<<

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

Working memory in children’s math learning and

its disruption in dyscalculia

Vinod Menon

Working memory (WM) plays an essential role in children’s WM components at different stages of mathematical skill

mathematical learning. WM influences both the early acquisition. Deficits in WM in children with dyscalculia

foundational phases of knowledge acquisition and contribute to weaknesses in the representation of quanti-

subsequent maturation of problem solving skills. The role of ty information, as well as the ability to manipulate this



individual WM components in mathematical cognition depends information during numerical problem solving [7 ]. Con-

not only on problem complexity but also on individual vergent findings from studies provide fun-

differences in mathematical abilities. Furthermore, the damental insights into the link between WM and

contributions of individual WM components change mathematical cognition, and the mechanisms by which

dynamically over development with visuospatial processes poor WM contributes to dyscalculia. A common neural

playing an increasingly important role in learning and enhancing locus of deficits in visuospatial quantity representations

mathematical proficiency. Convergent findings from and visuospatial WM likely contributes to both numerical

neuroimaging studies are now providing fundamental insights magnitude judgment and arithmetic problem solving

into the link between WM and mathematical cognition, and the deficits in children with dyscalculia.

mechanisms by which poor WM contributes to learning

disabilities. Evidence to date suggests that visuospatial WM is

Working memory in children’s mathematical

a specific source of vulnerability in children with mathematical

cognition and learning

learning disabilities and needs to be considered as a key

The particular emphasis on WM in developmental stud-

component in cognitive, neurobiological, and developmental

ies has its origins in children’s immature problem solving

models of typical and atypical mathematical skill acquisition.

abilities, which require them to break down numerical

Address problems into more basic components. The use of such

Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States strategies necessitates greater reliance on WM systems

for problem solving in children. For example, children

Corresponding author: Menon, Vinod ([email protected])

rely more on counting strategies during simple arithmetic

problem solving and need to access multiple WM com-

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132 ponents including short-term storage and rule-based

manipulation and updating of the contents of stored

This review comes from a themed issue on Neuroscience of

information [8]. With increased proficiency and a switch

to fact retrieval strategies there is less demand and need

Edited by De´ nes Szu¨ cs, Fumiko Hoeft and John DE Gabrieli

for WM resources [9,10]. The link between WM and

For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial

children’s mathematical cognition and learning has large-

Available online 7th June 2016

ly been based on Baddeley and Hitch’s influential mul-

doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.014 ticomponent model [11,12]. Briefly, this model includes a

2352-1546/# 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. central executive component, responsible for high-level

control, monitoring, and task switching, along with two

subordinate, modality-dependent components, impor-

tant for short-term storage of verbal and visuospatial

information, respectively [11]. Crucially, all three com-

ponents of WM can be distinguished from an early age

Introduction [13].

Many aspects of children’s academic skill acquisition

require access to working memory (WM) resources Developmental studies using the Baddeley and Hitch

[1–3]. In no academic domain is this truer than in math- model have predominantly reported a strong link

ematical cognition where problem solving abilities de- between the central executive and visuospatial WM

pend on the capacity to efficiently manipulate quantity components and math abilities [9,14–17] (Simmons



representations in WM [4 ,5]. Over three decades of et al., 2012). The effects of phonological WM have gen-

behavioral research have established that numerical prob- erally been much weaker, and are typically more evident

lem solving tasks place strong demands on the active during very early stages (ages 4–5), when phonological

maintenance and manipulation of task-relevant informa- representations for are still weak and word-

tion in WM [5,6]. Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies based problem solving places greater demands on

are providing new insights into the role of individual comprehension. In a detailed cognitive analysis of the

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132

126 Neuroscience of education

factors that contribute to mathematical abilities, Szucs Longitudinal studies have expanded on these findings

and colleagues found strong relations between visuospa- and shown that the central executive component predicts

tial WM measures, but not phonological WM measures, performance on single-digit addition tasks in grades 1 to

and mathematical abilities in a large well-characterized 3 as well as faster transitions from simple (e.g., counting)

group of 9 year-old children [18]. to sophisticated (e.g., decomposition) solution strategies

Figure 1

(a) Numerical L R (b) Arithmetic

(c) Working Memory

(d) Visuospatial

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

Common patterns of fronto-parietal network activations elicited by numerical, arithmetic, working memory and visuospatial processing tasks.

