Risk Aversion and Emotions
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
bs_bs_banner Pacific Economic Review, 19: 3 (2014) pp. 296–312 doi: 10.1111/1468-0106.12067 RISK AVERSION AND EMOTIONS YEN NGUYEN Tilburg University CHARLES N. NOUSSAIR* Tilburg University Abstract. We consider the relationship between emotions and decision-making under risk. Specifi- cally, we examine the emotional correlates of risk-averse decisions. In our experiment, individuals’ facial expressions are monitored with face reading software, as they are presented with risky lotteries. We then correlate these facial expressions with subsequent decisions in risky choice tasks. We find that a more positive emotional state is positively correlated with greater risk taking. The strength of a number of emotions, including fear, happiness, anger and surprise, is positively correlated with risk aversion. JEL Classification Code: C9 1. INTRODUCTION The mechanisms that underpin choice under risk have been a primary focus of research in economics, psychology and neuroscience. The traditional approach in economics has been to postulate the existence of a utility function, possessing the expected utility property, that represents an individual’s preferences, and to note that concavity implies a preference for relatively safe lotteries, while convexity corresponds to a preference for relatively risky lotteries. Neuroscien- tists have documented the physiological correlates of risky decision-making. Psychologists and behavioural economists stress the role of emotions such as happiness or fear in guiding individuals’ choices in risky situations (see e.g. Forgas, 1995; Loewenstein and Lerner, 2003; Coget et al., 2011). The focus of the work reported here is to better understand the connection between emotions, which are ultimately psychological terms describing profiles of physiological responses, and the level of risk aversion exhibited in financial decision-making tasks. We study the physiological manifestation of emotion that accompanies decision-making under risk. We report an experiment in which we record individuals’ facial expressions while they are presented with a series of risky lotteries. We then consider whether the emotional response of an individual correlates with his or her subsequent decisions between risky *Address for Correspondence: Department of Economics,Tilburg University, PO Box 90153 500 LE Tilburg, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]. We thank Adriana Breaban and Dan Nguyen for research assistance. We thank participants at the 2012 Alhambra Experimental Work- shop and the 2012 Xiamen University International Workshop of Experimental Economics. We are grateful to Gary Charness, Shachar Kariv and Sigrid Suetens for comments. We thank the CentER for Economic Research at Tilburg University for funding for the experimental sessions. © 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd RISK AVERSION AND EMOTIONS 297 alternatives. The hypotheses we test originate in previous experimental research and are discussed in Section 2. These hypotheses are that the levels of (i) fear, (ii) happiness, (iii) anger and (iv) overall positive emotion that the decision-maker exhibits when presented with risk lotteries correlate with the level of observed risk aversion in the subsequent decision task. We introduce a new research method, face-reading, to economics. The software we use, Noldus Facereader (Noldus et al., 2005), analyses facial expres- sions, and measures the degree of conformity with the six basic universal emo- tions catalogued by Ekman (2007). These emotions are fear, happiness, anger, disgust, surprise and sadness (Ekman and Friesen, 1986; Ekman, 1994; Izard, 1994).1 They are described as universal because the same facial expressions are associated with these emotions in all cultures, the expressions are common to all primates, and they are identical in blind and sighted people. These properties indicate that the associations are innate rather than the product of social learn- ing. Facereader also registers an overall measure of emotional valence. Unlike questionnaire data, Facereader records participants’ emotional states in nearly real time (Reisenzein, 2000), at a sample rate of five times per second. Compared to other methodologies measuring biological responses, such as Skin conduct- ance response registration and functional magnetic resonance imaging, FaceReader has the advantage that it interprets physiological responses directly in terms of specific emotions. Some previous research has taken a valence-based approach to affect, focus- ing on the impact of an overall negative or positive emotional state (Johnson and Tversky, 1983; Isen, 1997; Capra, 2004). However, as DeSteno et al. (2000), Kugler et al. (2012) and others argue, it is important to distinguish the various emotions that make up overall valence. Therefore, other studies have focused on the role of specific emotions, such as fear (Lopes, 1987), anger and disgust (Fessler et al., 2004). Appraisal theory distinguishes emotions at a