most applicable.

86

A. Gilbert

References

Alosco, M. L., Tripodis, Y., Jarnagin, J., Baugh, C. M., Martin, B., Chaisson, C. E., … Stern, R. A. (2017). Repetitive head impact exposure and later-life plasma total tau in former National Football League players. Alzheimer’s and Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Disease Monitoring, 7, 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2016.11.003 Arnone, D., Abou-Saleh, M. T., & Barrick, T. R. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in addiction. Neuropsychobiology, 54(2), 107–113. https://doi.org/10.1159/000096992 Babbs, C. F. (2001). Biomechanics of heading a soccer ball: implications for player safety. The Scientific World Journal, 1, 281–322. https://doi.org/10.1100/tsw.2001.56 Babikian, T., Merkley, T., Savage, R. C., Giza, C. C., & Levin, H. (2015). Chronic aspects of pediatric traumatic brain injury: Review of the literature. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(23), 1849– 1860. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2015.3971 Bailes, J., Petraglia, A., Omalu, B., Nauman, E., & Talavage, T. (2013). Role of subconcussion in repetitive mild traumatic brain injury: A review. J Neurosurg, 119(5), 1235–1245. https://doi.org/10.3171/2013.7.JNS121822 Bamac, B., Tamer, G.S., Colak, T., Seyrek, E., Duman, C., Colak, S., & Özbek, A. (2011). Effects of repeatedly heading a soccer ball on serum levels of two neurotrophıc factors of brain tissue BDNF and NGF. Biology of Sport, 28(3). https://doi.org/10.5604/959284 Barnett, J., Timotijevic, L., Shepherd, R., & Senior, V. (2007). Public responses to precautionary information from the Department of Health (UK) about possible health risks from mobile phones. Health Policy, 82(2), 240–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.healthpol.2006.10.002 Barnett, J., Timotijevic, L., Vassallo, M., & Shepherd, R. (2008). Precautionary advice about mobile phones: public understandings and intended responses. Journal of Risk Research, 11(4), 525–540. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669870802086430 Bauer, J. A., Thomas, T. S., Cauraugh, J. H., Kaminski, T. W., & Hass, C. J. (2001). Impact forces and neck muscle activity in heading by collegiate female soccer players. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19(3), 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1080/026404101750095312 Baugh, C. M., Stamm, J. M., Riley, D. O., Gavett, B. E., Shenton, M. E., Lin, A., … Stern, R. A. (2012). Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: neurodegeneration following repetitive concussive and subconcussive brain trauma. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 6(2), 244–254. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-012-9164-5 Bazarian, J. J., Zhu, T., Zhong, J., Janigro, D., Rozen, E., Roberts, A., … Blackman, E. G. (2014). Persistent, long-term cerebral white matter changes after sports-related repetitive head impacts. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e94734. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094734 Belanger, H. G., Vanderploeg, R. D., & McAllister, T. (2016). Subconcussive blows to the head. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 31(3), 159–166. https://doi.org/10.1097/HTR.0000000000000138 Blair, S. N., & Morris, J. N. (2009). Healthy hearts-and the universal benefits of being physically active: Physical activity and health. Annals of Epidemiology, 19(4), 253–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2009.01.019 Blais, A. R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 33–47. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/A-Domain-Specific-Risk-Taking-(DOSPERT)- Scale-for-Blais-Weber/2ad37f7124498ec3a4a474dd02357a8817130d58

87

A. Gilbert

Blaylock, R. L., & Maroon, J. (2011). Immunoexcitotoxicity as a central mechanism in chronic traumatic encephalopathy—A unifying hypothesis. Surgical Neurology International, 2, 107. https://doi.org/10.4103/2152-7806.83391 Bodansky, D. (1991). Scientific uncertainty and the precautionary principle. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development, 33(7), 4–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/00139157.1991.9929978 Breedlove, E. L., Robinson, M., Talavage, T. M., Morigaki, K. E., Yoruk, U., O’Keefe, K., … Nauman, E. A. (2012). Biomechanical correlates of symptomatic and asymptomatic neurophysiological impairment in high school football. Journal of Biomechanics, 45(7), 1265–1272. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JBIOMECH.2012.01.034 Bretzin, A. C., Mansell, J. L., Tierney, R. T., & McDevitt, J. K. (2017). Sex differences in anthropometrics and heading kinematics among division I soccer athletes. Sports Health: A Multidisciplinary Approach, 9(2), 168–173. https://doi.org/10.1177/1941738116678615 Brewer, N. T., Chapman, G. B., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., McCaul, K. D., & Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: The example of vaccination. Health Psychology, 26(2), 136–145. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278- 6133.26.2.136 Broglio, S. P., Guskiewicz, K. M., Sell, T. C., & Broglio, M. (2004). No acute changes in postural control after soccer heading. Br J Sports Med, 38, 561–567. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2003.004887 Broglio, S. P., Ju, Y. Y., Broglio, M. D., & Sell, T. C. (2003). The efficacy of soccer headgear. Journal of Athletic Training, 38(3), 220–224. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14608431 Broglio, S. P., Schnebel, B., Sosnoff, J. J., Shin, S., Feng, X., He, X., & Zimmerman, J. (2010). Biomechanical properties of concussions in high school football. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 42(11), 2064–2071. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181dd9156 Broomell, S. B., & Kane, P. B. (2017). Public perception and communication of scientific uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(2), 286–304. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000260 Bunc, G., Ravnik, J., & Velnar, T. (2017). May heading in soccer result in traumatic brain injury? A review of literature. Medical Archives (Sarajevo, Bosnia and Herzegovina), 71(5), 356–359. https://doi.org/10.5455/medarh.2017.71.356-359 Caccese, J. B., & Kaminski, T. W. (2016). Minimizing head acceleration in soccer: A review of the literature. Sports Medicine, 46(11), 1591–1604. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-016- 0544-7 Caccese, J. B., Lamond, L. C., Buckley, T. A., & Kaminski, T. W. (2016). Reducing purposeful headers from goal kicks and punts may reduce cumulative exposure to head acceleration. Research in Sports Medicine, 24(4), 407–415. https://doi.org/10.1080/15438627.2016.1230549 Caccese, J. B., Buckley, T. A., Tierney, R. T., Arbogast, K. B., Rose, W. C., Glutting, J. J., & Kaminski, T. W. (2018). Head and neck size and neck strength predict linear and rotational acceleration during purposeful soccer heading. Sports Biomechanics, 17(4), 426-476. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2017.1360385 Caccese, J. B., Buckley, T. A., Tierney, R. T., Rose, W. C., Glutting, J. J., & Kaminski, T. W. (2017). Sex and age differences in head acceleration during purposeful soccer heading. Research in Sports Medicine, 26(1), 64–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/15438627.2017.1393756 Catenaccio, E., Caccese, J., Wakschlag, N., Fleysher, R., Kim, N., Kim, M., … Lipton, M. L. (2016). Validation and calibration of HeadCount, a self-report measure for quantifying heading

88

A. Gilbert

exposure in soccer players. Research in Sports Medicine, 24(4), 416–425. https://doi.org/10.1080/15438627.2016.1234472 Catenaccio, E., Mu, W., Kaplan, A., Fleysher, R., Kim, N., Bachrach, T., … Lipton, M. L. (2017). Characterization of neck strength in healthy young adults. PM&R, 9(9), 884–891. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmrj.2017.01.005 Chrisman, S. P. D., Whitlock, K. B., Kroshus, E., Schwien, C., Herring, S. A., & Rivara, F. P. (2019). Parents’ perspectives regarding age restrictions for tackling in youth football. Pediatrics, 143(5). https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-2402 Clark, J. F., Elgendy-Peerman, H. T., Divine, J. G., Mangine, R. E., Hasselfeld, K. A., Khoury, J. C., & Colosimo, A. J. (2017). Lack of eye discipline during headers in high school girls soccer: A possible mechanism for increased concussion rates. Medical Hypotheses, 100, 10–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2016.12.016 Colvin, A. C., Mullen, J., Lovell, M. R., West, R. V., Collins, M. W., & Groh, M. (2009). The role of concussion history and gender in recovery from soccer-related concussion. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 37(9), 1699–1704. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546509332497 Dashnaw, M. L., Petraglia, A. L., & Bailes, J. E. (2012). An overview of the basic science of concussion and subconcussion: where we are and where we are going. Neurosurgical Focus, 33(6), 1–9. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3171/2012.10.FOCUS12284 Davenport, E. M., Whitlow, C. T., Urban, J. E., Espeland, M. A., Jung, Y., Rosenbaum, D. A., … Maldjian, J. A. (2014). Abnormal white matter integrity related to head impact exposure in a season of high school varsity football. Journal of Neurotrauma, 31(19), 1617–1624. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2013.3233 Davenport, E. M., Urban, J. E., Mokhtari, F., Lowther, E. L., Van Horn, J. D., Vaughan, C. G., … Maldjian, J. A. (2016). Subconcussive impacts and imaging findings over a season of contact sports. Concussion, 1(4), CNC19. https://doi.org/10.2217/cnc-2016-0003 Delaney, J. S., Al-Kashmiri, A., Drummond, R., Correa, J. A., & Delaney, J. S. (2008). The effect of protective headgear on head injuries and concussions in adolescent football (soccer) players. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 42(2), 110–115. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2007.037689 Dezman, Z. D. W., Ledet, E. H., & Kerr, H. A. (2013). Neck strength imbalance correlates with increased head acceleration in soccer heading. Sports Health: A Multidisciplinary Approach, 5(4), 320–326. https://doi.org/10.1177/1941738113480935 Di Virgilio, T. G., Hunter, A., Wilson, L., Stewart, W., Goodall, S., Howatson, G., … Ietswaart, M. (2016). Evidence for acute electrophysiological and cognitive changes following routine soccer heading. EBioMedicine, 13, 66–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.10.029 Didehbani, N., Cullum, C. M., Mansinghani, S., Conover, H., & Hart, J. (2013). Depressive symptoms and concussions in aging retired NFL players. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28, 418–424. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/act028 Dorminy, M., Hoogeveen, A., Tierney, R. T., Higgins, M., Mcdevitt, J. K., & Kretzschmar, J. (2015). Effect of soccer heading ball speed on S100B, sideline concussion assessments and head impact kinematics. Brain Inj, 29(10), 1158–1164. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2015.1035324 Downs, D. S., & Abwender, D. (2002). Neuropsychological impairment in soccer athletes. The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 42(1), 103–107. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11832883 Dyck, J., & Talty, F. (2016). Citizen attitudes about sports, concussions and CTE. http://www.uml.edu/polls

89

A. Gilbert

Earle, T. C. (2010). Trust in risk management: A model-based review of empirical research. Risk Analysis, 30(4), 541–574. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2010.01398.x Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183–201. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047707 Eaton, L. (2002). Coroner cites football as reason for brain injury. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 325(7373), 1133. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.325.7373.1133/a Echemendia, R. J., Putukian, M., Mackin, R. S., Julian, L., & Shoss, N. (2001). Neuropsychological test performance prior to and following sports-related mild traumatic brain injury. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 11(1), 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1097/00042752-200101000- 00005 Ellemberg, D., Leclerc, S., Couture, S., & Daigle, C. (2007). Prolonged neuropsychological impairments following a first concussion in female university soccer athletes. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 17(5), 369–374. https://doi.org/10.1097/JSM.0b013e31814c3e3e FIFA Communications Division. (2007, May 31). FIFA Big Count 2006: 270 million people active in football. https://resources.fifa.com/image/upload/big-count-stats-package- 520046.pdf?cloudid=mzid0qmguixkcmruvema Fischhoff, B., & Davis, A. L. (2014). Communicating scientific uncertainty. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(Suppl 4), 13664–13671. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317504111 Forbes, C. R., Glutting, J. J., & Kaminski, T. W. (2016). Examining neurocognitive function in previously concussed interscholastic female soccer players. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 5(1), 14–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2014.933108 Foster, K. R., Vecchia, P., & Repacholi, M. H. (2000). Science and the precautionary principle. Science, 288(5468), 979–981. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3075092 Frewer, L., Hunt, S., Brennan, M., Kuznesof, S., Ness, M., & Ritson, C. (2003). The views of scientific experts on how the public conceptualize uncertainty. Journal of Risk Research, 6(1), 75–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/1366987032000047815 Gallant, C., Drumheller, A., & McKelvie, S. J. (2017). Effect of improper soccer heading on serial reaction time task performance. Current Psychology, 36(2), 286–296. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9414-6 Gavett, B. E., Stern, R. A., & Mckee, A. C. (2011). Chronic traumatic encephalopathy: A potential late effect of sport-related concussive and subconcussive head trauma. Clin Sports Med, 30, 179–188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.csm.2010.09.007 Gessel, L. M., Fields, S. K., Collins, C. L., Dick, R. W., & Comstock, R. D. (2007). Concussions among United States high school and collegiate athletes. Journal of Athletic Training, 42(4), 495– 503. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0162-0908(08)79294-8 Gnacinski, S. L., Arvinen-Barrow, M., Brewer, B. W., & Meyer, B. B. (2017). Factorial validity and measurement invariance of the Perceived Susceptibility to Sport Injury scale. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 27(7), 754–761. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.12681 Goldstein, B. D. (2001). The precautionary principle also applies to public health actions. American Journal of Public Health, 91(9), 1358–1361. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.91.9.1358 Goldstein, B., & Carruth, R. S. (2004). The precautionary principle and/or risk assessment in world trade organization decisions: A possible role for risk perception. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 491–499. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00452.x

