Putting Beauty Back in the Eye of the Beholder

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Putting Beauty Back in the Eye of the Beholder Presidents’ Award Putting beauty back in the eye of the beholder ANTHONY LITTLE and DAVID PERRETT (winner of the 2000 Presidents’ Award for Distinguished Contributions to Psychological Knowledge) discuss evolution and individual differences in face preference. UR magazines and television This problem with considering beauty evolutionary theory, which posits that the screens are filled with images as only in the eye of the beholder is nicely attractiveness of individuals is directly O of ‘attractive’ people, and it is illustrated in Figure 1. Here, the two faces linked to their value as mates. High-value obvious that both women and men are are both symmetric and have perfectly mates are those who can best enhance the highly concerned with good looks in smooth skin, yet the composite on the right reproductive success of the individual who a partner. But exactly what is it that makes made from the faces of 50 models is chooses them. Individuals who were a face beautiful? What makes people seek systematically different from the composite attentive to cues to high mate-value, and out and desire to mate with the owners of student face in both shape and coloration. based mate-choice decisions on these cues, beautiful faces? Most people will agree it is more attractive. left behind more offspring (and so more It is difficult to verbalise what Some people are beautiful and some people genes for attending to attractive cues) than differentiates average from good-looking are not, and most people agree on who is those who failed to attend to these cues. individuals, and in modern times there is and who is not beautiful. Such evolutionary reasoning can also be a pervasive view that attractiveness simply Agreement between individuals is in applied to humans. In essence, the cannot be defined. Common phrases such fact one of the best-documented and most evolutionary view suggests that when as ‘beauty is in the eye of the beholder’ and robust findings in facial attractiveness making attractiveness decisions we are all, ‘beauty is only skin deep’ are testaments to research since the 1970s. Across many albeit unconsciously, asking the question our belief that attractiveness is ephemeral. studies it has been found that there is ‘What can the owners of beautiful faces do For example, the philosopher David Hume a high degree of agreement from for us in order that our genes survive to is often quoted for making the argument individuals within a particular culture, and subsequent generations?’ that beauty ‘is no quality in things high agreement between individuals from In many studies this evolutionary view themselves: it exists merely in the mind different cultures (see Langlois et al., 2000, of attractiveness has been powerful in which contemplates them; and each mind for a meta-analytic review). If different predicting the specific characteristics of contemplates a different beauty’ (Hume, people can agree on which faces are attractive faces (see Thornhill & 1757, p.208–209). attractive and which faces are not attractive Gangestad, 1999, for a review). A central But something in this politically when judging faces of varying ethnic tenet of the evolutionary view is that correct view of beauty just does not ring background, then this suggests that people individuals should agree on the true. Admittedly the latest big movie star everywhere are all using the same, or at characteristics that make up attractiveness is not everyone’s favourite pin-up; but it is least similar, criteria in their judgements. (e.g. Langlois et al., 2000). For example, undeniable that on average Hollywood males who preferred women past stars are generally more attractive than Evolution and attractiveness reproductive age would not have left their the people we meet in the street. You may Physical appearance is important to genes in the next generation and so we disagree over your best friend’s choice of humans and there appear to be certain partner but there are countless individuals features that are found attractive across FIGURE 1 that you and your friend could agree were individuals and cultures. The same holds more or less attractive than the particular true across the animal kingdom: most partner. non-human species rely on external factors, such as the sizes, shape and colour of adornments (e.g. feathers, fur, and fins) to attract mates. Research on animals has WEBLINK focused on individual traits that are For information on experiments and to attractive across individuals (and even participate in ongoing research: species) such as symmetry (e.g. Møller www.perceptionlab.com & Thornhill, 1998). Such experiments are driven by 28 The Psychologist Vol 15 No 1 January 2002 Presidents’ Award might expect a universal preference for FIGURE 2 assortative mating occurs when individuals youth in males. form pairs in which the individuals Proposed universal preferences are involved are more similar to each other consistent with the cross-cultural than would be expected by chance (Burley, agreement on