Results from meta-analysis conducted using Neurosynth (www.neurosynth.org) with the corresponding search terms.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132 www.sciencedirect.com

Working memory in children’s math learning Menon 127

[16]. Similarly, in a large sample of 673 children, Lee and abilities in third graders [17]. Similarly, Li and Geary

Bull found that WM updating capacity in kindergarten reported individual differences in the growth rate of visuo-

predicted growth rate of math abilities (numerical opera- spatial WM during childhood. Notably, they found that

tions) in subsequent grades [19]. these differences became increasingly important for learn-

ing over time [20]. Nuerk and colleagues examined longi-

It is important to note that the role of individual WM tudinal changes associated with multiplication fact retrieval



components depends not only on task complexity but also [21 ]. They found that multiplication task performance was

on children’s developmental stage. The changing role of correlated with verbal WM in third graders but with visuo-

WM components can be detected even in a 1-year time- spatial WM in grade four. Taken together, these patterns of

window between ages 8 and 9. Meyer and colleagues found relationships suggest that the contributions of individual

that while the central executive and phonological compo- WM processes change dynamically over development with

nents of WM predicted mathematical abilities in second visuospatial WM processes playing an increasingly impor-

graders, it was the visuospatial component that predicted tant role in enhancing mathematical proficiency.

Figure 2

SMG IPS

SMG CE

PL VS

Overlap of VS and CE Overlap of PL and CE L SMG L IPS

Z = 34 Y = –43 Z = 54 Y = –47

CE r = .334 CE r = .417 1 VS r = .353 1 PL r = .374 ) ) β β 0 0

Activity ( –1 Activity ( –1

–2 –2 51015 2025 30 5 15 25 35 Working Memory Score Working Memory Score

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

Functional dissociations and overlap between brain areas associated with each of the three components of working memory in 7 to 9-year-old

children (N = 74). The neural correlates of the central executive (CE), phonological (PL) and visuo-spatial (VS) components of working memory

were examined by contrasting brain responses to two different types of addition problems that differed in complexity. Overlap between the CE

and VS components was observed only in left supramarginal gyrus (SMG); overlap between CE and PL components was observed only in the left

intra-parietal sulcus (IPS); no overlap was observed between VS and PL components. Negative correlation between activity and PL ability is not

depicted. No overlap for VS and PL (magenta) was observed. Bottom panel: coronal slices depict regions of interest selected as overlap in

correlations of activity and individual working memory components. Scatter plots are based on functional clusters identified using whole-brain

regression analysis, and are provided for the purpose of visualization. L, left. 

Source: [23 ].

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132

128 Neuroscience of education

Figure 3

(a) (a) TD R AIC L IPS R IPS L MFG L MFG R MFG R MFG

R Visual L IFG Cortex Y = 32 L IPS Z = 10 Precuneus R MFG

R Fusiform Gyrus

Cingulate Gyrus Cerebellum Cerebellum Y = –44 X = 0 Y = –56 X = 40 (b) DD

L Postcentral Gyrus Positive Correlations 2 4

Negative Correlations 2 4

TD > DD (b)

(a) Prefrontal

L IFG R MFG 4 4 ∗∗ 3 r = –.65 3 ∗ 2 r = –.23 2 r = –.29 r = –.60 1 1

T Score 0 T Score 0 –1 –1 –2 –2 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 X = –42 Block Recall Z = 36 Block Recall (b) Parietal

L IPS R SMG 4 4 3 ∗ 3 ∗ 2 r = .55 r = –.50 r = –.21 2 r = –.15 1 1

T Score 0 T Score 0 –1 –1 –2 –2 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 Block Recall Z = 20 Z = 36 Block Recall DD 2 4 TD