90

A. Gilbert

Gollier, C., & Treich, N. (2003). Decision-making under scientific uncertainty: The economics of the precautionary principle. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 27(1), 77–103. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025576823096 Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Bailes, J., McCrea, M., Cantu, R. C., Randolph, C., & Jordan, B. D. (2005). Association between recurrent concussion and late-life cognitive impairment in retired professional football players. Neurosurgery, 57(4), 719–726. https://doi.org/10.1227/01.NEU.0000175725.75780.DD Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Broglio, S. P., Cantu, R. C., & Kirkendall, D. T. (2002). No evidence of impaired neurocognitive performance in collegiate soccer players. The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 30(2), 157–162. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F03635465020300020201 Guskiewicz, K. M., & Mihalik, J. P. (2011). Biomechanics of sport concussion. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 39(1), 4–11. https://doi.org/10.1097/JES.0b013e318201f53e Gutierrez, G. M., Conte, C., & Lightbourne, K. (2014). The relationship between impact force, neck strength, and neurocognitive performance in soccer heading in adolescent females. Pediatric Exercise Science, 26(1), 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1123/pes.2013-0102 Gysland, S. M., Mihalik, J. P., Register-Mihalik, J. K., Trulock, S. C., Shields, E. W., & Guskiewicz, K. M. (2012). The relationship between subconcussive impacts and concussion history on clinical measures of neurologic function in collegiate football players. Annals of Biomedical Engineering, 40(1), 14–22. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10439-011-0421-3 Hanlon, E. M., & Bir, C. A. (2012). Real-time head acceleration measurement in girls’ youth soccer. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 44(6), 1102–1108. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3182444d7d Hanlon, E., & Bir, C. (2010). Validation of a wireless head acceleration measurement system for use in soccer play. Journal of Applied Biomechanics, 26(4), 424–431. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.26.4.424 Haran, F., Tierney, R., Wright, W., Keshner, E., & Silter, M. (2012). Acute changes in postural control after soccer heading. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 34(04), 350–354. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1304647 Harmon, K. G., Drezner, J. A., Gammons, M., Guskiewicz, K. M., Halstead, M., Herring, S. A., … Roberts, W. O. (2013). American Medical Society for Sports Medicine position statement: concussion in sport. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 47(1), 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1136/BJSPORTS-2012-091941 Harremoës, P., Gee, D., MacGarvin, M., Stirling, A., & Keys, J. (2001). Late lessons from early warnings: the precautionary principle 1896-2000. European Environment Agency. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.418.1171&rep=rep1&type=pd f Harris, J., & Holm, S. (2002). Extending human lifespan and the precautionary paradox. The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 27(3), 355–368. https://doi.org/10.1076/jmep.27.3.355.2983 Harriss, A., Johnson, A. M., Walton, D. M., & Dickey, J. P. (2019). Head impact magnitudes that occur from purposeful soccer heading depend on the game scenario and head impact location. Musculoskeletal Science and Practice, 40, 53–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msksp.2019.01.009 Harriss, A., Walton, D. M., & Dickey, J. P. (2018). Direct player observation is needed to accurately quantify heading frequency in youth soccer. Research in Sports Medicine, 26(2), 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1080/15438627.2018.1431534

91

A. Gilbert

Hicks, R. R., Martin, V. B., Zhang, L., & Seroogy, K. B. (1999). Mild experimental brain injury differentially alters the expression of neurotrophin and neurotrophin receptor mRNAs in the hippocampus. Experimental Neurology, 160(2), 469–478. https://doi.org/10.1006/exnr.1999.7216 Hiles, S. A., Lamers, F., Milaneschi, Y., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2017). Sit, step, sweat: Longitudinal associations between physical activity patterns, anxiety and depression. Psychological Medicine, 47(8), 1466–1477. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716003548 Hodson, J. (2020). Sydney football club APIA Leichhardt bans headers for kids. Ministry of Sport. https://ministryofsport.com.au/sydney-football-club-apia-leichhardt-bans-headers-for- kids/ Hootman, J. M., Dick, R., & Agel, J. (2007). Epidemiology of collegiate injuries for 15 sports: Summary and recommendations for injury prevention initiatives. Journal of Athletic Training, 42(2), 311–319. Hughes, J. (2006). How not to criticize the precautionary principle. The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 31(5), 447–464. https://doi.org/10.1080/03605310600912642 Hunter, L. E., Ifrah, C., Zimmerman, M. E., Kim, M., Lipton, R. B., Stewart, W. F., & Lipton, M. L. (2018). Heading and unintentional head impacts have opposing associations with Patient Reported Outcomes in amateur soccer players. Research in Sports Medicine, 26(4), 390– 400. https://doi.org/10.1080/15438627.2018.1492396 Hwang, S., Ma, L., Kawata, K., Tierney, R., & Jeka, J. J. (2017). Vestibular dysfunction after subconcussive head impact. Journal of Neurotrauma, 34(1), 8–15. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2015.4238 Janda, D. H., Bir, C. A., & Cheney, A. L. (2002). An evaluation of the cumulative concussive effect of soccer heading in the youth population. Injury Control and Safety Promotion, 9(1), 25–31. https://doi.org/10.1076/icsp.9.1.25.3324 Jansen, P., & Lehmann, J. (2018). Investigating cognitive performance deficits in male and female soccer players after a 4-week heading-training programme: A controlled study. Brain Impairment, 19(02), 133–140. https://doi.org/10.1017/BrImp.2017.4 Johnson, B. B., & Slovic, P. (1995). Presenting uncertainty in health risk assessment: Initial studies of its effects on risk perception and trust. Risk Analysis, 15(4), 485–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1995.tb00341.x Jordan, S. E., Green, G. A., Galanty, H. L., Mandelbaum, B. R., & Jabour, B. A. (1996). Acute and chronic brain injury in United States national team soccer players. The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 24(2), 205–210. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/036354659602400216 Kalichová, M., & Lukášek, M. (2019). Soccer heading evaluation during learning process using an accelerometer. Journal of Physical Education and Sport, 19, 335–343. https://doi.org/10.7752/jpes.2019.s2050 Kaminski, T. W., Wikstrom, A. M., Gutierrez, G. M., & Glutting, J. J. (2007). Purposeful heading during a season does not influence cognitive function or balance in female soccer players. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(7), 742–751. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580600976911 Kaminski, T. W., Cousino, E. S., & Glutting, J. J. (2013). Examining the relationship between purposeful heading in soccer and computerized neuropsychological test performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79(2), 235–244. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2008.10599486

92

A. Gilbert

Kawata, K., Tierney, R., Phillips, J., & Jeka, J. J. (2016). Effect of repetitive sub-concussive head impacts on ocular near point of convergence. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 37(5), 405–410. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0035-1569290 Kawata, K., Rubin, L. H., Lee, J. H., Sim, T., Takahagi, M., Szwanki, V., … Langford, D. (2016). Association of football subconcussive head impacts with ocular near point of convergence. JAMA Ophthalmology, 134(7), 763. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2016.1085 Kawata, K., Rubin, L. H., Takahagi, M., Lee, J. H., Sim, T., Szwanki, V., … Langford, D. (2017). Subconcussive impact-dependent increase in plasma S100b levels in collegiate football players. Journal of Neurotrauma, 34(14), 2254–2260. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2016.4786 Kawata, K., Rubin, L. H., Wesley, L., Lee, J. H., Sim, T., Takahagi, M., … Langford, D. (2018). Acute changes in plasma total tau levels are independent of subconcussive head impacts in college football players. Journal of Neurotrauma, 35(2), 260–266. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2017.5376 Kemp, S., Duff, A., & Hampson, N. (2016). The neurological, neuroimaging and neuropsychological effects of playing professional football: Results of the UK five-year follow-up study. Brain Injury, 30(9), 1068–1074. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2016.1148776 Keohane, R. O., Lane, M., & Oppenheimer, M. (2014). The ethics of scientific communication under uncertainty. Politics, Philosophy & Economics, 13(4), 343–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470594X14538570 Killam, C., Cautin, R. L., & Santucci, A. C. (2005). Assessing the enduring residual neuropsychological effects of head trauma in college athletes who participate in contact sports. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 599–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2005.02.001 King, D., Hume, P., Gissane, C., Brughelli, M., & Clark, T. (2016). The influence of head impact threshold for reporting data in contact and collision sports: Systematic review and original data analysis. Sports Medicine, 46(2), 151–169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-015- 0423-7 Kirkendall, D. T., & Garrett, W. E. (2001). Heading in soccer: Integral skill or grounds for cognitive dysfunction? Journal of Athletic Training, 36(3), 328–333. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12937505 Kirkendall, D. T., Jordan, S. E., & Garrett, W. E. (2001). Heading and head injuries in soccer. Sports Med, 31(5), 369–386. https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200131050-00006 Koerte, I. K., Ertl-Wagner, B., Reiser, M., Zafonte, R., & Shenton, M. E. (2012). White matter integrity in the brains of professional soccer players without a symptomatic concussion. JAMA, 308(18), 1859. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2012.13735 Koerte, I. K., Lin, A. P., Muehlmann, M., Merugumala, S., Liao, H., Starr, T., … Shenton, M. E. (2015). Altered neurochemistry in former professional soccer players without a history of concussion. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(17), 1287–1293. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2014.3715 Koerte, I. K., Lin, A. P., Willems, A., Muehlmann, M., Hufschmidt, J., Coleman, M. J., … Shenton, M. E. (2015). A review of neuroimaging findings in repetitive brain trauma. Brain Pathology, 25(3), 318–349. https://doi.org/10.1111/bpa.12249 Koerte, I. K., Mayinger, M., Muehlmann, M., Kaufmann, D., Lin, A. P., Steffinger, D., … Shenton, M. E. (2016). Cortical thinning in former professional soccer players. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10(3), 792–798. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9442-0

93

A. Gilbert

Koerte, I. K., Nichols, E., Tripodis, Y., Schultz, V., Lehner, S., Igbinoba, R., … Sereno, A. B. (2017). Impaired cognitive performance in youth athletes exposed to repetitive head impacts. Journal of Neurotrauma, 34(16), 2389–2395. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2016.4960 Kontos, A. P., Dolese, A., Elbin, R. J., Covassin, T., & Warren, B. L. (2011). Relationship of soccer heading to computerized neurocognitive performance and symptoms among female and male youth soccer players. Brain Injury, 25(12), 1234–1241. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2011.608209 Kontos, A. P., Braithwaite, R., Chrisman, S. P. D., Mcallister-Deitrick, J., Symington, L., Reeves, V. L., & Collins, M. W. (2017). Systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of football heading. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 51(15), 1118–1124. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2016-096276 Kunreuther, H. (2002). Risk analysis and risk management in an uncertain world. Risk Analysis, 22(4), 655–664. https://doi.org/10.1111/0272-4332.00057 Kuran, T., & Sunstein, C. R. (1999). Availability cascades and risk regulation. Stanford Law Review, 51(4), 683. Lamond, L. C., Caccese, J. B., Buckley, T. A., Glutting, J., & Kaminski, T. W. (2018). Linear acceleration in direct head contact across impact type, player position, and playing scenario in collegiate women’s soccer players. Journal of Athletic Training, 53(2), 115–121. https://doi.org/10.4085/1062-6050-90-17 Lautenschlager, N. T., Cox, K. L., Flicker, L., Foster, J. K., Van Bockxmeer, F. M., Xiao, J., … Almeida, O. P. (2008). Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: A randomized trial. JAMA - Journal of the American Medical Association, 300(9), 1027–1037. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.300.9.1027 Lee, E. B., Kinch, K., Johnson, V. E., Trojanowski, J. Q., Smith, D. H., & Stewart, W. (2019). Chronic traumatic encephalopathy is a common co-morbidity, but less frequent primary dementia in former soccer and rugby players. Acta Neuropathologica, 138(3), 389–399. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-019-02030-y Lemons, J., Shrader-Frechette, K., & Cranor, C. (1997). The precautionary principle: Scientific uncertainty and type I and type II errors. Foundations of Science, 2(2), 207–236. https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1023%2FA%3A1009611419680.pdf Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 14, pp. 473–493. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402763 Levitch, C. F., Zimmerman, M. E., Lubin, N., Kim, N., Lipton, R. B., Stewart, W. F., … Lipton, M. L. (2019). Recent and long-term soccer heading exposure Is differentially associated with neuropsychological function in amateur players. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24, 147–155. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000790 Ling, H., Morris, H. R., Neal, J. W., Lees, A. J., Hardy, J., Holton, J. L., … Williams, D. D. R. (2017). Mixed pathologies including chronic traumatic encephalopathy account for dementia in retired association football (soccer) players. Acta Neuropathologica, 133(3), 337–352. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00401-017-1680-3 Lipton, M. L., Ifrah, C., Stewart, W. F., Fleysher, R., Sliwinski, M. J., Kim, M., & Lipton, R. B. (2018). Validation of HeadCount-2w for estimation of two-week heading: Comparison to daily reporting in adult amateur player. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 21(4), 363–367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2017.08.008 Lipton, M. L., Kim, N., Zimmerman, M. E., Kim, M., Stewart, W. F., Branch, C. A., & Lipton, R. B. (2013). Soccer heading is associated with white matter microstructural and cognitive abnormalities. Radiology, 268(3), 850–857. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.13130545