attractiveness. For example, 1983). In humans, mating with similar youth, which is associated with fertility and individuals to oneself may have genetic the potential for offspring production in benefits. For example, Thiessen and Gregg women, has been found to be attractive in (1980) have proposed that mating with faces across individuals and cultures (Buss, similar-looking individuals increases the 1989). We will now look in more detail at chances that those individuals have genes another example of a universal in common with you, and that mating with characteristic of beauty and its proposed such individuals can be advantageous to evolutionary root. your genes. Of course the advantages of mating with similar individuals has its Facial symmetry limits. Mating with your close family Research on symmetry has been may be a range of mating strategies members is referred to as inbreeding, conducted on many animal species and that can be employed based on both which reduces genetic diversity and provides a good example of a characteristic environmental cues and individual physical increases the chances of harmful recessive of human faces that is proposed to be attributes. In humans, whilst individuals genes being expressed in offspring. The attractive across observers. Symmetry may share certain basic criteria for finding best strategy might be to avoid mating with refers to the extent that the left half of an faces attractive, many factors may close relatives but to mate with someone organism is the same as the right. It is influence the specific types of face they genetically more similar to you than a significant characteristic, as it can be seen find attractive. average. Bateson (1982) called this notion as a measure of the ability of an organism optimal outbreeding and demonstrated its to cope with environmental stress (e.g. food Look-alikes occurrence in Japanese quail. Birds of both scarcity): the optimal developmental It is a widespread belief that partners look sexes were attracted to first cousins over outcome for most characteristics is alike (e.g. the 1930 portrait by Grant Wood siblings and unrelated individuals. symmetry. Only high-quality individuals called American Gothic, below). Positive Early research on assortative mating in can maintain symmetric development under environmental and genetic stress; therefore symmetry can serve as an indicator of the quality of an individual as well as the quality of their genes (see Møller & Thornhill, 1998). For example, individuals with genes coding for strong immune function will be more resistant to infection, allowing their bodies to grow more symmetrically than individuals with lower immune capacity, who will be ‘stressed’ more by exposure to the same infection. When manipulating symmetry with computer graphics, symmetric faces are found to be preferred over less symmetric faces (e.g. the symmetric faces on the bottom row of Figure 2) (Perrett et al., 1999; Rhodes et al., 1998). Measuring symmetry in real faces reveals a positive correlation with rated attractiveness (e.g. Grammer & Thornhill, 1994; Mealy et al., 1999; Penton-Voak et al., 2001). Individual differences Having argued for the universality of attractive traits, we now examine some factors that may lead to individual differences in the perception of facial attractiveness and speculate on how such differences may arise from learning and differences in life history. Across the animal kingdom not all members of a given species engage in the same mating behaviour. Indeed, within a species there Birds of a feather? 29 January 2002 The Psychologist Vol 15 No 1 Presidents’ Award humans focused on correlations in physical Research thus suggests that awareness tendency to develop a sexual aversion to characteristics between partners, such as of one’s own traits may encourage choice individuals with whom they live closely arm length. Reviews show an overall of partners similar to oneself. Seeking out in infancy and early childhood (usually pattern of low positive correlations (0.01 a partner who is similar to you, however, is siblings and parents). Originally proposed to 0.35) for many physical features (e.g. also consistent with attraction to parental as a mechanism for avoiding sibling incest, Spuhler, 1968). Married partners’ faces also traits, as usually children physically such negative imprinting would prevent resemble each other in ways that allow resemble their parents. This leads us to inbreeding in a population. Westermarck’s them to be identified as partners, possibly consider the influence of parental hypothesis has received empirical support indicating assortative mating for similar characteristics on mate preferences in from ethnographic studies where male and face shapes (e.g. Hinsz, 1989). humans. female non-siblings are raised together in Partners also appear to assort for many a way similar to real siblings (e.g. Shepher, non-physical traits such as religion, Attraction to parental traits 1971). Such individuals avoided marriage educational level and personality (Botwin In humans there have been few studies on or sexual relations with those with whom et al., 1997; Thiessen & Gregg, 1980).