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

Children with dyscalculia do not use visuospatial working memory resources appropriately during arithmetic problem solving. (A) Brain areas in

which activity during arithmetic problem solving was significantly correlated with visuo-spatial working memory abilities in the typically developing

(TD) and developmental dyscalculia (DD) groups. (a) In the TD group, Block Recall, a measure of visuo-spatial working memory, was correlated

with activity in bilateral middle frontal gyrus (MFG), left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right anterior insula (AIC), anterior, middle and posterior

cingulate cortex and precuneus, bilateral (IPS), right fusiform gyrus, left temporal pole and the cerebellum. No negative

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132 www.sciencedirect.com

Working memory in children’s math learning Menon 129

Working memory and fronto-parietal systems the fractionation of neurofunctional systems associated

associated with children’s mathematical with distinct WM components during numerical problem



cognition solving [23 ]. Analysis of the relation between the central

Functional neuroimaging research has revealed signifi- executive, phonological and visuospatial components of

cant overlap in multiple parietal and prefrontal cortex WM and brain activation during an arithmetic verification

regions involved in WM and numerical problem solving task in a large (N = 74) group of 7 to 9-year-old children

[22–24]. Overlapping patterns of activation have most revealed that visuospatial WM is the strongest predictor

prominently been detected in the supramarginal gyrus of mathematical ability in children in this age group and is

and intraparietal sulcus in the posterior parietal cortex, associated with increased arithmetic complexity-related

the premotor cortex, and the ventral and dorsal aspects responses in left dorsolateral and right ventrolateral pre-

of the lateral prefrontal cortex (Figure 1). It is impor- frontal cortices as well as in the bilateral intra-parietal

tant to note, however, that the common patterns of sulcus and supramarginal gyrus in posterior parietal cortex

fronto-parietal cortex engagement during WM and nu- (Figure 2). This neurobiological finding confirms a pivotal

merical problem solving cannot be conflated with role of visuospatial WM during arithmetic problem-solv-

shared neural mechanisms, and research on this topic ing in primary-school children.

has used both correlational and causative analyses

to gain a deeper understanding of the shared neural Metcalfe and colleagues also found that visuospatial WM

mechanisms [25]. and the central executive component were associated

with largely distinct patterns of brain responses during

Neuroimaging studies in typical and atypical develop- arithmetic problem-solving, and overlap was only ob-

ment are helping to provide a more mechanistic under- served in the ventral aspects of the left supramarginal

standing of the link between individual WM components gyrus in the posterior parietal cortex, suggesting that this

and brain responses associated with mathematical prob- region is an important locus for the integration of infor-

lem solving. The involvement of WM in mathematical mation in WM during numerical problem solving [29,31–

cognition had initially been surmised based on overlap- 35].

ping responses in posterior parietal cortex and prefrontal

cortex in the two domains [26–29]. Studies of typical Finally, there is also evidence that immature prefrontal

development provided initial evidence for the changing control systems associated with central executive func-

role of WM with age. For example, Rivera and colleagues tions may contribute to weaker math skills in children.

found that relative to adolescents and young adults, Supekar and colleagues used dynamic causal analysis to

children engage the posterior parietal cortex less, and probe interactions between the prefrontal and parietal

the prefrontal cortex more, when solving arithmetic pro- cortices in children and adults [36]. They found that

blems [29], likely reflecting the increased role of visuo- despite higher levels of activation, the strength of causal

spatial WM processes, and concurrent decrease in regulatory influences from the fronto-insular control net-

demands on cognitive control with age. Other studies work to the posterior parietal cortex was significantly

have more directly addressed the link between WM weaker in children and weak signaling mechanisms con-

abilities and numerical problem solving skills. tributed to lower levels of performance in children, com-

Dumontheil and Klingberg [30] found that activity in pared to adults. More broadly, immature prefrontal

the intraparietal sulcus during a visuospatial WM task control systems may contribute to weaknesses in the

predicted arithmetic performance two years later in a ability to inhibit irrelevant information such as arithmetic

sample of 6- to 16-year-old children and adolescents. facts or operations during numerical problem solving