94

A. Gilbert

MacDonald, J., & Myer, G. D. (2017, October 1). “Don’t let kids play football”: A killer idea. British Journal of Sports Medicine, Vol. 51, pp. 1448–1449. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports- 2016-096833 MacGregor, D. G., Slovic, P., & Morgan, M. G. (1994). Perception of risks from electromagnetic fields: A psychometric evaluation of a risk-communication approach. Risk Analysis, 14(5), 815–828. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1994.tb00293.x Mackay, D. F., Russell, E. R., Stewart, K., MacLean, J. A., Pell, J. P., & Stewart, W. (2019). Neurodegenerative disease mortality among former professional soccer players. New England Journal of Medicine, 381(19), 1801–1808. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa1908483 Maher, M. E., Hutchison, M., Cusimano, M., Comper, P., & Schweizer, T. A. (2014). Concussions and heading in soccer: A review of the evidence of incidence, mechanisms, biomarkers and neurocognitive outcomes. Brain Inj, 28(3), 271–285. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2013.865269 Mainwaring, L., Ferdinand Pennock, K. M., Mylabathula, S., & Alavie, B. Z. (2018). Subconcussive head impacts in sport: A systematic review of the evidence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 132, 39–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.01.007 Malcolm, D. (2019). Football’s concussion crisis tells us as much about the dangers of sport as it does about the society we live in. Huffington Post. https://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/entry/football- concussion_uk_5daf24afe4b0f34e3a7d56c0?guccounter=1&guce_referrer=aHR0cHM6Ly9 3d3cuZ29vZ2xlLmNvbS8&guce_referrer_sig=AQAAAKVHXTGkJIMSYoxTYFhVeRbdx5Mnb 9TNIa-q-rm_X-34cRBFVS0a0jzM5M9LvBje- 9S5V8mmO05IfApdS_IDywgLtBkXiDyofeFHfSlpPLelHevJvEkQ8Vw- r5ZDsyFi5Z8GxtW9BxwK7yBLDGuRIMj-UtEUefhI42RtjOy40tXA Mangus, B. C., Wallmann, H. W., & Ledford, M. (2004). Analysis of postural stability in collegiate soccer players before and after an acute bout of heading multiple soccer balls. Soccer, Sports Biomechanics, 3(2), 209–220. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763140408522841 Mansell, J., Tierney, R. T., Sitler, M. R., Swanik, K. A., & Stearne, D. (2005). Resistance training and head-neck segment dynamic stabilization in male and female collegiate soccer players. Journal of Athletic Training, 40(4), 310-319. Martland, H. S. (1928). Punch drunk. Journal of the American Medical Association, 91(15), 1103. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1928.02700150029009 Matser, J. T., Kessels, A. G. H., Jordan, B. D., Lezak, M. D., & Troost, J. (1998). Chronic traumatic brain injury in professional soccer players. Neurology, 51(3), 791–796. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.51.3.791 Matser, E. J. T., Kessels, A. G., Lezak, M. D., Jordan, B. D., & Troost, J. (1999). Neuropsychological impairment in amateur soccer players. JAMA, 282(10), 971. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.282.10.971 Matser, J., Kessels, A., & Troost, J. (2001). A dose-response relation of headers and concussions with cognitive impairment in professional soccer players. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(6), 770–774. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.6.770.1029 Mayer, A. R., Kaushal, M., Dodd, A. B., Hanlon, F. M., Shaff, N. A., Mannix, R., … Meier, T. B. (2018). Advanced biomarkers of pediatric mild traumatic brain injury: Progress and perils. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 94, 149–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.002

95

A. Gilbert

McCrory, P., Makdissi, M., Davis, G., & Collie, A. (2005). Value of neuropsychological testing after head injuries in football. Br J Sports Med, 39, 58–63. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.020776 McCrory, P., Meeuwisse, W. H., Kutcher, J. S., Jordan, B. D., & Gardner, A. (2013). What is the evidence for chronic concussion-related changes in retired athletes: behavioural, pathological and clinical outcomes? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 47(5), 327–330. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2013-092248 McCuen, E., Svaldi, D., Breedlove, K., Kraz, N., Cummiskey, B., Breedlove, E. L., … Nauman, E. A. (2015). Collegiate women’s soccer players suffer greater cumulative head impacts than their high school counterparts. Journal of Biomechanics, 48, 3720–3723. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2015.08.003 McKee, A. C., Cantu, R. C., Nowinski, C. J., Hedley-Whyte, E. T., Gavett, B. E., Budson, A. E., … Stern, R. A. (2009). Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in athletes: Progressive tauopathy after repetitive head injury. Journal of Neuropathology & Experimental Neurology, 68(7), 709– 735. https://doi.org/10.1097/NEN.0b013e3181a9d503 Mez, J., Daneshvar, D. H., Kiernan, P. T., Abdolmohammadi, B., Alvarez, V. E., Huber, B. R., … McKee, A. C. (2017). Clinicopathological evaluation of chronic traumatic encephalopathy in players of American football. JAMA - Journal of the American Medical Association, 318(4), 360–370. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2017.8334 Ministry for the Environment. (2000). National guidelines for managing the effects of radiofrequency transmitters. https://www.mfe.govt.nz/sites/default/files/radio-freq- guidelines-dec00.pdf Molcho, M., & Pickett, W. (2011). Some thoughts about “acceptable” and “non-acceptable” childhood injuries. Injury Prevention, 17(3), 147–148. https://doi.org/10.1136/ip.2010.030023 Montenigro, P. H., Alosco, M. L., Martin, B. M., Daneshvar, D. H., Mez, J., Chaisson, C. E., … Tripodis, Y. (2017). Cumulative head impact exposure predicts later-life depression, apathy, executive dysfunction, and cognitive impairment in former high school and college football players. Journal of Neurotrauma, 34(2), 328–340. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2016.4413 Moore, G. (2015, November 11). FA have no plans to copy US ban on headers. The Independent. https://www.independent.co.uk/sport/football/news-and-comment/us-authorities-ban- under-11s-from-heading-a6730491.htmla Myer, G. D., Barber Foss, K., Thomas, S., Galloway, R., DiCesare, C. A., Dudley, J., … Yuan, W. (2019). Altered brain microstructure in association with repetitive subconcussive head impacts and the potential protective effect of jugular vein compression: a longitudinal study of female soccer athletes. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 53(24), 1539–1551. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2018-099571 Nabi, R. (2002). Anger, fear, uncertainty, and attitudes: a test of the cognitive-functional model. Communication Monographs, 69(3), 204–216. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750216541 Narimatsu, K., Takeda, T., Nakajima, K., Konno, M., Ozawa, T., & Ishigami, K. (2015). Effect of clenching with a mouthguard on head acceleration during heading of a soccer ball. General Dentistry, 63(6), 41–46. Naunheim, R. S., Standeven, J., Richter, C., & Lewis, L. M. (2000). Comparison of impact data in hockey, football, and soccer. The Journal of Trauma, 48(5), 938–941. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005373-200005000-00020 Naunheim, R. S., Ryden, A., Standeven, J., Genin, G., Lewis, L., Thompson, P., & Bayly, P. (2003). Does soccer headgear attenuate the impact when heading a soccer ball? Academic Emergency Medicine, 10(1), 85–90. https://doi.org/10.1197/aemj.10.1.85

96

A. Gilbert

Neselius, S., Brisby, H., Marcusson, J., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., & Karlsson, T. (2014). Neurological assessment and its relationship to CSF biomarkers in amateur boxers. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e99870. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099870 Nielsen, J. B., Elstein, A., Gyrd-Hansen, D., Kildemoes, H. W., Kristiansen, I. S., & Støvring, H. (2010). Effects of alternative styles of risk information on EMF risk perception. Bioelectromagnetics, 31(7), 504–512. https://doi.org/10.1002/bem.20586 NZ Football. (2018, July 3). Football a favourite for Kiwis. https://www.nzfootball.co.nz/newsarticle/64807#:~:text=There%20were%20over%202 10%2C000%20recreational,in%20junior%20football%20each%20weekend. O’kane, J. W. (2016). Is heading in youth soccer dangerous play? The Physician and Sportsmedicine, 44(2), 190–194. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913847.2016.1149423 O’Kane, J. W., Spieker, A., Levy, M. R., Neradilek, M., Polissar, N. L., & Schiff, M. A. (2014). Concussion among female middle-school soccer players. JAMA Pediatrics, 168(3), 258–264. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.4518 Oliver, J. M., Jones, M. T., Kirk, K. M., Gable, D. A., Repshas, J. T., Johnson, T. A., … Zetterberg, H. (2016). Effect of docosahexaenoic acid on a biomarker of head trauma in American football. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 48(6), 974–982. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0000000000000875 Padilla, L. M., Hansen, G., Ruginski, I. T., Kramer, H. S., Thompson, W. B., & Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2015). The influence of different graphical displays on nonexpert decision making under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000037 Papa, L., Ramia, M. M., Edwards, D., Johnson, B. D., & Slobounov, S. M. (2015). Systematic review of clinical studies examining biomarkers of brain injury in athletes after sports-related concussion. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(10), 661–673. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2014.3655 Pearce, A. J. (2016). The neurophysiological response following sub-concussive soccer heading. EBioMedicine, 13, 3–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.10.043 Pepper, J. K., Squiers, L. B., Peinado, S. C., Bann, C. M., Dolina, S. D., Lynch, M. M., … McCormack, L. A. (2019). Impact of messages about scientific uncertainty on risk perceptions and intentions to use electronic vaping products. Addictive Behaviors, 91, 136–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ADDBEH.2018.10.025 Peterson, M. (2006). The precautionary principle is incoherent. Risk Analysis, 26(3), 595–601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00781.x Peterson, M. (2007). The precautionary principle should not be used as a basis for decision- making. Talking point on the precautionary principle. EMBO Reports, 8(4), 305–308. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.embor.7400947 Podaskoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 Politi, M. C., Clark, M. A., Ombao, H., Dizon, D., & Elwyn, G. (2011). Communicating uncertainty can lead to less decision satisfaction: A necessary cost of involving patients in shared decision making? Health Expectations, 14(1), 84–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369- 7625.2010.00626.x Poole, V. N., Abbas, K., Shenk, T. E., Breedlove, E. L., Breedlove, K. M., Robinson, M. E., … Dydak, U. (2014). MR spectroscopic evidence of brain injury in the non-diagnosed collision sport athlete. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(6), 459–473. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2014.940619