Recommended publications
  • THE DESCENT of MADNESS: Evolutionary Origins of Psychosis
    THE DESCENT OF MADNESS Drawing on evidence from across the behavioural and natural sciences, this book advances a radical new hypothesis: that madness exists as a costly consequence of the evolution of a sophisticated social brain in Homo sapiens. Having explained the rationale for an evolutionary approach to psych- osis, the author makes a case for psychotic illness in our living ape relatives, as well as in human ancestors. He then reviews existing evolutionary theor- ies of psychosis, before introducing his own thesis: that the same genes causing madness are responsible for the evolution of our highly social brain. Jonathan Burns’ novel Darwinian analysis of the importance of psychosis for human survival provides some meaning for this form of suffering. It also spurs us on to a renewed commitment to changing our societies in a way that allows the mentally ill the opportunity of living. The Descent of Madness will be of interest to those in the fields of psychiatry, psychology, sociology and anthropology, and is also accessible to the general reader. Jonathan Burns is chief specialist psychiatrist at the Nelson Mandela School of Medicine. His main areas of research include psychotic illnesses, human brain evolution and evolutionary origins of psychosis. THE DESCENT OF MADNESS Evolutionary Origins of Psychosis and the Social Brain Jonathan Burns First published 2007 by Routledge 27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex BN3 2FA Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada by Routledge 270 Madison Ave, New York, NY 10016 Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business © 2007 Jonathan Burns This edition published in the Taylor & Francis e-Library, 2007.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Neuroscience
    Chapter 5 Social Cognitive Neuroscience M ATTHEW D . L IEBERMAN Who we are as humans has a lot to do with what happens have become leaders in the field, despite few having pub- between our ears. What happens between our ears has a lot lished social cognitive neuroscience findings at that point. to do with the social world we traverse, engage, and react There were introductory talks on social cognition and cog- to. The former has been the province of neuroscience and nitive neuroscience by Neil Macrae and Jonathan Cohen, the latter the province of social psychology for nearly a respectively, along with symposia on stereotyping (William century. Recently, scientists have begun to study the social Cunningham, Jennifer Eberhardt, Matthew Lieberman, mind by literally looking between the ears using the tools and Wendy Mendes), self - control (Todd Heatherton, Kevin of neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience uses the tools Ochsner, and Cary Savage), emotion (Ralph Adolphs, of neuroscience to study the mental mechanisms that cre- Turhan Canli, Elizabeth Phelps, and Stephanie Preston), ate, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the imitation and social relations (Alan Fiske, Marco Iacoboni, social world. On its worst days, social cognitive neurosci- David Perrett, and Andrew Whiten), and theory of mind ence is phrenological, cataloguing countless brain regions (Chris Ashwin, Josep Call, Vittorio Gallese, and Kevin involved in the vast array of social processes. On its best McCabe). If this meeting represented the first time that all days, social cognitive neuroscience enhances our under- of the ingredients of social cognitive neuroscience were standing of the social mind as well as any other method.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Non-Human Primates in Research in Primates Non-Human of Use The
    The use of non-human primates in research The use of non-human primates in research A working group report chaired by Sir David Weatherall FRS FMedSci Report sponsored by: Academy of Medical Sciences Medical Research Council The Royal Society Wellcome Trust 10 Carlton House Terrace 20 Park Crescent 6-9 Carlton House Terrace 215 Euston Road London, SW1Y 5AH London, W1B 1AL London, SW1Y 5AG London, NW1 2BE December 2006 December Tel: +44(0)20 7969 5288 Tel: +44(0)20 7636 5422 Tel: +44(0)20 7451 2590 Tel: +44(0)20 7611 8888 Fax: +44(0)20 7969 5298 Fax: +44(0)20 7436 6179 Fax: +44(0)20 7451 2692 Fax: +44(0)20 7611 8545 Email: E-mail: E-mail: E-mail: [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Web: www.acmedsci.ac.uk Web: www.mrc.ac.uk Web: www.royalsoc.ac.uk Web: www.wellcome.ac.uk December 2006 The use of non-human primates in research A working group report chaired by Sir David Weatheall FRS FMedSci December 2006 Sponsors’ statement The use of non-human primates continues to be one the most contentious areas of biological and medical research. The publication of this independent report into the scientific basis for the past, current and future role of non-human primates in research is both a necessary and timely contribution to the debate. We emphasise that members of the working group have worked independently of the four sponsoring organisations. Our organisations did not provide input into the report’s content, conclusions or recommendations.