 

This finding further reinforces the link between visuo- [4 ,37,38,39 ].

spatial WM and numerical problem solving and suggests a

common underlying process in the intraparietal sulcus Working memory disruption in children with

subdivision of the posterior parietal cortex. dyscalculia

Studies of children with dyscalculia provide a unique

More detailed analyses of the neural correlates of indi- window into the role of WM in .

vidual components of WM have provided evidence for Dyscalculia is a specific deficit in arithmetic ability in

(Figure 3 continued) correlations were observed in the TD group. (b) In the DD group, Block Recall was negatively correlated with activity in left

postcentral gyrus. No positive correlations were observed in the DD group. (B) Fronto-parietal cortical areas where the relation between activity

during arithmetic problem solving and visuo-spatial working memory abilities differed significantly between the TD and DD groups. (a) Prefrontal

cortex. In TD children, left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and right middle frontal gyrus (MFG) showed significant positive correlation between

activation during Complex addition problems and Block Recall, a measure of visuo-spatial working memory. In contrast, correlations were

nonsignificant in children with DD. (b) Parietal cortex. In TD children, the left intra-parietal sulcus (IPS), and right supramarginal gyrus (SMG)

showed significant positive correlation between activation during arithmetic problem solving and Block Recall. In the DD group there were no

significant correlations (*P < .05, **P < .01). 

Source: [52 ].

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132

130 Neuroscience of education

the presence of preserved intellectual and verbal abilities and dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices were

[40–43]. Children with dyscalculia show poor perfor- positively correlated with visuospatial WM ability in

mance on a broad range of numerical tasks, including typically developing children, but no such relation was

magnitude judgment [44–47] and enumeration seen in children with dyscalculia. This result suggests



[4 ,48,49]. They also lag behind their typically develop- that children with dyscalculia fail to appropriately exploit

ing peers in basic arithmetic problem solving skills visuospatial WM resources during problem solving. While



[4 ,50]. There is growing evidence that deficits in still preliminary, extant findings point to the intraparietal

WM can contribute to multiple aspects of dyscalculia, sulcus as a common locus of visuospatial WM deficits and

encompassing not only complex arithmetic problem solv- arithmetic problem solving deficits in children with dys-

 

ing but also basic quantity representation [4 ,51 ]. calculia. On the basis of these and other related findings,

we have suggested that parietal cortex mechanisms for

Multiple experimental paradigms across extended peri- storing and manipulating quantity representations are

  

ods of early skill acquisition in the domains of number impaired in dyscalculia [23 ,55 ,56 ].

sense and arithmetic have highlighted the involvement of

visuospatial WM in developmental models of dyscalculia. Conclusion

At a more fundamental level, deficits in visuospatial WM WM plays an integral role in children’s math learning and

can influence the ability to engage and manipulate repre- development of problem solving abilities. The role of

sentations of magnitude on a mental number line and individual WM components in mathematical cognition is

estimate non-symbolic quantity. Furthermore, other learning-stage dependent, both in terms of proficiency and

areas of difficulty that define the profile of children with age. Behavioral and neuroimaging studies are converging

dyscalculia, such as counting and subitizing, may have on the idea that the contributions of individual WM



their roots in visuospatial WM deficits [4 ,49]. Conver- processes and their neural substrates change dynamically

gent with these observations, several lines of evidence over development, with visuospatial WM processes play-

point to disruptions in visuospatial WM in children with ing an increasingly important role in learning and enhanc-

dyscalculia. Even when they are matched with typically ing mathematical proficiency. Although the role of the

developing children on general intelligence, reading and visuospatial component of WM has often been considered

other cognitive measures, children with dyscalculia dem- secondary to that of the central executive component in

onstrate lower visuospatial WM despite preserved pho- typical arithmetic skill acquisition, and has generally been



nological and central executive WM abilities [52 ]. neglected in prior accounts of dyscalculia and math learn-

Furthermore, Swanson et al. [53] found deficits in visuo- ing disabilities, recent studies suggest that visuospatial

spatial, but not in other WM components, differentiating WM is a critical component for successfully building

children with dyscalculia from children with reading quantity representations and efficiently manipulating

difficulties. Consistent with these findings, Rotzer et al. them during problem solving. These processes are impor-

[54] found that children with dyscalculia had lower scores tant at all stages of learning and skill acquisition, and are

than typically developing children on a Corsi Block- significantly disrupted in children with dyscalculia.

