On the Interaction of Social Affect and Cognition: Empathy, Compassion and Theory of Mind

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Load more

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind

Katrin Preckel1, Philipp Kanske1,2 and Tania Singer1

Empathy, compassion and Theory of Mind (ToM) are central topics in social psychology and neuroscience. While empathy enables the sharing of others’ emotions and may result in empathic distress, a maladaptive form of empathic resonance, or compassion, a feeling of warmth and concern for others, ToM provides cognitive understanding of someone else’s thoughts or intentions. These socio-affective and sociocognitive routes to understanding others are subserved by separable, independent brain networks. Nonetheless they are jointly required in many complex social situations. A process that is critical for both, empathy and ToM, is self-other distinction, which is implemented in different temporoparietal brain regions. Thus, adaptive social behavior is a result of dynamic interplay of socio-affective and socio-cognitive processes.

which may be detrimental to the observer and to others and compassion, on the other hand, which is a feeling of warmth and concern for the other. We conclude with a concise summary and an opinion statement.

Defining and neurally characterizing empathy, compassion and Theory of Mind

Empathy describes the process of sharing feelings, that is, resonating with someone else’s feelings, regardless of valence (positive/negative), but with the explicit knowledge that the other person is the origin of this emotion [1]. This socio-affective process results from neural network activations that resemble those activations observed when the same emotion is experienced first-hand (shared network hypothesis) [2–5]. The first studies in neuroscience targeting empathy investigated empathy in the domain of pain, showing that directly experiencing pain and witnessing another person receiving painful stimuli results in shared neural activations in the anterior insula (AI) and anterior middle cingulate cortex (aMCC) [6,7]. Meta-analyses have later identified these regions as a core network that is activated whenever we witness the suffering of others [8,9]. Furthermore, this network is modulated by individual differences in experienced negative affect and empathy [6,10]. While empathy refers to an isomorphic representation of someone else’s affective state, compassion is a complementary social emotion elicited by witnessing the suffering of others and is rather associated with feelings of concern and warmth, linked to the motivation to help [2,11]. Empathy and compassion also differ on a neural level: compassion activates networks that have previously been associated with reward and affiliation processes including the ventral striatum (VS), the nucleus accumbens, the ventral tegmental area (VTA), the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) and the

Addresses

1 Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstraße 1A, 04103 Leipzig, Germany

2 Institute of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Department of

  • ¨
  • Psychology, Technische Universitat Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Corresponding author: Preckel, Katrin ([email protected])

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2018, 19:1–6

This review comes from a themed issue on Emotion-memory

interactions Edited by Mara Mather and Michael Fanselow

For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial Available online 31st July 2017

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.010

2352-1546/ã 2017 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creative-

commons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

  • subgenual
  • anterior
  • cingulate
  • (sgACC)

As a social species, humans are continuously required to process complex social signals. Successful multifaceted social interactions are enabled by socio-affective and socio-cognitive capacities such as empathy, compassion and Theory of Mind (ToM). In this review we will first define these social functions and describe the neural networks associated with each of them. Further, we discuss the interaction of empathy and ToM and delineate the importance of one crucial process, that is, selfother distinction. Socio-affective and socio-cognitive processes are also essential for how (prosocially) we interact with others, particularly when faced with others’ suffering. Two possible outcomes of empathic sharing of others’ suffering are empathic distress on the one hand,
[12,13,14,15,16,17]. Congruently with these activations in reward-associated and affiliation-associated networks, compassion generates positive affect towards others’ suffering. In contrast to compassion, empathic distress, which is an alternative outcome of empathy, may be detrimental to the experiencer as well as to the suffering other [15,18].

In contrast to socio-affective processes, socio-cognition refers to taking another person’s perspective (also referred to as ToM, mentalizing, or cognitive empathy). Rather than an emotional state, ToM yields abstract, propositional knowledge about the other’s mental state. It describes the process of inferring and reasoning about

www.sciencedirect.com

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2018, 19:1–6

2

Emotion-memory interactions

the beliefs, thoughts or emotions of others [19–22]. Crucial brain regions involved in ToM include the ventral temporoparietal junction (TPJ), superior temporal sulcus (STS), temporal poles (TP), medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and precuneus/posterior cingulate (PCC) [23]. necessarily proficient mentalizers [27]. In line with such independent functions, selective impairments in empathy or ToM have been observed in different psychopathologies such as autism and psychopathy. In autism, ToM is deficient [28–31], while no empathy deficits are observed when controlling for alexithymia [32–35]. In psychopathy or chronic aggression, in contrast, ToM is intact, but the propensity to empathize with others is reduced [36,37].