    [Show full text]
  • Facial Imitation Improves Emotion Recognition in Adults with Different Levels of Sub-Clinical Autistic Traits
    Journal of Intelligence Article Facial Imitation Improves Emotion Recognition in Adults with Different Levels of Sub-Clinical Autistic Traits Andrea E. Kowallik 1,2,3,*, Maike Pohl 3 and Stefan R. Schweinberger 1,2,3,4,5,* 1 Early Support and Counselling Center Jena, Herbert Feuchte Stiftungsverbund, 07743 Jena, Germany 2 Social Potential in Autism Research Unit, Friedrich Schiller University, 07743 Jena, Germany 3 Department of General Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Friedrich Schiller University Jena, Am Steiger 3/Haus 1, 07743 Jena, Germany 4 Michael Stifel Center Jena for Data-Driven and Simulation Science, Friedrich Schiller University, 07743 Jena, Germany 5 Swiss Center for Affective Science, University of Geneva, 1202 Geneva, Switzerland * Correspondence: [email protected] (A.E.K.); [email protected] (S.R.S.); Tel.: +49-(0)-3641-945181 (S.R.S.); Fax: +49-(0)-3641-945182 (S.R.S.) Abstract: We used computer-based automatic expression analysis to investigate the impact of im- itation on facial emotion recognition with a baseline-intervention-retest design. The participants: 55 young adults with varying degrees of autistic traits, completed an emotion recognition task with images of faces displaying one of six basic emotional expressions. This task was then repeated with instructions to imitate the expressions. During the experiment, a camera captured the participants’ faces for an automatic evaluation of their imitation performance. The instruction to imitate enhanced imitation performance as well as emotion recognition. Of relevance, emotion recognition improve- ments in the imitation block were larger in people with higher levels of autistic traits, whereas imitation enhancements were independent of autistic traits.
    [Show full text]
  • A Look Into Receptivity to Casual Sex Based on Gender a Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Divi
    PSYCHOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES ON SEX: A LOOK INTO RECEPTIVITY TO CASUAL SEX BASED ON GENDER A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTERS OF ARTS IN PSYCHOLOGY MAY 2012 By Mercedes E. Tappé Thesis Committee: Kentaro Hayashi, Chairperson Richard Rapson Elaine Hatfield ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Elaine Hatfield for her immense patience and support over the last few years of these studies. She has been instrumental throughout every step of this long research process. Her research and past works were my inspiration for these studies as well as my interest in the areas of love and sex within Social Psychology. I would like to also thank Dr. Richard Rapson for his assistance in the development and, more importantly, with the implementation of the following studies. Lastly, I would like to express my appreciation to Dr. Kentaro Hayashi for the many hours he has put into the research analysis assistance. Without his expertise in statistical analysis and write-up the completion of this research and the thesis would not have been possible. Thank you all for your understanding, support, patience, and research skills. This could not have been done without each of you. Mahalo. ii ABSTRACT In these three studies I attempt to explore reasons behind the differences in receptivity of men and women to dating and sexual offers from perfect strangers and whether responses vary by gender. For Studies 1 and 2, I hypothesized that there would be (1) a main effect for type of request, (2) a main effect for gender, and (3) an interaction between Gender and Type of Request.