Tapping Test. Thus, visuospatial WM deficits appear

to be a specific source of mathematical difficulty in Phonological WM appears most prominently in the earli-

dyscalculia. est stages of learning the verbal mapping of quantity

representations and later gives way to visuospatial WM

Visuospatial working memory and fronto- processes important for the representation and manipu-

parietal impairments in children with lation of quantity information in short-term memory. The

dyscalculia central executive system helps scaffold the early stages of

The importance of visuospatial WM and associated learning by providing support for building new semantic

fronto-parietal processing during arithmetic problem- representations. The central executive component is also

solving is further highlighted by neuroimaging studies required at subsequent stages for more complex problem

in children with dyscalculia. Rotzer et al. [54] found that solving procedures, including the active maintenance of

compared to typically developing children, children with intermediate results and rule-based problem solving.

low math abilities had lower visuospatial abilities and

lower activity levels in the right anterior intraparietal Within the neurocognitive framework highlighted in this

sulcus, inferior frontal gyrus, and insular cortex during review, the engagement of the intraparietal sulcus and



a visuospatial WM task. Ashkenazi and colleagues [52 ] supramarginal gyrus in the posterior parietal cortex, and

identified impaired WM components in children with the ventral and dorsal aspects of the lateral prefrontal

dyscalculia and then examined their role in modulating cortex changes dynamically with problem complexity and

brain responses to numerical problem solving (Figure 3). developmental stage. Findings to date suggest that the

Children with dyscalculia had specific deficits in visuo- intraparietal sulcus plays an essential role not only in

spatial WM in addition to deficits in arithmetic task quantity representations but also in maintaining quantity-

performance. Crucially, activations in intraparietal sulcus, related information in short-term WM. Rule-based

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132 www.sciencedirect.com

Working memory in children’s math learning Menon 131

Review addressing challenges of defining and diagnosing dyscalculia,

manipulation of these representations in WM is in turn

emphasizing its heterogeneous nature and developmental sequela.

supported by multiple prefrontal cortical areas, with the

8. Bull R, Lee K: Executive functioning and

supramarginal gyrus as a key locus for integrating frontal

achievement. Child Dev Perspect 2014, 8(1):36-41.

control systems with quantity representations supported

9. Bailey DH, Littlefield A, Geary DC: The codevelopment of skill at

by the intraparietal sulcus. Together, they provide mul-

and preference for use of retrieval-based processes for

tiple functional circuits that support essential WM pro- solving addition problems: individual and sex differences from

first to sixth grades. J Exp Child Psychol 2012, 113(1):78-92.

cesses in children’s mathematical cognition.

10. Geary DC, Hoard M, Nugent L, Byrd-Craven J, Berch D,

Mazzocco M: Strategy use, long-term memory, and working

A challenging question for future research is to under-

memory capacity. In Why is math so hard for some children?.

stand how WM processes are used dynamically to support Edited by Anonymous. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing

Co.; 2007:83-105.

different types of mathematical learning and how they

change with different stages of development. Addressing 11. Baddeley AD: Working memory: theories, models, and

controversies. Annu Rev Psychol 2012, 63:1-29.

this question will require developing appropriate compu-

12. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ: Working memory. In The psychology of

tational models of dynamic causal interactions between

learning and motivation: advances in research and theory. Edited

brain regions, analyzing different stages of information by Bower GH. New York: Academic; 1974:47-89.

processing, and utilizing more appropriate experimental

13. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H: The

designs that involve the controlled manipulation of quan- structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev

Psychol 2004, 40(2):177-190.

tity representations in WM [57]. Finally, training studies

also have the potential to inform causal links between 14. Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA: Short-term memory, working

 memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers:

WM processing and mathematical learning [55 ].

longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age

7 years. Dev Neuropsychol 2008, 33(3):205-228.