Interactions of social affect and social cognition

Both socio-affective and socio-cognitive processes have been extensively investigated [6,8,9,23,24]. However, research has only recently begun to study how these processes are related and work together to achieve adaptive social behavior. Making use of a novel task (EmpaToM) [25ꢀꢀ] that stimulates both functions concurrently (see Figure 1), the respective neural correlates can be directly compared. The EmpaToM clearly activates separable brain networks that can be replicated in resting state functional connectivity [25ꢀꢀ] and on a brain structural level [26]. Crucially, individual differences in empathy and ToM are unrelated on a behavioral and neural level, that is, strong empathizers are not
Despite being separable, empathy and ToM are jointly required in many complex social situations. An indirect source of evidence for such co-activation is a meta-analysis on different empathy for pain paradigms, which suggests that the core region of the ToM network is coactivated in empathy paradigms when additional inferring from a cue is required to understand the other’s state. [8]. The paradigms investigated in this meta-analysis varied depending on the information provided to the participants. In picture-based paradigms, they were presented with visual depictions of someone in a painful situation,

Figure 1

EmpaToM task

  • Neutral
  • Emotional

Theory

of Mind
Factual

  • nonToM
  • ToM
  • nonToM
  • ToM

Reasoning

Anna thinks that It is correct that a) ... b) ... c) ... a) ... b) ... c) ...

Valence rating
Compassion rating
Confidence rating

  • +
  • Anna
  • +
  • Question
  • +

  • 1-3 s 2 s
  • ~ 15 s
  • 4 s

How do you feel?

  • 4 s
  • 1-3 s max. 15 s
  • 0-2 s 4 s

How much compassion do you feel?
How confident do you feel?

  • negative neutral positive none
  • very much
  • uncertain
  • certain

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

The EmpaToM task. This figure presents the experimental set-up of the EmpaToM task (previously published here [25ꢀꢀ]). The design is a 2 (emotionality of video) Â 2 (ToM requirements) design, resulting in four different video types. Each actor presented each video type: emotional negative and neutral videos, as well as videos with ToM requirements or factual reasoning demands. Each video was followed by various ratings, a valence rating (ranging from negative to positive) and a compassion rating (ranging from none to very much compassion they felt for the person in the video. Afterwards they answered a question that either probed for ToM or factual reasoning. After each question, participants were asked how confident they felt about their answer. For further information on this task, please refer to Kanske et al. [25ꢀꢀ].

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2018, 19:1–6

www.sciencedirect.com

On the interaction of social affect and cognition Preckel, Kanske and Singer

3

whereas in cue-based paradigms, participants were presented a cue/hint that someone else is receiving painful stimulation. Common activations were observed in the typical empathy related core network. However, the two paradigm types resulted in different co-activations. As the cue-based paradigms require additional inference of the other’s affective state from an abstract cue, the ToM related network is co-active [8]. The pictorial presentation of someone in a painful situation, on the other hand, co-activates the action-observation network to decode the potential affective consequences of a certain action. These findings suggest that, dependent on the provided information in a given context, different networks will be co-activated together with the core empathy-related network. external world [39]. It has, however, also been discussed as important in psychopathology, for instance in borderline personality disorder for which a stress related mentalizing deficit has been hypothesized [40].

Self-other distinction

The distinction of self and other constitutes an important element of both empathy and ToM, as it enables the differentiation between one’s own emotional or mental states and the states shared with others. Failure of selfother distinction results in a blending of these states, thereby inducing an egocentricity bias, the tendency to project one’s own emotional or mental state on someone else [41,42], or an altercentric bias, the influence of others’ states on judgments about oneself [43]. Cognitive egocentricity occurs when the own knowledge about a situation influences the reasoning about what someone else thinks about the situation [42], but can also affect simple decisions about the visual perspective of another [44]. Emotional egocentricity occurs when one’s own emotional state influences the judgment of someone else’s affective state [41]. On a neural level, the right TPJ seems to be critical to overcome cognitive egocentricity [45]. It is also functionally connected to other parts of the ToM neural network, such as the medial and ventrolateral PFC and the PCC [45,46]. Overcoming emotional egocentricity involves a slightly anterior temporoparietal region in the right supramarginal gyrus (rSMG) [41]. Correspondingly, disruption of rSMG activity using transcranial magnetic stimulation leads to increased emotional egocentricity [41]. Interestingly, the SMG (see Figure 2) is also part of a different functional connectivity network with extensive connections to the AI [47]. Moreover, increased coupling of SMG to dorsolateral PFC has been related to a reduction in emotional egocentricity in development
The two networks associated with empathy and ToM are also jointly activated for complex evaluations of someone else’s feelings such as empathic accuracy, the ability to infer what someone else is feeling [38]. Here, participants are asked to continuously rate another person’s emotions over an extended period of time; so the emotion of the other may be empathically shared, but it is also critical to acquire abstract knowledge about that emotion to complete the task accurately.