    [Show full text]
  • Program Committee
    28thAnnual Meeting June 29 – July 2, 2016 Vancouver, BC, Canada Plenary and Keynote Breakout Talks Rooms Breakout Room Coffee Breaks and Book Display BBQ HBES Opening Night Registration Reception 2 HBES 2016 Welcome to HBES 2016 Welcome to the 28th Annual Human Behavior and Evolution Conference at the Westin Bayshore Hotel in Vancouver, Canada. Douglas College, Simon Fraser University, the University of British Columbia, Kwantlen Polytechnic University and the University of Idaho are excited to be joint hosts of this event. In keeping with the past few years, the conference will begin with a plenary talk on Wednesday afternoon and will end with the Keynote on Saturday night. We have continued the tradition of holding conference- wide New Investigator and Post-Doctoral Competition sessions on Friday afternoon to highlight the work of the next generation of HBES researchers. The poster session will be held on Thursday evening in the Stanley Park Ballroom, with the posters being displayed through Friday (6pm). This year the BBQ falls on Canada Day, so after dinner we will have the pleasure of enjoying the annual fireworks display that will be held at 10:15pm over the harbor behind the hotel. We are also pleased to continue the tradition of hosting a Graduate Student Mentor Lunch on Thursday. Given the enormous interest in one of the mentor sessions: “Evolutionary Psychology: The future of the field”, we have arranged to video-record this panel. For the second year, there will also be a “Women of HBES” gathering on Thursday (6-7:30pm) at The Park at English Bay.
    [Show full text]
  • Webinar Slides
    The Discovery And Reach of Animal Culture Andrew Whiten Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution University of St Andrews CES NIMBios Webinar 2020 The Discovery And Reach of Animal Culture Andrew Whiten Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution University of St Andrews Part 1 - The Discovery of Animal Culture Part 2 - The Reach of Animal Culture “Human beings owe their biological supremacy to the possession of a form of inheritance quite unlike that of other animals: exogenetic or exosomatic inheritance. In this form of heredity information is transmitted from one generation to the next through non-genetic inheritance … in general, by the entire apparatus of culture” Professor Sir Peter Medawar, Nobel Laureate The New York Review of Books, 1977 1999 British Birds, 1949 pre-1935 1947 Social Learning and Cultural Transmission of Birdsong William. H. Thorpe, Nature, 1954, 1958 2008 Papers referenced on ‘bird song dialect’ n = 84 Approx 40% of bird species are songbirds: N ~ 4,000 species see also Primates, 1963 WILD CHIMPANZEE CULTURES discoveries in the twentieth century 1986 three limitations in charts based on published literature • Not every observation is published • Frequency often not recorded – habitual or not? • Absence critical, yet often not published 1992 WILD CHIMPANZEE CULTURES discoveries in the twentieth century 1999 A Two-phase Study 1. Establish a list of potential cultural variants - 65 candidates 2. Classify each as: • Customary – done by all able bodied individuals • Habitual – done repeatedly by several individuals • Absent, with or without ecological explanation Multiple, diverse traditions • Food processing • Tool use • Social behaviour • Grooming style • Courtship Communities display unique arrays of traditions Mesh barrier A Two-Traditions Lift Poke ‘Pan-pipes’ Study Flap Whiten, Horner & de Waal Nature 2005 Chute Will traditions spread from group to group? ? Whiten, A., Spiteri, A.