Conflict of interest statement

15. De Smedt B, Janssen R, Bouwens K, Verschaffel L, Boets B,

Nothing declared. Ghesquiere P: Working memory and individual differences in

mathematics achievement: a longitudinal study from first

grade to second grade. J Exp Child Psychol 2009, 103(2):186-

Acknowledgements 201.

It is a pleasure to thank Teresa Iuculano, Rachel Rehert and Se Ri Bae for

16. Geary DC, Hoard MK, Nugent L: Independent contributions of

valuable feedback and careful proof-reading, and Se Ri Bae for assistance

the central executive, intelligence, and in-class attentive

with the figures. I also thank two anonymous reviewers for valuable feedback.

behavior to developmental change in the strategies used to

solve addition problems. J Exp Child Psychol 2012, 113(1):49-65.

References and recommended reading

17. Meyer ML, Salimpoor VN, Wu SS, Geary DC, Menon V:

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review,

Differential contribution of specific working memory

have been highlighted as:

components to mathematics achievement in 2nd and 3rd

graders. Learn Individ Differ 2010, 20(2):101-109.

 of special interest

 of outstanding interest 18. Szucs D, Devine A, Soltesz F, Nobes A, Gabriel F: Cognitive

components of a mathematical processing network in 9-year-

old children. Dev Sci 2014, 17(4):506-524.

1. Alloway TP, Alloway RG: Investigating the predictive roles of

working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J Exp Child

19. Lee K, Bull R: Developmental changes in working memory.

Psychol 2010, 106(1):20-29.

Updat Math Achiev 2015.

2. Passolunghi MC, Vercelloni B, Schadee H: The precursors of

20. Li Y, Geary DC: Developmental gains in visuospatial memory

mathematics learning: working memory, phonological ability

predict gains in mathematics achievement. PLOS ONE 2013,

and numerical competence. Cognit Dev 2007, 22(2):165-184.

8(7):e70160.

3. Raghubar KP, Barnes MA, Hecht SA: Working memory and

21. Soltanlou M, Pixner S, Nuerk HC: Contribution of working

mathematics: a review of developmental, individual

 memory in multiplication fact network in children may shift

difference, and cognitive approaches. Learn Individ Differ 2010,

20(2):110-122. from verbal to visuo-spatial: a longitudinal investigation. Front

Psychol 2015:6.

4. Geary DC: Early foundations for mathematics learning and Longitudinal study reveals that the contributions of individual working

 their relations to learning disabilities. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013, memory processes change dynamically over time with visuospatial work-

22(1):23-27. ing memory processes playing an increasingly important role during the

Review examines whether quantitative learning is supported by an later stages of learning.

inherent sense of magnitude, an attentional-control system for proces-

22. Arsalidou M, Taylor MJ: Is 2 + 2 = 4? Meta-analyses of brain

sing mathematical symbols, or logical problem solving abilities. Suggests

areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage 2011,

that each of these competencies are involved, but to different degrees

54(3):2382-2393.

and at different points in the learning process.

23. Metcalfe AWS, Rosenberg-Lee M, Ashkenazi S, Menon V:

5. LeFevre JA, DeStefano D, Coleman B, Shanahan T: Mathematical

 Fractionating the neural correlates of individual working

cognition and working memory. In Handbook of mathematical

memory components underlying problem solving skills in

cognition. Edited by Campbell JID. New York: Psychology Press;

2005:361-395. young children. Dev Cognit Neurosci (Under revision) 2013:135.