more fine-grained level than merely positive or negative (Lerner and Keltner, 2001; Lerner and Tiedens, 2006). We take both approaches here, considering both the explanatory power of overall valence, as well as specific emotions, on risk tolerance. With a few exceptions (e.g. Fessler et al., 2004; Lerner et al., 2004; Bruyneel et al., 2009), studies of the effect of incidental emotions on risk taking have been limited to risk assessments or hypothetical choices, rather than tasks involving real decision consequences. In this study, we use real incentives, in keeping with the conventional approach in experimental economics. We also depart from the previous literature, in that instead of inducing emotions exogenously, we merely measure them and consider how they react to different levels of stimuli over the course of our experimental sessions. 1 Zaman and Shrimpton-Smith (2006) compare data measuring individuals’ emotional states from three sources: the results from respondent questionnaires, interviewer loggings of respondent emo- tions and FaceReader data from respondent facial expressions. They found that FaceReader output was highly correlated with the other two measures. They note that FaceReader has the advantage that it operates in real time and can, therefore, register very brief emotional episodes and rapid changes. © 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd 298 Y. NGUYEN AND C. N. NOUSSAIR It is important to emphasize the distinction between emotions and moods. Several features distinguish the two concepts. Emotions are shorter-lived, more intense and typically reactions to specific circumstances (Capra, 2004). Moods, in contrast, are longer in duration, less intense and not usually linked to a proximate event. Some emotions express themselves in distinctive facial expres- sions, while moods do not (Ekman, 1994).2 The issue at hand in the study reported here is whether emotional reactions to specific stimuli correlate with subsequent decisions. Section 2 describes the experiment and Section 3 lays out our hypotheses. Section 4 reports the results and Section 5 contains some concluding remarks. 2. THE EXPERIMENT The 30 participants in our study were Tilburg University students who were recruited via online self-enrolment for the experiments through the CenterLab website at Tilburg University. The sample consisted of 13 men and 17 women, all aged between 18 and 25 years. Each session consisted of four segments. Only one subject participated in each session. The experiment was implemented using the online VeconLab software, developed at the University of Virginia, with modified instructions. The first and second segments each consisted of 10 trials. In the first segment, individuals were presented with a payoff level in each trial. The level was either: 1, 3.5, 6, 8.5 or 11 euros in a given trial.3 Each value appeared exactly twice within the 10-period segment. We average the response across the 2 identical trials. This is a typical procedure in neuroeconomic studies involving physiological measures. It reduces the influence of noise, here in the form of incidental face movements, on the data. Such one-off facial move- ments can greatly affect an individual trial, but have less of an impact if multi- ple trials are averaged. Each subject was required to wait for 30 seconds after the payoff level for the current trial appeared on his or her screen. After the 30 seconds had elapsed, the subject could click on a button and proceed to the next trial. The second segment, which also consisted of 10 trials, was similar, except that the individual was presented with various risky lotteries. Each lottery yielded 6+ε euros with probability 0.5 and 6 – ε euros with probability 0.5. The value of ε varied between 1 and 5 by trial, with each value appearing twice within the 10 trials. Thus, in each trial the individual faced a lottery with an expected value of 6 euro, but with a variance that differed across trials. In both the first and the second segments, the stimuli were presented in a random order that differed by subject. 2 For instance, when many facial expressions of anger are observed, one can infer that that person is in an irritable mood. However, there is no distinctive facial expression of irritability (Ekman, 1994). 3 Throughout the session, the payoffs were denominated in terms of experimental dollars (E$), which were convertible to euros at a rate of 2E$ = 1 euro. © 2014 Wiley Publishing Asia Pty Ltd RISK AVERSION AND EMOTIONS 299 Table 1. The 10 decision tasks in segment 3 of the experiment Risky alternative (payoffs Percentage Expected Standard in euros, each realized choosing risky mean payoff deviation of Task with probability 0.5) alternative (in euros) payoff (in euros) 1 (7, 5) 63 6 0.56 2 (8, 4) 43 6 0.93 3 (9, 3) 30 6 1.16 4 (7.5, 5) 73 6.18 0.77 5 (9, 4) 67 6.34 1.48 6 (10.5, 3) 70 6.52 2.26 7 (8, 5) 83 6.42 1.02 8 (10, 4) 87 6.87 2.09 9 (12, 3) 70 7.05 2.81 10 (14, 2) 70 7.40 3.74 In the third segment, subjects made 10 pairwise choices between a safe and a risky alternative.