    [Show full text]
  • ISHE 2010 20Th Biennial Congress of the International Society for Human Ethology
    ISHE 2010 20th Biennial Congress of the International Society for Human Ethology August 1-5, 2010 | Madison, Wisconsin USA What are you looking for? THE PYLE CENTER TABLE OF CONTENTS 02 / 03 WELCOME LETTER 02 / 03 SCHEDULE AT A GLANCE 04 / 05 ISHE 2010 PROGRAM DETAILS 08 / 09 SPECIAL EVENTS & TOURS 48 / 49 HELPFUL RESOURCES 50 / 51 INDEX OF AUTHORS 54 WALKING MAP OF CONGRESS LOCATIONS 58 ISHE 2010 PROGRAM MADISON, WISCONSIN MADISON, Welcome from ISHE! Welcome to the 20th biennial congress of the International Society for Human Ethology! We are especially glad to have the congress in Madison since Russ and his wife Susie retired here and Glenn is an alumnus of the University of Wisconsin. Please forgive us for our expressions of blatant attachment to this wonderful town. Thanks to you researchers, we have a full program of outstanding talks and posters. Our keynote speakers are outstanding scholars whose work represents the range of methods and disciplinary perspectives characteristic of human ethology. David Buss is an evolutionary psychologist who has conducted cross-cultural research on mate choice, jealousy, conflict between the sexes, and other topics. Applying observational methods, Heidi Keller analyzes biocultural influences on the development of attachment. Patricia McConnell is an ethologist who investigates dog behavior and training. Toni Ziegler, a neuroendocrinologist, studies paternal hormones in marmosets and tamarins. The work of this array of speakers neatly represents Tinbergen’s four levels of behavioral analysis: ontogeny, proximate mechanisms, comparative analysis, and function. Planning the congress has been greatly eased by Patti Thompson and Deb Curry of the University’s Pyle Center and their staff, who have been extremely helpful.
    [Show full text]
  • Genes Fit for a Queen by Geoffrey Miller Published As: Miller, GF
    Genes fit for a queen By Geoffrey Miller Published as: Miller, G. F. (2011). Genes fit for a queen: How Kate won her mate. New Scientist, April 27 [This was published just before Kate Middleton wed Prince William, heir to the British throne] Ah, the eugenic thrill of a good wedding! Status weds beauty: a promising start. Royalty weds a good-genes commoner: excellent progress. A 6’3” prince who flies rescue helicopters and shows self-deprecating humor, weds a 5’10” amazon with a good eye for fashion: truly, this is the romance at the end of the rainbow. On April 29, these two fit young humans, Prince William and Kate Middleton, will be wed, and two billion people will watch. What will we see? Honestly, it depends on your sex. A confusing thing about living in a two-sexed species is that certain social rituals have quite different meanings depending on whether you watch them with a male or a female brain. Nowhere is this divide greater than in the realms of courtship and reproduction, where eons of sexual choice have shaped the evolution of human nature. A wedding may be played against a backdrop of gauzy sentimentalism, but beneath all the trimmings – the dress, the flowers, and the finger-buffet – lie the iron imperatives of sexual conflict. The mating minds of men and women have evolved to achieve different ends so it is hardly surprising that we interpret any wedding, let alone this one, in entirely different ways. Male brains will be sparked by Will’s military titles (Flight Lieutenant, Captain in the Blues and Royals, Commodore-in-Chief of Scotland) and Kate’s signals of fertility and fitness (cheekbones, legs, style, humor) to conjure a primal mating scenario – King Kong and Ann Darrow, Genghis Khan lying on the white-bellied daughters of those he has vanquished, that sort of thing.