97

A. Gilbert

Press, J. N., & Rowson, S. (2017). Quantifying head impact exposure in collegiate womenʼs soccer. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 27(2), 104–110. https://doi.org/10.1097/JSM.0000000000000313 Putukian M., Echemendia R. J., & Mackin S. (2000). The acute neuropsychological effects of heading in soccer: A pilot study. Clinical Journal of Sports Medicine, 10(2), 104–109. Quintero, L. M., Moore, J. W., Yeager, M. G., Rowsey, K., Olmi, D. J., Britton‐Slater, J., … Zezenski, L. E. (2019). Reducing risk of head injury in youth soccer: An extension of behavioral skills training for heading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53, 237-248. doi:10.1002/jaba.557 Raffensperger, C., Schettler, T., & Myers, N. (2000). Precaution: Belief, regulatory system, and overarching principle. International Journal of Occupational and Environmental Health, 6(4), 266–269. https://doi.org/10.1179/oeh.2000.6.4.266 Resnik, D. B. (2003). Is the precautionary principle unscientific? Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 34(2), 329–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1369-8486(02)00074-2 Rex, C. C., & Metzler, J. N. (2016). Development of the Sport Injury Anxiety Scale. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 20(3), 146–158. https://doi.org/10.1080/1091367X.2016.1188818 Reynolds, B. B., Patrie, J., Henry, E. J., Goodkin, H. P., Broshek, D. K., Wintermark, M., & Druzgal, T. J. (2017a). Comparative analysis of head impact in contact and collision sports. Journal of Neurotrauma, 34(1), 38–49. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2015.4308 Reynolds, B. B., Patrie, J., Henry, E. J., Goodkin, H. P., Broshek, D. K., Wintermark, M., & Druzgal, T. J. (2017b). Effects of sex and event type on head impact in collegiate soccer. Orthopaedic Journal of Sports Medicine, 5(4), 232596711770170. https://doi.org/10.1177/2325967117701708 Rieder, C., & Jansen, P. (2011). No neuropsychological consequence in male and female soccer players after a short heading training. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(7), 583–591. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acr055 Rodrigues, A. C., Lasmar, R. P., & Caramelli, P. (2016). Effects of soccer heading on brain structure and function. Frontiers in Neurology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2016.00038 Rubin, T. G., Catenaccio, E., Fleysher, R., Hunter, L. E., Lubin, N., Stewart, W. F., … Lipton, M. L. (2018). MRI-defined white matter microstructural alteration associated with soccer heading is more extensive in women than men. Radiology, 289(2), 478–486. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.2018180217 Rubin, L. H., Tierney, R., Kawata, K., Wesley, L., Lee, J. H., Blennow, K., … Langford, D. (2019). Brain injury NFL blood levels are moderated by subconcussive impacts in a cohort of college football players. Brain Injury, 33(4), 456–462. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699052.2019.1565895 Russell, E. R., Stewart, K., Mackay, D. F., Maclean, J., Pell, J. P., & Stewart, W. (2019). Football’s InfluencE on Lifelong health and Dementia risk (FIELD): Protocol for a retrospective cohort study of former professional footballers. BMJ Open, 9(5), e028654. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-028654 Rutherford, A., Stephens, R., Fernie, G., & Potter, D. (2009). Do UK university football club players suffer neuropsychological impairment as a consequence of their football (soccer) play? Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 3(1), 15–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390802484755

98

A. Gilbert

Rutherford, A., Stephens, R., & Potter, D. (2003). The neuropsychology of heading and head trauma in association football (soccer): A review. Neuropsychology Review, 13, 153-179. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025525613477 Salinas, C. M., Webbe, F. M., & Devore, T. T. (2009). The epidemiology of soccer heading in competitive youth players. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 3(1), 15–33. https://doi.org/10.1123/jcsp.3.1.15 Sandin, P. (1999). Dimensions of the precautionary principle. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment: An International Journal, 5(5), 889–907. https://doi.org/10.1080/10807039991289185 Sandin, P., Peterson, M., Hansson, S. O., Rudén, C., & Juthe, A. (2002). Five charges against the precautionary principle. Journal of Risk Research, 5(4), 287–299. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669870110073729 Sandmo, S. B., McIntosh, A. S., Andersen, T. E., Koerte, I. K., & Bahr, R. (2019). Evaluation of an in- ear sensor for quantifying head impacts in youth soccer. The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 47(4), 974–981. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546519826953 Schuppe, J. (2015, November 11). Will soccer’s new header rules make kids safer? NBC News. https://www.nbcnews.com/news/us-news/will-soccers-new-header-rules-make-kids- safer-n460916 Schütz, H., Wiedemann, P., & Clauberg, M. (2007). The public perception of precaution. EMBO Reports, 8(7), 612–613. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.embor.7401019 Shenton, M. E., Hamoda, H. M., Schneiderman, J. S., Bouix, S., Pasternak, O., Rathi, Y., … Zafonte, R. (2012). A review of magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging findings in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 6(2), 137–192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-012-9156-5 Shewchenko, N., Withnall, C., Keown, M., Gittens, R., & Dvorak, J. (2005a). Heading in football. Part 1: Development of biomechanical methods to investigate head response. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39(Suppl 1). https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.019034 Shewchenko, N., Withnall, C., Keown, M., Gittens, R., & Dvorak, J. (2005b). Heading in football. Part 2: Biomechanics of ball heading and head response. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39(Suppl 1). https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.019042 Shewchenko, N., Withnall, C., Keown, M., Gittens, R., & Dvorak, J. (2005c). Heading in football. Part 3: Effect of ball properties on head response. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39(Suppl 1). https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.019059 Shrader-Frechette, K. S. (1993). Problematic inferences in assessing repository risks. In K. S. Shrader-Frechette (Ed.), Burying uncertainty: risk and the case against geological disposal of nuclear waste (pp. 103–159). https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/j.ctt1pp0mt.10%0A Shultz, S. R., MacFabe, D. F., Foley, K. A., Taylor, R., & Cain, D. P. (2011). Sub-concussive brain injury in the Long-Evans rat induces acute neuroinflammation in the absence of behavioral impairments. Behavioural Brain Research, 229, 145–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.015 Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G., & Roth, C. (2000). Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk/benefit perception. Risk Analysis, 20(3), 353–362. https://doi.org/10.1111/0272- 4332.203034 Smith, D. W., Bailes, J. E., Fisher, J. A., Robles, J., Turner, R. C., & Mills, J. D. (2012). Internal jugular vein compression mitigates traumatic axonal injury in a rat model by reducing the intracranial slosh effect. Neurosurgery, 70(3), 740–746. https://doi.org/10.1227/NEU.0b013e318235b991

99

A. Gilbert

Sortland, O., & Tysvaer, A. T. (1989). Brain damage in former association football players - An evaluation by cerebral computed tomography. Neuroradiology, 31(1), 44–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00342029 Spear, J. (1995). Are professional footballers at risk of developing dementia? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10(12), 1011–1014. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.930101203 Spiegelhalter, D., Pearson, M., & Short, I. (2011). Visualizing uncertainty about the future. Science, 333(6048), 1393–1400. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1191181 SportCIC. (2016, March 1). Open letter: Preventing injuries in children playing school rugby. http://www.sportcic.com/resources/Open%20Letter%20SportCIC%20March1st%20201 6.pdf Sport New Zealand. (2019). Active NZ 2018 Participation Report. Wellington: Sport New Zealand. https://sportnz.org.nz/assets/Uploads/Published-Final-Active-NZ-Main-Report.-The- New-Zealand-Participation-Survey-2018-12-August-2019.pdf Stålnacke, B. M., Ohlsson, A., Tegner, Y., & Sojka, P. (2006). Serum concentrations of two biochemical markers of brain tissue damage S-100B and neurone specific enolase are increased in elite female soccer players after a competitive game. Br J Sports Med, 40, 313– 316. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.021584 Stephan, Y., Deroche, T., Brewer, B. W., Caudroit, J., & Le Scanff, C. (2009). Predictors of perceived susceptibility to sport-related injury among competitive runners: The role of previous experience, neuroticism, and passion for running. Applied Psychology, 58(4), 672– 687. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00373.x Stephens, R., Rutherford, A., Potter, D., & Fernie, G. (2005). Neuropsychological impairment as a consequence of football (soccer) play and football heading: A preliminary analysis and report on school students (13–16 years). Child Neuropsychology, 11(6), 513–526. https://doi.org/10.1080/092970490959629 Stephens, R., Rutherford, A., Potter, D., & Fernie, G. (2010). Neuropsychological consequence of soccer play in adolescent U.K. school team soccer players. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 22(3), 295–303. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.2010.22.3.295 Stewart, W. F., Kim, N., Ifrah, C. S., Lipton, R. B., Bachrach, T. A., Zimmerman, M. E., … Lipton, M. L. (2017). Symptoms from repeated intentional and unintentional head impact in soccer players. Neurology, 88(9), 901–908. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003657 Stewart, W. F., Kim, N., Ifrah, C., Sliwinski, M., Zimmerman, M. E., Kim, M., … Lipton, M. L. (2018). Heading frequency is more strongly related to cognitive performance than unintentional head impacts in amateur soccer players. Frontiers in Neurology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018.00240 Stone, T., Stone, N., Roy, N., Melton, W., Jackson, J. B., & Nelakuditi, S. (2018). On smart soccer ball as a head impact sensor. IEEE Transactions on Instrumentation and Measurement, 68(8), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1109/TIM.2018.2872307 Straume-Næsheim, T. M., Andersen, T. E., Dvorak, J., & Bahr, R. (2005). Effects of heading exposure and previous concussions on neuropsychological performance among Norwegian elite footballers. Br J Sports Med, 39, 70–77. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.019646 Straume-Næsheim, T. M., Andersen, T. E., Holme, I. M. K., McIntosh, A. S., Dvorak, J., & Bahr, R. (2009). Do minor head impacts in soccer cause concussive injury? A prospective case- control study. Neurosurgery, 64(4), 719–725. https://doi.org/10.1227/01.NEU.0000340681.12949.6D

100

A. Gilbert

Straume-Næsheim, T. M., Andersen, T. E., Jochum, M., Dvorak, J., & Bahr, R. (2008). Minor head trauma in soccer and serum levels of S100B. Neurosurgery, 62(6), 1297–1306. https://doi.org/10.1227/01.neu.0000312714.63191.f5 Sunstein, C. R. (2005). Laws of Fear: Beyond the Precautionary Principle. Cambridge University Press Svaldi, D. O., McCuen, E. C., Joshi, C., Robinson, M. E., Nho, Y., Hannemann, R., … Talavage, T. M. (2017). Cerebrovascular reactivity changes in asymptomatic female athletes attributable to high school soccer participation. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 11(1), 98–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9509-6 Tagge, C. A., Fisher, A. M., Minaeva, O. V, Gaudreau-Balderrama, A., Moncaster, J. A., Zhang, X.-L., … Goldstein, L. E. (2018). Concussion, microvascular injury, and early tauopathy in young athletes after impact head injury and an impact concussion mouse model. Brain, 141(2), 422–458. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awx350 Taha, Z., & Hassan, M. H. A. (2016). Parametric analysis of the influence of elastomeric foam on the head response during soccer heading manoeuvre. Procedia Engineering, 147, 139–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.proeng.2016.06.203 Taioli, E. (2007). All causes mortality in male professional soccer players. European Journal of Public Health, 17(6), 600–604. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckm035 Talavage, T. M., Nauman, E. A., Breedlove, E. L., Yoruk, U., Dye, A. E., Morigaki, K. E., … Leverenz, L. J. (2014). Functionally-detected cognitive impairment in high school football players without clinically-diagnosed concussion. Journal of Neurotrauma, 31(4), 327–338. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2010.1512 Tarnutzer, A. A., Straumann, D., Brugger, P., & Feddermann-Demont, N. (2017). Persistent effects of playing football and associated (subconcussive) head trauma on brain structure and function: A systematic review of the literature. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 51(22), 1592 LP – 1604. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2016-096593 Teramoto, M., & Bungum, T. J. (2010). Mortality and longevity of elite athletes. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 13(4), 410–416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2009.04.010 Terwilliger, V. K., Pratson, L., Vaughan, C. G., & Gioia, G. A. (2016). Additional post-concussion impact exposure may affect recovery in adolescent athletes. Journal of Neurotrauma, 33(8), 761–765. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2015.4082 The FA. (2020, February 24). Updated heading guidance for youth training sessions. http://www.thefa.com/news/2020/feb/24/updated-heading-guidance-announcement- 240220 Tierney, R. T., Higgins, M., Caswell, S. V., Brady, J., McHardy, K., Driban, J. B., & Darvish, K. (2008). Sex differences in head acceleration during heading while wearing soccer headgear. Journal of Athletic Training, 43(6), 578–584. https://doi.org/10.4085/1062-6050-43.6.578 Timotijevic, L., & Barnett, J. (2006). Managing the possible health risks of mobile telecommunications: Public understandings of precautionary action and advice. Health, Risk & Society, 8(2), 143–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/13698570600677324 Topping, A. (2020, January 16). Mixed reception to proposal to ban children heading footballs. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/football/2020/jan/16/mixed-reception-to- proposal-to-ban-children-heading-footballs Tsushima, W. T., Ahn, J., Siu, A. M., Yoshinaga, K., Choi, S. Y., & Murata, N. M. (2018). Effects of repetitive subconcussive head trauma on the neuropsychological test performance of high school athletes: A comparison of high, moderate, and low contact sports. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 8(3), 223–230. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2018.1427095