Identifies distinct patterns of fronto-parietal response associated with

6. Hitch GJ: Role of short-term working memory in mental visuospatial, central executive, and phonological components of WM.

arithmetic. Cognit Psychol 1978, 10(3):302-323. Visuospatial working memory identified as the strongest predictor of

mathematical abilities in children.

7. Kaufmann L, Mazzocco MM, Dowker A, von Aster M, Go¨ bel SM,

 Grabner RH, Henik A, Jordan NC, Karmiloff-Smith AD, Kucian K, 24. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB,

Rubinsten O, Szucs D, Shalev R, Nuerk H-C: Dyscalculia from a Fox PT, Eickhoff SB: Modelling neural correlates of working

developmental and differential perspective. Front Psychol memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage 2012,

2013, 4:516. 60(1):830-846.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132

132 Neuroscience of education

25. Menon V: Arithmetic in the child and adult brain. Oxford University; 44. Geary DC, Hamson CO, Hoard MK: Numerical and arithmetical

2015. cognition: a longitudinal study of process and concept deficits

in children with . J Exp Child Psychol 2000,

26. Cantlon JF, Libertus ME, Pinel P, Dehaene S, Brannon EM, 77(3):236-263.

Pelphrey KA: The neural development of an abstract concept of

number. J Cognit Neurosci 2009, 21(11):2217-2229. 45. Mussolin C, De Volder A, Grandin C, Schlogel X, Nassogne MC,

Noe¨ l MP: Neural correlates of symbolic number comparison in

27. Grabner RH, Ansari D, Koschutnig K, Reishofer G, Ebner F,

developmental dyscalculia. J Cogn Neurosci 2010, 22(5):860-

Neuper C: To retrieve or to calculate? Left 874.

mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem

solving. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47(2):604-608.

46. Piazza M, Facoetti A, Trussardi AN, Berteletti I, Conte S,

Lucangeli D, Dehaene S, Zorzi M: Developmental trajectory of

28. Ischebeck A, Zamarian L, Egger K, Schocke M, Delazer M:

number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental

Imaging early practice effects in arithmetic. NeuroImage 2007,

dyscalculia. Cognition 2010, 116(1):33-41.

36(3):993-1003.

47. Price GR, Holloway I, Rasanen P, Vesterinen M, Ansari D:

29. Rivera SM, Reiss AL, Eckert MA, Menon V: Developmental

Impaired parietal magnitude processing in developmental

changes in mental arithmetic: evidence for increased

dyscalculia. Curr Biol 2007, 17(24):R1042-R1043.

functional specialization in the left inferior parietal cortex.

Cereb Cortex 2005, 15(11):1779-1790.

48. Landerl K, Bevan A, Butterworth B: Developmental dyscalculia

and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old

30. Dumontheil I, Klingberg T: Brain activity during a visuospatial

students. Cognition 2004, 93(2):99-125.

working memory task predicts arithmetical performance

2 years later. Cereb Cortex 2012, 22(5):1078-1085.

49. Schleifer P, Landerl K: Subitizing and counting in typical and

31. Ansari D: Effects of development and enculturation on number atypical development. Dev Sci 2011, 14(2):280-291.

representation in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008,

9(4):278-291. 50. Shalev R, Manor O, Gross-Tsur V: Developmental dyscalculia: a

prospective six-year follow-up. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005,

32. Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L: Three parietal circuits for 47(2):121-125.

number processing. Cognit Neuropsychol 2003, 20(3/4/5/6):487-

506. 51. Fias W, Menon V, Szucs D: Multiple components of

 developmental dyscalculia. Trends Educ Neurosci 2013, 2(2):43-

33. Kucian K, von Aster M, Loenneker T, Dietrich T, Martin E: 47.

Development of neural networks for exact and approximate Provides a fresh perspective on unresolved controversies regarding the

calculation: a FMRI study. Dev Neuropsychol 2008, functional impairments at the origin of dyscalculia, including working

33(4):447-473. memory, approximate number system and attention. Argues for a neu-

rocognitive network framework involving multiple functional components

34. Menon V, Rivera SM, White CD, Eliez S, Glover GH, Reiss AL:

that contribute to inefficient numerical problem solving and dyscalculia.