    [Show full text]
  • A Geographical History of Social Cognitive Neuroscience
    NeuroImage 61 (2012) 432–436 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Review A geographical history of social cognitive neuroscience Matthew D. Lieberman Franz Hall, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA article info abstract Article history: The history of social cognitive neuroscience (SCN) began with isolated islands of research in Europe and the Accepted 15 December 2011 United States in the 1990s. In the decade between 1995 and 2004 most of the major areas of current SCN re- Available online 28 January 2012 search were identified in a series of high profile first studies. This paper reviews the timeline as well as the geography of important moments in the short history of this field. Of note is the different focus seen in Euro- Keywords: pean contributions (theory of mind, mirror neurons, and empathy) and the more self-focused U.S. contribu- Social cognitive neuroscience tions (self-knowledge, emotion regulation, implicit attitudes). Social neuroscience Social cognition © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Contents References ................................................................. 435 This special issue commemorates the twentieth anniversary of focused on the differing contributions of the two cerebral hemispheres the journal Neuroimage and this period covers the vast majority of and in particular how lateralized functions could explain social social cognitive neuroscience's history as well. If one were so moti- psychological peculiarities such as rationalization. Although current vated, the history of social cognitive neuroscience (SCN) could be SCN theories rarely rely on lateralization as a key distinction, this book traced to Phineas Gage in the mid-nineteenth century or to Floyd represents the first modern attempt to explain the emergence of social Allport's (1924) first textbook on social psychology, which included psychological phenomena in terms of the organization or function of the a chapter on the brain bases of social psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Downloads.Shtml] and the Model Is Described in Detail by Itti and Koch
    Gaze selection in the real world: finding evidence for a preferential selection of eyes. by ELINA BIRMINGHAM B. Sc., University of British Columbia, 2002 M. A., University of Toronto, 2003 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES (Psychology) THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA July, 2008 © Elina Birmingham, 2008 Abstract We have a strong intuition that people's eyes are unique, socially informative stimuli. As such, it is reasonable to propose that humans have developed a fundamental tendency to preferentially attend to eyes in the environment. The empirical evidence to support this intuition is, however, remarkably thin. Over the course of eight chapters, the present thesis considers the area of social attention, and what special role (if any) the selection of eyes has in it. Chapters 2 and 3 demonstrate that when observers are shown complex natural scenes, they look at the eyes more frequently than any other region. This selection preference is enhanced when the social content and activity in the scene is high, and when the task is to report on the attentional states in the scene. Chapters 4 and 5 establish that the bias to select eyes extends to a variety of tasks, suggesting that it may be fundamental to human social attention. In addition, Chapter 5 shows that observers who are told that they will have to remember the scenes look more often at the eyes than observers who are unaware of the forthcoming memory test; moreover this difference between groups persists to scene recognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Symbols, Stimulus Equivalence and the Origins of Language
    Behavior and Philosophy, 29, 221-244 (2001). © 2001 Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies SYMBOLS, STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE AND THE ORIGINS OF LANGUAGE Thomas E. Dickins Nottingham Trent University David W. Dickins University of Liverpool ABSTRACT: Recent interest in the origins of language, within the strongly cognitive field of Evolutionary Psychology, has predominantly focused upon the origins of syntax (cf. Hurford, Knight, & Studdert-Kennedy, 1998). However, Ullin Place’s (2000a) theory of the gestural origins of language also addresses the more fundamental issue of the antecedents of symbols, and does so from a behaviorist perspective, stressing the importance of the peculiarly human ability to form stimulus equivalence classes. The rejection by many developmental psychologists of a behaviorist account of language acquisition has led to a modular and distinctly nativist psychology of language (cf. Pinker, 1994, 1997; Pinker & Bloom, 1990). Little has been said about the role or nature of learning mechanisms in the evolution of language. Although Place does not provide any defense of a behaviorist linguistic ontogeny, this is no reason to rule out his phylogenetic speculations. We aim to outline Place’s evolutionarily parsimonious view of symbol origins and their relation to stimulus equivalence. We applaud Ullin Place for bringing symbols into focus within the broader discipline of language origins and suggest that he has raised an interesting set of questions to be discussed in future work. Key words: symbols, stimulus equivalence, learning. The central aim of this paper is to analyze Ullin T. Place’s contribution to the study of symbols and particularly to their evolutionary origins. Place was both a philosopher and a psychologist, but in both fields he was committed to a form of behaviorism that made him somewhat unusual in comparison with many of his colleagues (though by no means all).
    [Show full text]