101

A. Gilbert

Tysvaer, A. T., Storli, O.V., & Bachen, N. I. (1989). Soccer injuries to the brain: A neurologic and electroencephalographic study of former players. Acta Neurol. Scand, 80(2), 151–156. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1989.tb03858.x Tysvaer, A. T., & Lochen, E. A. (1991). Soccer injuries to the brain: A neuropsychologic study of former soccer players. The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 19(1), 56–60. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/036354659101900109 Tysvaer, A. T., & Storli, O.V. (1989). Soccer injuries to the brain: A neurologic and electroencephalographic study of active football players. The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 17(4), 573–578. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/036354658901700421 US Club Soccer. (2016). Concussions and head injuries. https://www.usclubsoccer.org/head- injuries van Asselt, M. B. . A., & Vos, E. (2006). The precautionary principle and the uncertainty paradox. Journal of Risk Research, 9(4), 313–336. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669870500175063 Vann Jones, S. A., Breakey, R. W., & Evans, P. J. (2014). Heading in football, long-term cognitive decline and dementia: Evidence from screening retired professional footballers. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48(2), 159–161. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2013- 092758 Wallace, C., Smirl, J. D., Zetterberg, H., Blennow, K., Bryk, K., Burma, J., … van Donkelaar, P. (2018). Heading in soccer increases serum neurofilament light protein and SCAT3 symptom metrics. BMJ Open Sport & Exercise Medicine, 4(1), e000433. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjsem-2018-000433 Wallsten, T. S., Budescu, D. V., Rapoport, A., Zwick, R., & Forsyth, B. (1986). Measuring the vague meanings of probability terms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(4), 348– 365. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.4.348 Webbe, F. M., & Ochs, S. R. (2003). Recency and frequency of soccer heading interact to decrease neurocognitive performance. Applied Neuropsychology, 10(1), 31–41. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN1001_5 Wiedemann, P. M., Schuetz, H., Boerner, F., Clauberg, M., Croft, R., Shukla, R., … Barnett, J. (2013). When precaution creates misunderstandings: The unintended effects of precautionary information on perceived risks, the EMF case. Risk Analysis, 33(10), 1788–1801. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12034 Wiedemann, P. M., & Schütz, H. (2005). The precautionary principle and risk perception: Experimental studies in the EMF area. Environmental Health Perspectives, 113(4), 402–405. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.7538 Wiedemann, P. M., Thalmann, A. T., Grutsch, M. A., & Schütz, H. (2006). The impacts of precautionary measures and the disclosure of scientific uncertainty on EMF risk perception and trust. Journal of Risk Research, 9(4), 361–372. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669870600802111 Wiedemann, P., Freudenstein, F., Böhmert, C., Wiart, J., & Croft, R. (2017). RF EMF risk perception revisited: Is the focus on concern sufficient for risk perception studies? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(6), 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14060620 Wiener, J. B., & Rogers, M. D. (2002). Comparing precaution in the United States and Europe. Journal of Risk Research, 5(4), 317–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669870210153684 Windschitl, P. D., & Wells, G. L. (1996). Measuring psychological uncertainty: Verbal versus numeric methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2(4), 343–364. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.2.4.343

102

A. Gilbert

Withnall, C., Shewchenko, N., Wonnacott, M., & Dvorak, J. (2005). Effectiveness of headgear in football. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, i40-i48. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2005.019174 Witol, A. D., & Webbe, F. M. (2003). Soccer heading frequency predicts neuropsychological deficits. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(4), 397-314. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/18.4.397 World Health Organization. (1948). Constitution of the World Health Organization. https://apps.who.int/gb/bd/PDF/bd47/EN/constitution-en.pdf?ua=1 World Health Organization. (2000). Electromagnetic fields and public health: cautionary policies. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/57268/Backgrounder-2000-March- eng.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y World Health Organization. (2010). Global recommendations on physical activity for health. https://www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/factsheet_recommendations/en/ Yang, Y. T., & Baugh, C. M. (2016). US youth soccer concussion policy: Heading in the right direction. JAMA Pediatrics, 170(5), 413–414. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.0338 Zetterberg, H., Jonsson, M., Rasulzada, A., Popa, C., Styrud, E., Hietala, M. A., … Zetterberg, H. (2007). No neurochemical evidence for brain injury caused by heading in soccer. Br J Sports Med, 41, 574–577. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2007.037143 Zhang, D. C., Highhouse, S., & Nye, C. D. (2019). Development and validation of the General Risk Propensity Scale (GRiPS). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 32(2), 152–167. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2102 Zonner, S. W., Ejima, K., Fulgar, C. C., Charleston, C. N., Huibregtse, M. E., Bevilacqua, Z. W., & Kawata, K. (2019). Oculomotor response to cumulative subconcussive head impacts in US high school football players. JAMA Ophthalmology, 137(3), 265-270. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.6193 Zuckerman, S. L., Solomon, G. S., Forbes, J. A., Haase, R. F., Sills, A. K., & Lovell, M. R. (2012). Response to acute concussive injury in soccer players: is gender a modifying factor? J Neurosurg Pediatrics, 10(6), 504–510. https://thejns.org/pediatrics/view/journals/j- neurosurg-pediatr/10/6/article-p504.xml

103

A. Gilbert

Appendix

Attitudes Towards Football in NZ Survey INFORMATION SHEET FOR PARTICIPANTS

Thank you for showing an interest in this project. Please read this information sheet carefully before deciding whether or not to participate. If you decide to participate we thank you. If you decide not to take part there will be no disadvantage to you and we thank you for considering our request.

Upon completing this survey you can choose to be entered into a draw to win a $50 New World voucher.

What is the Aim of the Project?

The aim of this research project is to assess the attitudes of those who play amateur football (soccer) in New Zealand toward several aspects of the current game.

What Types of Participants are being sought?

This project requires New Zealand residents aged 18 and over. Participants must have played football for a New Zealand club within the last 12 months AND/OR be the parent of a child/children aged 18 or under who plays club football.

What will Participants be asked to do?

Should you agree to take part in this project, you will be asked to: Provide relevant demographic information e.g., age, gender, footballing background. Read a brief article describing recent developments in world football. Fill out a brief questionnaire – approx. 7-10 minutes in duration. Please be aware that you may decide at any point not to take part in the project without any disadvantage to yourself.

What Data or Information will be collected and what use will be made of it?

The data collected will be securely stored in such a way that only those mentioned below will be able to gain access to it. Data obtained as a result of the research will be retained for at least 5 years in secure storage. Any personal information held on the participants may be destroyed at the completion of the research even though the data derived from the research will, in most cases, be kept for much longer or possibly indefinitely.

No material that could personally identify you will be used in any reports on this study. Results of this research may be published. The data from this project will be publicly archived so that it may be used by other researchers. The results of the project may be published and will be available in the University of Otago Library (Dunedin, New Zealand) but every attempt will be made to preserve your anonymity.

What if Participants have any Questions?

If you have any questions about our project, either now or in the future, please feel free to contact either:- Alexander Gilbert or Associate Professor Jesse Bering Centre for Science Communication Centre for Science Communication + 64 3 479 7939 +64 3 479 7939 [email protected] [email protected]

104

A. Gilbert

This study has been approved by the Department stated above. However, if you have any concerns about the ethical conduct of the research you may contact the University of Otago Human Ethics Committee through the Human Ethics Committee Administrator (ph +643 479 8256 or email [email protected]). Any issues you raise will be treated in confidence and investigated and you will be informed of the outcome.

o I have read and understood the information above

Attitudes towards NZ Football Survey CONSENT FORM FOR PARTICIPANTS

I have read the Information Sheet concerning this project and understand what it is about. All my questions have been answered to my satisfaction. I understand that I am free to request further information at any stage.

I know that:- My participation in the project is entirely voluntary; I am free to withdraw from the project at any time without any disadvantage;

Personal identifying information will be destroyed at the conclusion of the project but any raw data on which the results of the project depend will be retained in secure storage for at least five years; This project contains potentially sensitive information regarding sport-related injury; The results of the project may be published and will be available in the University of Otago Library (Dunedin, New Zealand) but every attempt will be made to preserve my anonymity.

o I agree to take part in this survey

Do you play in a football (soccer) team or are you a parent of a child aged 18 or under who plays in one?

o I play in a football team

o I am a parent of a child who plays in a football team

o I play in a football team and I am also a parent of a child 18 or under who plays in a football team

o None of the above

Please enter your age in years (using numerals, not words)

o Age: ______

105

A. Gilbert

Are you a New Zealand citizen?

o Yes

o No

Please select your gender

o Male

o Female

o Other

Please select your highest level of education

o Less than high school

o High school graduate

o Some university

o Undergraduate degree

o Graduate certificate or diploma

o Master's or equivalent

o PhD or equivalent

o Other (please state) ______

106

A. Gilbert

Please select your ethnicity

o NZ European

o Māori

o Pacific Islander

o Asian

o Other (please state) ______

o Prefer not to say

For how many years have you played football?

o Less than 1 year

o 1-5 years

o 6-10 years

o 11-15 years

o 16-20 years

o 21-25 years

o 26+ years

In what type of position do you normally play?

o Goalkeeper

o Defence

o Midfield

o Forward

107

A. Gilbert

On a scale from 1 to 7, how frequently do you carry out the following drills in training?

About Every half of all Never training 2 3 5 6 trainings 7 1 4

Passing o o o o o o o

Shooting o o o o o o o

Heading o o o o o o o Small- sided games o o o o o o o

General fitness o o o o o o o

Please estimate how many times you would do the following during a typical game:

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50

Attempt a slide tackle

Control the ball with your chest

Take a set-piece

Header the ball

Have a shot at goal

108

A. Gilbert

On a scale from 1 to 7, how important is your involvement in football to your personal identity?

o Not at all important 1

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5

o 6

o Extremely important 7

How many children aged 18 or under do you have who play football?

▼ 1 ... 10

(Parents with 1 child) What is the age of your child who plays football?

o ______

(Parents with 1 child) For how many years has your child been playing football?

o ______

109

A. Gilbert

(Parents with 1 child) To the best of your knowledge, please indicate your child's feelings towards the following aspects of football training and/or games on a scale from 1 to 7:

Neither Dislikes Likes a likes a great great Don't 2 4 nor 5 6 deal deal know dislikes 1 7 4

Playing with others o o o o o o o o Making strong tackles o o o o o o o o

Heading the ball o o o o o o o o Challenging their physical o o o o o o o o fitness

Learning new skills o o o o o o o o

(Parents with multiple children) Please list the ages of your children who play football (aged 18 and under) from eldest to youngest (e.g., 12, 8, 6)

______

(Parents with multiple children) Please list the number of years that each of your children, from eldest to youngest, have been playing football (e.g., 5, 3, 1)

______

110

A. Gilbert

(Parents with multiple children) To the best of your knowledge, please indicate your ELDEST child's feelings towards the following aspects of football training and/or games on a scale from 1 to 7:

Neither Dislikes Likes a likes a great great Don't 2 3 nor 5 6 deal deal know dislikes 1 7 4

Playing with others o o o o o o o o Making strong tackles o o o o o o o o

Heading the ball o o o o o o o o Challenging their physical o o o o o o o o fitness

Learning new skills o o o o o o o o

(Parents with multiple children) To the best of your knowledge, please indicate your YOUNGEST child's feelings towards the following aspects of football training and/or games on a scale from 1 to 7:

Neither Dislikes Likes a likes a great great Don't 2 3 nor 5 6 deal deal know dislikes 1 7 4

Playing with others o o o o o o o o Making strong tackles o o o o o o o o

Heading the ball o o o o o o o o Challenging their physical o o o o o o o o fitness

Learning new skills o o o o o o o o

111

A. Gilbert

On a scale from 1 to 5, please select to what extent you agree with the following statements:

Neither Strongly Strongly agree nor disagree 2 4 agree disagree 1 5 3

I attempt to recycle my waste whenever o o o o o possible

I am at a high risk of injuring myself when playing o o o o o sport

I find myself irritable if I have not had any o o o o o caffeine

I would take a risk even if it meant I might o o o o o get hurt

I tend to support local businesses even if it costs o o o o o me more money

I spend more time watching television/movies o o o o o than I do reading

I would consider sunbathing without sunscreen to be o o o o o risky

I try to get at least 8 hours of sleep each night o o o o o

I would consider myself more physically active than the average o o o o o person

I am comfortable approaching new people o o o o o

112

A. Gilbert

What do you believe is the chance that heading a football will damage your brain during your sporting career?

o 1 = very low chance

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5 = very high chance

How susceptible do you feel you are to heading-related brain damage during your sporting career?

o 1 = not at all susceptible

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5 = very susceptible

What do you believe is the chance that heading a football will damage your brain during your sporting career in terms of percentages?

o Less than 20% chance

o 20% to 40% chance

o 40% to 60% chance

o 60% to 80% chance

o More than 80% chance

113

A. Gilbert

What do you believe your chances are of damaging your brain from heading a football compared with other players in your league?

o 1 = a lot lower

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5 = a lot higher

On a scale from 1 to 7, please select to what extent you agree with the following statements:

If I expressed any concern for heading a football, my teammates would think I was just being weak

o Strongly disagree 1

o 2

o 3

o Neither agree nor disagree 4

o 5

o 6

o Strongly agree 7

114

A. Gilbert

If I expressed any concern for heading a football, my teammates would think I was just being lazy

o Strongly disagree 1

o 2

o 3

o Neither agree nor disagree 4

o 5

o 6

o Strongly agree 7

If I expressed any concern for heading a football, my teammates would think I was faking it

o Strongly disagree 1

o 2

o 3

o Neither agree nor disagree 4

o 5

o 6

o Strongly agree 7

115

A. Gilbert

What do you believe is the chance that heading a football will damage your child's brain while they play the sport?

o 1 = very low chance

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5 = very high chance

How susceptible do you feel your child is to heading-related brain damage while playing football?

o 1 = not at all susceptible

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5 = very susceptible

What do you believe is the chance that heading a football will damage your child's brain while they play the sport in terms of percentages?

o Less than 20% chance

o 20% to 40% chance

o 40% to 60% chance

o 60% to 80% chance

o More than 80% chance

116

A. Gilbert

What do you believe your child's chances are of damaging their brain from heading a football compared with other players in their league?

o 1 = a lot lower

o 2

o 3

o 4

o 5 = a lot higher

On a scale from 1 to 5, to what extent would you prefer that your child played in a league that regulated heading over one that did not, if this were an option?

o Not at all 1

o 2

o 3

o 4

o Definitely yes 5

Earlier in the survey you read an article about some health problems that might arise from heading a football. Do you remember how the article described the movement of the brain within the skull? If you do, click the answer below that describes what you remember. If you do not, click "I don't remember."