Functional optimization of arithmetic processing in perfect

performers. Cognit Brain Res 2000, 9(3):343-345.

52. Ashkenazi S, Rosenberg-Lee M, Metcalfe AW, Swigart AG,

 Menon V: Visuo-spatial working memory is an important

35. Rosenberg-Lee M, Barth M, Menon V: What difference does a

source of domain-general vulnerability in the development of

year of schooling make? Maturation of brain response and

arithmetic cognition. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51(11):2305-2317.

connectivity between 2nd and 3rd grades during arithmetic

Demonstrates that children with dyscalculia do not use visuospatial

problem solving. NeuroImage 2011.

working memory resources appropriately during problem solving, and

identifies the neural correlates of this deficit.

36. Supekar K, Menon V: Developmental maturation of dynamic

causal control signals in higher-order cognition: a

53. Swanson HL, Howard CB, Saez L: Do different components of

neurocognitive network model. PLoS Comput Biol 2012,

8(2):e1002374. working memory underlie different subgroups of reading

disabilities? J Learn Disabil 2006, 39(3):252-269.

37. Chen Z, Siegler RS: Intellectual development in childhood. In

54. Rotzer S, Loenneker T, Kucian K, Martin E, Klaver P, von Aster M:

Handbook of intelligence. Edited by Sternberg RJ. Cambridge:

Dysfunctional neural network of spatial working memory

Cambridge University Press; 2000:92-116.

contributes to developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychologia

38. Cho S, Metcalfe AWS, Young CB, Ryali S, Geary DC, Menon V: 2009, 47(13):2859-2865.

Hippocampal-prefrontal engagement and dynamic causal

interactions in the maturation of children’s fact retrieval. J 55. Iuculano T, Rosenberg-Lee M, Richardson J, Tenison C, Fuchs L,

Cognit Neurosci 2012, 24(9):1849-1866.  Supekar K, Menon V: Cognitive tutoring induces widespread

neuroplasticity and remediates brain function in children with

39. Szucs D, Devine A, Soltesz F, Nobes A, Gabriel F: Developmental mathematical learning disabilities. Nat Commun 2015:6.

 dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition First demonstration that cognitive training in children with mathematical

impairment. Cortex 2013, 49(10):2674-2688. learning disabilities can normalize aberrant responses in fronto-parietal

Identifies weaknesses in visuospatial working memory and inhibition responses that support problem solving and working memory.

control as key cognitive factors contributing to developmental dyscalcu-

lia. 56. Jolles DD, Ashkenazi S, Kochalka J, Evans T, Richardson J,

 Rosenberg-Lee M, Zhao H, Supekar K, Chen T, Menon V: Parietal

40. Butterworth B, Varma S, Laurillard D: Dyscalculia: from brain to

hyper-connectivity, aberrant brain organization, and circuit-

education. Science 2011, 332(6033):1049-1053.

based biomarkers in children with mathematical disabilities.

Dev Sci 2016. (in press).

41. Rubinsten O, Henik A: Developmental dyscalculia:

Identifies hyper-connectivity of the intraparietal sulcus with multiple

heterogeneity might not mean different mechanisms. Trends

frontal and parietal areas, as well as higher levels of spontaneous low

Cognit Sci 2009, 13(2):92-99.

frequency fluctuations in fronto-parietal cortex, in children with dyscal-

culia. Provides novel evidence for the view that dyscalculia is a network-

42. Szu˝ cs D, Goswami U: Developmental dyscalculia: fresh

level deficit.

perspectives. Trends Neurosci Educ 2013, 2(2):33-37.

43. von Aster M, Shalev R: Number development and 57. Luck SJ, Vogel EK: Visual working memory capacity: from

developmental dyscalculia. Dev Med Child Neurol 2007, psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences.

49(11):868-873. Trends Cognit Sci 2013, 17(8):391-400.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2016, 10:125–132 www.sciencedirect.com