Please be honest! There is no penalty for failing to remember, but we need to know in order to use your data.

The article referred to movement of the brain within the skull as:

o Flushing

o Sloshing

o Lashing

o I don't remember

117

A. Gilbert

Creative component

Are we Heading for Disaster?

The link between football and dementia is strengthening, and our concern for the sport’s head injury burden continues to grow. The act of intentionally “heading” a football has been identified as a potential culprit, and many want to see its removal from the field. As the world’s largest sport faces an unprecedented era of change, players and parents alike are faced with a looming question: is it time for us to be worried?

At first glance, I’d have called Jeff Astle one of the luckiest men to have ever lived.

In my case, this denomination is typically reserved for anyone whose job entails kicking a ball about in front of thousands of adoring fans. But even by the standards of being a professional football player, Jeff was lucky. Born in 1942, Astle went on to play at the highest level of English football throughout the ‘60s and ‘70s. During his time at Albion, Astle was dubbed “the King” by the club’s fans, and the big centre-forward’s signature goal celebration— both arms raised high in unparalleled jubilation—was a common sight at for the better part of a decade.

“People used to ask him, what’s your proudest moment? And although Dad was extremely proud to play for England, obviously, he actually said it was when he scored the winning goal in the Cup final,” Jeff’s daughter, Dawn Astle, tells me from her home in the East Midlands. She’s referring, of course, to the 1968 FA Cup final, in which her father scored in extra time to clinch victory over Everton and lift the oldest trophy in world football, making him really, truly, lucky.

Astle’s Cup-winning finish flaunts all the hallmarks of a natural-born striker, placing the ball into the top corner with frightening power and precision. Although goals such as this were not uncommon throughout his career, it was Astle’s aerial prowess that solidified his reputation. “My dad was a prolific header of the ball,” says Dawn. “I mean, there are still people who come up to us now, of a certain age, who will say that nobody could head a ball like your dad. In fact, one comedian used to say Jeff Astle can head a ball further than I go on my holidays.” Even from the other side of the world, on the end of a barely adequate phone connection, Dawn’s sense of pride in her father’s achievements is unmistakeable. More prominent, though, is the rueful tone in her voice.

Dawn’s heartache was jarring to me. After all, growing up as a football-mad child, the lives and adventures of players such as Jeff Astle had permeated my wildest dreams. There was no one

118

A. Gilbert alive that I admired or envied more than the Beckhams, Zidanes and Ronaldos of the world. For my part, the most fortunate man alive was nothing if he never had the chance to play for club and country. It’s easy enough to dispel this sentiment as the fancies of a whimsical child, but I would sooner consider it a testament to the power of stories—especially those of our heroes. Some stories inspire an image of success, impelling us to seek out its glory. Others tear at this veil and threaten to lay bare a cold, distinct reality. As I would soon discover, the story of Jeff Astle belongs to the latter.

***

On the 19th of January, 2002, Astle choked to death in front of his family following an ongoing struggle with dementia. He was 59 years old.

“It haunts me every single day,” says Dawn. The memory of her father’s death remains just as vivid eighteen years on, but the preceding years as a helpless onlooker, watching as dementia gradually tightened its grip on his once vibrant mind, have clearly lost no potency. “When you see what it does, it’s just the most brutal, heart-breaking thing that… you never get over. You never get over.”

Contrary to my boyhood assumption of a footballer’s glistening life, Astle’s untimely death reveals a man’s decidedly unlucky fate, a reality masked by the triumph and reverence of a professional sporting career. Such a tale is by no means unfamiliar, but the demise of Jeff Astle perhaps holds more salience today than ever before. At the heart of a dispute that now grips the beautiful game, Astle’s death begs a troubling question: does heading a football cause brain damage?

From Dawn’s perspective, the answer is obvious. “I don’t think anybody will ever convince me that it’s not the repeated heading of footballs that’s the main problem,” she says, conceding that her opinions are likely to sound recalcitrant. In fact, her misgivings about the act of heading emerged long before her father succumbed to dementia, as she grew increasingly convinced that his reputation for this aspect of the sport would eventually betray a sobering truth: “The game Dad loved and mastered ultimately claimed his life.”

An account of her father’s final years helps to illuminate why Dawn feels as strongly as she does about heading. “He was about 54, and he just kept forgetting things,” she told me. Dawn recounted for me the various things—important things—that would elude her father’s memory on a regular basis, such as his grandson’s name, and if his mother was still alive. “My

119

A. Gilbert grandmother had died—heck—about 18 years beforehand.” In listening to her, it was clear that time had done little to dull the pain of these memories, and unravelling them for me was arduous, pensive work. “He was doing Fantasy Football with and — around the time of the Euros or the World Cup—and he was doing a lot of live television which is really quite daunting,” Dawn says. “I think we just wanted to think it was the pressure from that, and it was getting too much for him. But deep down…” she pauses, “I mean, my dad ran out in front of a hundred-odd thousand at Hampden Park when he played Scotland. He was never really fazed by nerves, at all.”

After my conversation with Dawn that night, I searched through numerous clips of her dad, and it soon became abundantly clear that her assessment of Jeff’s confidence was no exaggeration. Episodes of the mid-90’s TV show included a recurring segment titled “Jeff Astle Sings”, during which the Albion legend would conduct a studio-wide sing along to a different classic tune at the close of each show. In every piece of footage I have yet found of Astle, he has never failed to come across as the quintessential showman. The many dedicatory clips from friends and teammates that followed in the wake of his death only reinforced what I had come to assume: when he wasn’t on the pitch, the man was an entertainer, a prankster and an indomitable character.

Behind the scenes, however, Jeff was struggling. “He started to get quite a bit worse,” Dawn remembers. “And my mum used to beg him to go see a doctor, but my dad never recognised there was anything wrong.” Eventually, his family convinced him to undergo tests for his declining cognition. “My mum said that when she first walked into the hospital with Dad, the world was full of colour. But when she walked out, everything was grey. That’s when she knew her life, Dad’s life, all of our lives, would never be the same again.” In the weeks to come, additional scans confirmed the worst: still in his early 50s, Jeff was suffering from early onset dementia.

Dawn would go on to paint the harrowing picture of a loving husband and father, gradually whittled away by the devastating disease. “At one stage he’d try to eat anything that wasn’t edible. He would try to drink anything that was in liquid form, whether it was cooking oil, vinegar—he was incredibly reckless. He tried to get out of a moving car. He became completely—and I mean completely—socially unacceptable. He would scream in the face of strangers,” Dawn recalled with a mournful sigh. “My dad would have been mortified if he’d have known.”

The news of Astle’s sudden death rocked the West Bromwich community and left a grief- stricken family in pieces. The following day, West Brom played Walsall Football Club at home.

120

A. Gilbert

As the players prepared for kick-off, the stadium fell into a forlorn silence, thousands of Albion fans mourning the loss of a club’s beloved icon. West Brom’s centre-forward Jason Roberts would net the only goal that day, revealing a tribute to the fallen great beneath the blue and white stripes of his Albion kit, the colours Jeff once wore with pride. Meanwhile, the King’s name thundered throughout the Hawthorns once again.

But the echoes of his name did not peter out at the stadium gates now emblazoned with Jeff’s image. Ten months later, a monumental announcement would send shockwaves through the footballing world. In November of 2002, South Staffordshire coroner Andrew Haigh ruled Astle’s death as that of an “industrial disease,” attributing the diffuse trauma in Astle’s brain— and resulting Alzheimer’s diagnosis—to his occupational exposure of heading footballs. This shock autopsy finding offered some vindication to Dawn and her family, who were still reeling from years of behind-the-scenes heartache and confusion. But more importantly, as far as Dawn was concerned, the coroner’s stunning report heralded the beginning of a shift in attitudes towards the act of heading in football.

***

In November of 2015, the United States Soccer Federation announced the removal of heading for all players under the age of 11. On behalf of the nation’s various youth associations, the announcement marked the resolution of an ongoing class-action lawsuit opened in August of the previous year. Originally filed against the global organisation, FIFA, a group of concerned parents and players sought compensation for head injuries in the youth game, citing nearly 50,000 concussions sustained by US high school players in 2010 alone. When the federal court dismissed the case, deeming FIFA clear of any wrongdoing, the plaintiffs doubled down on the home front. U.S. Soccer would opt for an amicable resolution, agreeing to develop a more stringent protocol for youth concussion.

Included in this initiative was the adoption of nationwide regulations on junior heading exposure, the first case of such regulation in football’s history. Despite tenuous evidence to support the danger of heading, consideration of the developing brains of children impelled U.S. Soccer to err on the side of caution. While heading of any kind is now prohibited for players aged 10 and under, those between the age of 11 and 13 are permitted 30 minutes of heading during training each week (20 headers in total), and are free to head the ball during games.

121

A. Gilbert

Public response to this decision was mixed, to say the least. Several notable athletes championed the move from the outset, including Taylor Twellman—one of the most prolific goal scorers in the history of America’s Major League Soccer. Others saw the move as a step backwards for a nation trying desperately to compete on the global stage, some even going as far as to send Twellman death threats for his support of the ban. The changing culture on American soil swiftly prompted questions in other parts of the world as well. When the English FA were pressed for an answer to growing heading anxieties, they insisted that the UK had no intention of following the example of U.S. Soccer, citing a lack of conclusive scientific evidence to implicate the dangers of heading.

Yet, in February of 2020, less than 5 years after the American regulations were put into place, the English, Irish and Scottish Football Associations released a joint statement announcing the adoption of new junior heading guidelines.

This abrupt U-turn came on the heels of a 2019 study of former professional Scottish players carried out at the University of Glasgow. The FIELD (Football’s InfluencE on Lifelong health and Dementia risk) study found that, of the participants who died during the longitudinal investigation, 1.7% of former players had succumbed to neurodegenerative disease, while only 0.5% of matched controls had suffered the same fate. Subsequent analysis showed that, within this sample, former players were 3.5 times more likely to die of neurodegenerative disease than members of the general public. Although these results do not point to an exact cause for this increased risk of dementia, the concerning results were enough to see UK football reassess their stance on junior heading.

“I think the time will come where they’ll all do it,” Dawn tells me, reflecting on the recent changes to the junior game. Having spent the last eighteen years of her life advocating for further research into football and dementia, she’s finally witnessing her tireless work, and the suffering of her father, count towards change. Since launching the Jeff Astle Foundation in 2015, a charitable organisation with patrons including and Gary Neville, Dawn and her family have strived to increase brain injury awareness and provide support for those living with dementia. The UK’s updated heading guidelines are a step in the right direction, she believes, but she now shares the same question as many other worried individuals: why is heading only now being addressed?

At the turn of the millennium, the notion of brain trauma arising from the heading of footballs was not entirely novel, although it didn’t capture much in the way of attention, either. In 1999, former Scottish footballer Billy McPhail—a fellow juggernaut as heading goes—attempted to prove that his own presenile dementia was the result of football-related head injury before a

122

A. Gilbert benefit appeal tribunal. The case was promptly dismissed, and the tribunal refused to even consider heading as a potential contributor to the degradation of McPhail’s brain. Alzheimer’s disease continued to erode Billy’s health, culminating with his death in 2003, aged 75.

Jeff Astle’s death would pique a burgeoning interest among those atop English football in the early 2000s, with the FA and PFA (Professional Footballer’s Association) funding a ten-year prospective study of dementia risk within professional players. However, when the study collapsed halfway through, the lack of incentive to sustain these efforts revealed an interest that was trifling at best. Neither Dawn nor the wider public were made aware of this failure until 2014—eight years after the fact.

“When we found that the research that the FA and PFA had started had failed halfway through, that’s when I learned about CTE,” Dawn tells me. CTE, or “chronic traumatic encephalopathy,” is a neurodegenerative disease characterised by exposure to repeated brain trauma. The pathology of the disease was first described in 1928, when Dr Harrison Martland noted the “Parkinsonian” symptoms of boxers who had received multiple head injuries. In-ring parlance referred to this phenomenon as “punch drunk,” likening the stumbling gait, slurred speech and general cognitive difficulties of injured fighters to the effects of intoxication. The condition was later dubbed dementia pugilistica to better fit the medical vernacular, but this would change once more when medical researchers discovered that “boxer’s brain" was not exclusive to those who stepped into the ring.

For Dawn, the notion of a neurodegenerative disease arising from head trauma was hard to ignore. Upon reflecting on the abrupt decline of her father’s health, she began to wonder if he had been misdiagnosed all along. Looking into the case of Mike Webster, the first NFL player to be diagnosed with CTE by Dr Bennet Omalu in 2002, Dawn knew there was something to this hunch. “Dr Omalu actually said one of the most striking things was that [Webster] looked fifty years older than he was when he died. That’s when I thought, hang on a minute, my dad looked a hundred years older than he was.” I couldn’t disagree. Looking at some of the last photos taken of Astle, one would never guess he was younger than 60. The fatigue and weariness etched into his face instead spoke of a man several decades his senior.

Unwilling to leave any questions unanswered, Dawn continued in her pursuit for closure. Twelve years after Jeff died, the Astle family requested that his brain, which had been donated to medical science in 2002, be re-examined by the University of Glasgow’s Brain Injury Research Group (GBIRG), a leading facility for the study of sports-related head injury. In May of 2014, Dr Willie Stewart, head researcher at GBIRG (and principal investigator of the aforementioned FIELD study), confirmed the family’s suspicions. “It was absolutely no surprise to us whatsoever

123

A. Gilbert to see that Dad had actually been misdiagnosed and that he didn’t have Alzheimer’s… he had CTE,” Dawn tells me. As the first footballer ever diagnosed with CTE, the comparisons between Mike Webster and Jeff Astle had come full circle.

But Jeff would certainly not be the last to suffer this grim fate. Further studies have since observed CTE in other past footballers, while the concerning incidence of neurodegenerative disease in former pros suggests the sport’s dementia risk even rivals that of American football. Most people would hesitate to consider football a contact sport, let alone one that harbours some deadly neurological risk. But the evidence is mounting, and much of the research focus in this area now lies on a greater understanding of how heading, an activity unique to football, might contribute to this threat.

***

The plasticity of the human brain is nothing short of a marvel. The ability to create, consolidate and rearrange trillions of neural connections each day accounts for the immense processing power we all possess. But this malleability also leaves us susceptible to serious injury.

Our semi-solid brains are kept largely safe within robust cranial vaults. As it pertains to traumatic brain injury (TBI), however, the suspension of the brain within a pocket of cerebrospinal fluid reveals a significant caveat. When a force is applied to the head, the small, fluid-filled space surrounding the brain allows slight room for movement. If a force is sufficiently high, this movement can send the brain crashing into the interior wall of the skull, applying undue stress to cerebral tissue and blood vessels. When an impact like this is followed by a noticeable distortion in brain function (e.g., memory loss, dizziness, or a loss of consciousness altogether), we refer to the injury as concussion.

While our understanding of the detrimental effects of sports-related concussion continues to grow, a recent shift in attention sees researchers addressing the effects of smaller head impacts—the kind from which no obvious clinical symptoms arise. Dr Michael Lipton, Associate Director of the Gruss Magnetic Resonance Research Centre at Albert Einstein College of Medicine, is one such researcher making substantial contributions to our understanding of this “subconcussive” phenomenon.

For Dr Lipton and his research team, the issue of heading—and subconcussion in general— demands consideration of very subtle brain changes. From his office in the heart of New York City, Lipton explains to me a common problem in epidemiology. “In general,” he says, “when you

124

A. Gilbert have an environmental exposure, it takes some amount of exposure to confer an actual biological effect, and even more to confer a biological effect in the tissue that persists.” Only when enough of this pathology is built up, he tells me, do we begin noticing overt functional effects.

Some studies observe just this, illustrating slight decreases in memory, attention, or reaction time among players who carry out just a handful of headers in a laboratory, although many studies fail to replicate these findings. As far as Lipton is concerned, a lack of functional deficit hardly absolves heading; rather, a focus on these inconsistent results reflects a deeper concern, “If you’re waiting to see neuropsychological deficit, then that’s almost like it’s too late.”

As a tool for identifying concussion, the utility of cognitive testing is unquestioned. “There’s certainly a role for cognitive assessments,” says Lipton, “but I think the problem is that we’re not doing them enough. You may or may not have a baseline, and then you wait for there to be an overt clinical event like a concussion—then you test cognition. The question is, what was the effect of all the hits that happened before that concussion? Or what was the effect of the first concussion compared to the second or third?” To answer these questions, Lipton and his colleagues turn to more refined techniques.

In a 2013 study of amateur footballers, for instance, Lipton observed evidence of structural damage to certain areas of the brain, which was only apparent at a microscopic level. Using magnetic resonance imaging, or MRI, the researchers found signs of abnormalities in the brain’s white matter tracts, the tissue responsible for relaying information between different regions of the brain. Worryingly, the more heading a player had done over the prior season, the greater these abnormalities were. Signs of microstructural damage also predicted poorer performance on tests of memory.

The predominant concern generated by findings such as these pertains to the rapid acceleration and deceleration of the brain associated with head impacts, a phenomenon known as the “slosh effect.” A common analogy likens the brain to a bowl of jelly. If you tap the side of the bowl, the jelly undulates back and forth. Signs of microstructural changes suggest that this “sloshing” can rapidly stretch and compress the brain’s white matter, causing damage on a very small scale and, potentially, affecting cumulative change if such impacts occur over and over. Even in the immediate sense, we sometimes see evidence of this damage in the form of increased neuroinflammation after heading.

Once again, however, the science is far from settled. Although some studies observe increased markers of subtle brain injury within blood samples taken after heading, these markers have also been shown to increase after playing basketball, running and even swimming. Moreover,

125

A. Gilbert signs of cellular injury following heading tend to resolve over short periods of time, suggesting these hits are not beyond the scope of our intrinsic repair mechanisms. For researchers like Lipton, the most pressing issue becomes a need to understand the cumulative effect of these otherwise minor events.

“The problem is that most of the research going [is] relatively small, cross-sectional studies,” Lipton points out to me, “and I don’t think they’re going to give us a lot more information than we already have. I think it’s crucial to have large studies which follow more people over longer periods of time. That’s really where a lot of the information is going to come from.”

Despite most headers appearing benign in nature, the continued jarring of the brain over a player’s career may see these small changes begin to accumulate, imposing a burden of long- lasting damage. Nonetheless, when I spoke to Lipton about U.S. Soccer’s decision to regulate junior heading, he expressed some doubts, “I think it’s well intended, that’s for sure. I mean, their goal is to protect kids. Unfortunately, it’s not really based on evidence.”

Even the most robust findings to date, including that of the FIELD study, fail to identify the precise role that heading plays in one’s risk of dementia. There are many other contextual factors to consider when assessing the risk of neurodegenerative disease, including age, sex, alcohol consumption—even genetics. How each of these factors contributes to dementia in former professional athletes, unfortunately, remains largely unknown. And, as the old saying goes, “correlation does not imply causation.”

Another concern that Lipton has relates to the differences between groups of footballers. “I get asked very often, ‘how come you don’t look at professional players?’” he says, “and the answer is that they’re not really relevant to the population at risk.” In his opinion, the disparity between populations should not be overlooked. Six months after our discussion, the UK’s decision to regulate junior heading on the back of the FIELD study showed we’re willing to do just that.

“A quarter of a billion people in the world play soccer. How many of those are pros? I think focussing on professionals, for me, is just really kind of silly,” says Lipton. Others have similarly questioned the wisdom of regulating juniors based on the dementia risk of professional players. In a Huffington Post article from October of last year, for example, Dr Dominic Malcolm, a Reader in the Sociology of Sport at Loughborough University, wrote that, “Changing how often children can practice heading is not the logical policy outcome of the Glasgow findings, restructuring professional football may be. Paradoxically that’s not even up for discussion.”

***

126

A. Gilbert

Growing up in New Zealand, there was (and still is) a relative paucity of homegrown footballing heroes to look up to. That being said, the few elite players from this small country have certainly done their part in inspiring young Kiwis to play football, and none more so than Wynton Rufer. Voted the Oceania Footballer of the Century, Rufer is widely regarded as the best player to ever come out of Aotearoa. Today, he spends his time coaching at his Auckland academy, the Wynton Rufer Soccer School of Excellence, hoping to produce our next batch of world-beaters. Naturally, I wanted to know his take on the junior heading dilemma.

“Overall, what I’m trying to do is develop more international athletes in football, so you’ve got to teach them all the technical parts of the game, including heading,” says Rufer. “Generally speaking, we’re not really a great nation in football, unfortunately. So when it comes to heading the ball, we have to train it all the time, heading with the young kids—7, 8, 9 years of age— because they’ve just got very poor technique.”

When I reached out for an interview with him, I knew Wynton was entrenched in the junior game. What I didn’t know was whether the former All White had considered the prospect of junior heading guidelines in New Zealand—or if he even cared to talk about it. When he called me at lunchtime the next day, I realised he was eager to have this discussion. “For the development of the game, it’s probably not ideal,” Wynton admits. Although he was unwilling to dismiss the possibility of a health risk attributable to heading, Rufer’s hesitancy to promote regulation in New Zealand stemmed from the relative scarcity of heading among junior players. “There’s hardly any heading during a game. In two halves of 20 to 25 minutes, there might be a maximum of 5 instances where a kid’s heading the ball, so it’s not a lot.”

Observational studies support Wynton’s claim. Much of the research on junior populations observe infrequent heading, with a player’s average exposure often ranging between 1 and 2 headers per game. Interestingly, the knowledge of heading’s rarity in junior matches factored into the UK’s decision to omit gameplay from their heading guidelines entirely, opting to focus on limiting heading during training instead.

“If the kids don’t have the confidence and they don’t have the technique, they generally just don’t try to head the ball,” Rufer tells me, somewhat despondently. “It’s disappointing, really, but that’s why I don’t think it would be a problem health-wise.” Those within New Zealand Football appear to take a similar stance. As part of a statement prepared in February of 2018 by Dr Mark Fulcher, NZ Football’s Medical Director, the sport’s governing body echoed the sentiment of UK officials prior to their change of heart: “Based on the available evidence at present we do not believe that there is any need to ban heading or alter the laws of the game.”

127

A. Gilbert

NZ Football have released no further statements since the UK guidelines took effect, indicating there are no plans to follow suit.

Speaking to Rufer, I realised there would be some pushback against regulation in New Zealand, just as there was in the US and the UK. Detractors of heading regulation often address the practical limitations of such precaution. After all, mastery demands repetition, and to reach the lofty goals set by an institution like the Wynton Rufer Soccer School of Excellence requires consistent training.

I was always told that a professional footballer should have all the skills they need to excel in the sport by the age of 15. Watching teenagers emerge and thrive on the world stage, it’s hard to argue against this notion. Meeting these expectations means training every day from a very young age, crafting proficiency in numerous techniques—heading included. Whether heading belongs in the junior game or not, professional aspirations will often prevail. But our focus on the risk of junior heading threatens to overshadow a well-established threat: football’s head injury woes extend far beyond the junior game.

I learned as much when I turned 15.

Following a sodden Thursday night training, my youth grade coach sat me down to talk. The club’s premier team, he told me, was struggling for numbers. Having just celebrated my fifteenth birthday, I was at last eligible to play men’s grade football, a transition I had been eagerly awaiting.

I knew my ability, and I wanted to prove that I belonged at that level. For the first 40 minutes of my premier debut, I felt I was doing just that. My touch was good, I was completing passes, and my confidence was building. But it wasn’t to last.

Five minutes from halftime, I rose to challenge for an aerial ball. As I leapt up, hoping to send the ball back up the pitch, I instead felt a sickening crack to the back of my head. In a vain attempt to reach the ball first, my opponent misjudged its flight and sent his head careening into mine. The shock rattled my teeth and jolted down my spine. There was a familiar metallic taste in my mouth and, for a few seconds, pulsating black spots crept into my field of vision. I didn’t know it at the time, but these are all symptoms of concussion. In an instant, my confidence was shattered. For the first time on a football pitch, I was afraid.

As halftime neared, the opposition won a freekick within striking distance. I was placed in the wall as the first line of defence, but at this point I was sufficiently shell-shocked. As the ball was fired towards goal, I reflexively ducked to shield my face. The shot flew harmlessly wide. Halftime was called and we were making our way off the pitch when one of my teammates

128

A. Gilbert pulled me aside. “That was a good half Alex, you’re playing well,” he said. “But don’t ever let me catch you turning your head like that again.”

This is an all-too familiar scene in football, in the amateur and professional game alike. A player takes a knock to the head, brushes themself off, and continues on their way. Following a concussion, one’s susceptibility to further injury increases markedly. Generally, team medics are tasked with removing a player from the field if they suspect a head injury has occurred, but we see this system fail time and again.

***

Several weeks ago, I got a call from my older brother at around midnight. Having just finished watching a documentary on former NFL player Aaron Hernandez, he wanted to know if I’d seen it. I hadn’t, so he filled me in. In April of 2015, Hernandez was sentenced to life in prison for first-degree murder. Two years and four days later, he was found hanging in his cell, leaving his family to contemplate the root of a loved one’s destructive behaviour. Here, the story turns down an increasingly familiar path. Following his death, Hernandez’s brain was released to Boston University to investigate the possibility of underlying neurodegenerative disease. Dr Ann McKee’s findings were later described as the worst case of CTE ever observed in someone so young. Thus, the 27-year-old was added to an ever-growing list of athletes ravaged by a disease conferred by their own sport.

Sporadic correspondence is simply in my brother’s nature, so I thought nothing of this late-night phone call. Only when I hung up an hour later did it strike me; this wasn’t the first time he’d called wanting to talk about this subject. Since beginning my research in this area, our conversations had begun shifting further from the usual benign chat of current affairs, movies and football, and closer to the sombre domain of head injury in the latter. It suddenly became clear to me that, having played football his entire life, this was beginning to play on his mind— just as it was playing on mine.

When I next spoke to my brother, he aired his concerns openly, “you’re going to encounter a lot of people who think [heading] isn’t an issue, but if anyone’s in trouble, it’s probably me.”

And we’re not the only ones having this conversation. When I spoke to Dawn, she recalled meeting Alan Shearer while filming for a BBC documentary on football and dementia, “I sat with him for hours, and I said to him, ‘are you worried?’ He just looked me straight in the eye and said ‘yeah, I am.’ Maybe there are a lot of pros out there that are worried, and my god, I’m not

129

A. Gilbert surprised.” To this day, Shearer remains the English Premier League’s all-time top goal scorer, a significant portion of which were scored with his head. Reflecting on his career, Shearer notes that, for every goal he ever scored with a header, he would have practised it a thousand times over in training.

The results of the FIELD study are concerning, that much is for sure. But are our worries regarding heading truly justified, despite the unsettled science? And is the regulation of juniors really an appropriate response to these anxieties? These are difficult questions to answer. But a good place to start is with an understanding of the social and cognitive factors that so often influence our judgments of risk. After all, when it comes to heading, both players and parents must decide if the risk outweighs the reward, whether the science is certain or not.

***

When faced with an uncertain hazard, we’re often tasked with unpacking and assessing an abundance of complex information. Traditional risk assessment aims to describe risks posed by activities, technologies and environmental exposures in quantitative terms, providing measured estimates of the dangers life can throw at us. Despite offering probabilistic grounds for seemingly “rational” decision making, the extent of a risk as assessed by experts is often very different to that which is perceived by the public. Many factors might help to explain this difference, but the most obvious is surely the most fitting: for the most part, we aren’t experts, so we don’t think like experts.

In psychology, the theory of “dual processing” posits two distinct systems through which our thoughts arise. “System I” entails rapid, intuitive thinking, while “System II” involves more deliberative, rule-based considerations. When we lack the resources to make a protracted evaluation of danger, we have no choice but to rely on our intuitions. Consider the scenario of stumbling across a rattlesnake while hiking. Before you even have a chance to ask, “am I in some kind of danger?”, System I will likely provide an instant, emphatic response. In situations like this, the unconscious musings of System I are utterly invaluable. When time and means are available, System II kicks in for more calculated decision making. Working in tandem, these systems enable us to successfully navigate a complex and often dangerous world. But intuitions can be misleading, and this form of heuristic-driven judgment is not without error.

In October of 2002, for example, a series of sniper attacks throughout the Washington D.C. area left ten people dead and a city in panic. The targets appeared to be random, and the blanket of

130

A. Gilbert anxiety that ensued saw many taking precautions. Sporting tournaments were cancelled, schools kept their students indoors, and service stations erected partitions to conceal their patrons. Some even left the city limits altogether before feeling safe enough to step outside of their cars and use a petrol pump. Despite the horrific nature of the events leading to this distress, this behaviour reveals a dramatic example of disproportionate fear in response to risk.

For those donning bullet-proof vests to venture outside, the prospect of becoming the victim of a sniper attack had become salient almost overnight. The frightening details of recent events fed into people’s “availability heuristic,” an intuitive tool that explains, in part, our tendency to weigh certain risks as genuine and worthy of attention, while disregarding other risks entirely. When it’s easy for us to imagine a given risk coming to fruition, that risk is available. Several factors may act to increase the availability of a risk, including familiarity and recency. But our familiarity with a risk does not have to be the result of first-hand experience; you don’t need to tread on a rattlesnake to know that this spells danger. Indeed, incoming information via news, movies—even word-of-mouth—can have a profound effect on the way we judge risks. Our perceptions of the dementia risk attributed to football is no exception.

The example of sniper-related fears in Washington D.C. highlights the power of emotionally gripping news. As human beings, we share a tendency to focus on worst-case scenarios, and this habit can lead to the severe neglect of probability. A desire to take precautions often appears justified in that it entails the complete removal of risk. For those driving two hours to Virginia in order to buy fuel, however, the infinitesimally small risk of being killed by a sniper at a petrol station in the nation’s capital “irrationally” eclipsed the undoubtedly greater risk of being involved in a fatal car accident.

Of course, when a risk becomes more salient, it often does so for good reason. Six months ago, many of us might not have thought twice about an individual openly coughing or sneezing in public. Today, in the wake of the global COVID-19 pandemic, our response to such a behaviour is likely to be far more visceral. Owing to the considerable news coverage of the pandemic, the risk of disease transmission has never been more cognitively available to us. The precautions we now take are not in response to a novel risk, per se; we have always faced the possibility of disease through social interaction and neglected hygiene. But this amplified response is necessary. Because the risk of disease has been made so salient in the current milieu, we can react instantly and accordingly. In other words, the risk already occupies the front of our minds, so it takes little effort to retrieve.

For some of us, the risk of heading in football inhabits a similar space. Retiring from professional football aged 30 following his seventh serious concussion, Taylor Twellman wants

131

A. Gilbert to see heading abolished from the junior game entirely. Ryan Mason, former Tottenham Hotspur midfielder, stepped away from the sport in 2018 after a horror head clash fractured his skull. He too thinks children should not head a football. These attitudes should come as no surprise. For people like Twellman and Mason, direct experience of the anguish of head injury readily accommodates the visualisation of this risk. As a result, imagining how things could go terribly wrong for a child becomes an easy task. The same goes for a concerned parent.

Risk perception research shows that emotion is relevant to our assessment of a hazard. If an activity evokes negative emotion, we’re more likely to make pessimistic judgments of the risk that activity poses. From where Dawn Astle stands, the negative emotion associated with heading is pervasive. However, to say that emotion drives Dawn’s attitude towards heading is not to cast doubt on the seriousness or legitimacy of her concerns—far from it. When a risk is highly uncertain, as that of heading currently is, our intuitions play a prominent role.

Undoubtedly, our fears surrounding the risk of junior heading are influenced by these cognitive heuristics; to ignore their role in decision-making would be a mistake. Public perceptions of risk are important, and risk managers are obliged to respond to our concerns. But the task of reconciling public fears and appropriate regulation is difficult. On the subject of precautionary regulation, American legal scholar Cass Sunstein writes, “A sensible approach would attempt to counteract, rather than to embody, the various cognitive limitations that people face in thinking about risks.” In other words, effective regulation should not rely on the predispositions that so often lead us astray.

The emergence of junior guidelines also speaks to our broader attitudes towards risk. For a start, we don’t like it when risks are incurred without our volition. A heavy focus on the junior game might therefore be unsurprising, as we’d rather take most decisions out of the hands of children. For older players, especially professionals, this is a different story, as adults are able to make their own informed choices about risk. We also aren’t happy when a risk is seen to be inequitably distributed: if heading appears especially dangerous for kids, we’ll take extra measures to level the playing field.

Junior players are considered an at-risk population by virtue of being children. This is nothing new. But the best evidence we have to incriminate heading points explicitly to the professional game. “The guidelines aren’t for professionals, so there are no limits on the heading they do in training, which is a bit disappointing,” says Dawn. “But I’m working on that.” Despite her relief to see regulation for juniors, watching as this protection is withheld from players like her father is clearly disheartening to Dawn, who has become a fierce advocate of heading reform for professionals.

132

A. Gilbert

“There’s no question that if you prevent kids from heading the ball, then they have no risk from those impacts,” Lipton tells me. Certainly, at a cursory glance, the benefits of junior regulation would appear to far outweigh the costs. But Lipton’s concerns are for players later in development, those for whom current guidelines do not protect. “They may all of a sudden begin heading in a higher-level environment without really having the strength or the skill or experience behind them. To me, I think the looming question is, are there some unintended consequences of ostensibly protecting kids when they’re younger?” Here, Lipton confronts a sobering truth: despite the best of intentions, risk can never be fully excised. To choose precaution may be to simply choose one risk over another.

The overprotection of junior players may also have broader implications. Anxiety surrounding head injury in sport has grown in recent years, undoubtedly reaching its zenith with the highly publicised unfolding of the NFL’s concussion crisis. The tumultuous saga of condemnation and deception in America’s own brand of football has proven to be an ethical nightmare—and that story is far from over. The litigious origins of heading regulation in the United States might have come as no surprise, then, considering the prominent status of head injury in the psyche of the American public.

“When I started looking at this area, one of the most common things I would hear would be a parent saying they didn’t want their son to play American football, because of the head injury risk,” Lipton recalls, “so they steered them toward soccer.” I have often heard this exact anecdote in New Zealand, only regarding rugby rather than the gridiron. “At the time,” says Lipton, “[soccer] wasn’t even really considered a collision sport. It is now, so I think that’s a really big change.”

When extra protection is used to temper public concerns, great care must be taken to ensure that these precautions do not exacerbate existing fears. In colloquial terms, this might pessimistically be seen as “scaremongering.” Precaution can often be interpreted as proof that a risk is genuine, simply because a decision to act implies it is worth acting. Heeding the theme of unforeseen consequences, it would be prudent to consider the possibility that this “no smoke without fire” mentality may foster concerns regarding heading and act to reduce participation in junior football. The World Health Organisation currently ranks physical inactivity as the fourth highest risk factor of global mortality. With sedentary behaviour presenting a very real health concern, ensuring that junior football remains accessible, as well as safe, must be our ultimate goal.

At the heart of this issue remains a fundamental need for continued research. While the risk of heading currently remains uncertain, uncertainties can be resolved—sometimes rapidly. If a

133

A. Gilbert causal link between heading and dementia is established incontrovertibly, then we must be prepared to make changes, and not just to the junior game.

***

“To be honest, the easiest thing in the world would have been to say ‘oh well, never mind, Dad had a great life. Carry on.’ But once you start getting phone calls telling you about someone else’s dad and the team they played in, five of them had dementia, you start to think ‘really? That seems to be an awful lot, that’s about half the team.’”

Phone calls like these, Dawn tells me, have not been uncommon. As the result of her father’s experiences, she’s committed her life to assisting others laid low by the effects of dementia. Although tinged with sadness, Jeff’s memory has motivated her all the way. “I’d like to think he’s really proud of us,” Dawn tells me, her voice beginning to waver. “People say to me, ‘I’m sure if your dad had his time again, he wouldn’t change anything’. Perhaps not. But above all, above everything, even though football was his job, he thought more of his family than he did of his job.”

Dawn described a picture to me, 5 feet in width, of her father scoring in the ’66 Cup final displayed proudly at the family’s home in South Derbyshire. In an old interview, Jeff reflects on scoring that goal, “It was a dream come true,” he said, “something I’ll never, ever forget.” Sadly, as a result of his dementia, that’s exactly what happened. “He would lie on the settee at my parents’ house,” Dawn says, “surrounded by everything he’d won in the game. There were England caps, an FA Cup winner’s medal, a League Cup winner’s medal—but he remembered none of it. He didn’t know that he’d ever been a footballer.”

134