When activated together in complex social situations, the two networks may also directly influence each other. Indeed, when confronted with others’ distress, the AI, which is activated for negative affect sharing, inhibits activity in TPJ, an important hub of the mentalizing network [27]. This reduction in TPJ activity is related to impaired ToM performance [27]. The adaptive value of this inhibitory mechanism in salient, emotional situations may be to prepare for immediate action in the

Figure 2

SMG
TPJ aMCC
PCC mPFC

DLPF
GP

Theory of Mind

Put
VS
STS
AI
NAcc

Compassion

mOFC

sgACC

VTA SN
TP

Empathy for Suffering

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

Neural networks underlying empathy, compassion and Theory of Mind. The separable neural networks underlying empathy (blue), compassion (red), and Theory of Mind (green) are presented. Abbreviations: AI, anterior insula; aMCC, anterior middle cingulate cortex; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; GP, globus pallidus; mOFC, medial orbitofrontal cortex; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; Put, putamen; sgACC, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex; SN, substantia nigra; STS, superior temporal sulcus; SMG, supramarginal gyrus; TP, temporal poles; TPJ, temporoparietal junction; VS, ventral striatum; VTA, ventral tegmental area.

www.sciencedirect.com

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2018, 19:1–6

4

Emotion-memory interactions

[46]. Thus, temporoparietal regions seem to play a crucial role in overcoming egocentricity, possibly contributing to self-other distinction. The activation of SMG and TPJ in complex empathy and ToM paradigms, respectively [25ꢀꢀ], corresponds with the crucial role of self-other distinction for both capacities. networks and compassion increases positive affect to buffer negative affect through the active generation of positive emotions via reward-related and affiliationrelated brain circuitries.

Concluding remarks

In the present paper, we have suggested that the social brain is supported by different functions and underlying neuronal networks and that it is important to differentiate between socio-affective versus socio-cognitive routes to the understanding of others. Sharing affective states with another person (empathy), feeling concern for another (compassion) and reasoning about another person’s mental state (ToM) are separable on a conceptual, behavioral and neural level; strong empathizers are not necessarily proficient mentalizers and each domain may be selectively impaired in psychopathologies such as autism or psychopathy. Furthermore, an original adaptive empathic response to the suffering of others can lead to a maladaptive response, that is, empathic distress which can be reversed by learning how to turn empathy into compassion. Compassion can thus be viewed as an emotionregulation strategy that buffers negative affect through the active generation of positive affect relying on rewardrelated and affiliation-related brain circuitries. Despite the separability of socio-affective and socio-cognitive functions, these social capacities are jointly activated and interact in complex social situations. In synthesis, the evidence supports a detailed view of the social mind, not as a monolithic ‘social intelligence’, but as dynamic interplay between different functions and subserving neuronal networks that enable in different ways to engage in prosocial behavior.

Social behavior

Empathy and ToM provide us with an understanding of our interaction partners’ emotional and cognitive states, thereby forming the basis for flexible interactive behavior and social decision making. Lately, neuroscientific research has focused on the relationship between social emotions and social cognition in predicting prosocial behaviors [48ꢀꢀ,49]. For instance, using donation choices for charity organizations, a recent study [48ꢀꢀ] showed that an extensive cortical network is involved in forming such prosocial decisions. Specifically, AI activity predicted generous donations when participants reported empathic feelings for the charity’s goal before, while TPJ activity predicted donations, when participants took the perspective of the organization’s cause [48ꢀꢀ]. Remarkably, empathy and ToM measurements from an independent task, were also related to these functions’ role in donation decisions, suggesting that stable individual propensities are involved in prosocial behavior. In contrast, impairments in empathy or ToM have been associated with antisocial behavior, for instance to hyperaggression [18,37,50]. While empathy may be related to prosociality, a particular outcome of empathy — empathic distress — describes the intense sharing of other’s emotions and may be detrimental, because it can result in own suffering as well as in asocial or even antisocial effects, such as aggressive verbal behavior [18]. Short-term contemplative training in compassion increases helping, even when it is costly [51]. In contrast to empathic distress, compassion reduces punishment behavior in long-term compassion meditation practitioners even when they were themselves the victim of fairness violations [52]. The critical property of compassion may be that it counteracts negative emotion elicited by experiencing others’ suffering through positive emotion generation, thereby acting as an emotion regulation strategy [14,15,53]. The direct comparison of compassion to the most widely investigated emotion regulation strategy — reappraisal — showed that in well-trained long-term meditators, cognitive reappraisal was most efficient in reducing negative affect, but compassion induction worked through up-regulation of positive affect and associated networks in the brain [14]. On a neural level, compassion was accompanied by activations in reward and positive emotion related networks [14]. Reappraisal on the other hand yielded activity in a fronto-parietal network associated with cognitive control and attention regulation [54]. The different brain activation patterns are, thus, in line with the hypothesis that reappraisal down-regulates negative affect mainly via executive and cognitive control

Conflict of interest statement

Nothing declared.

References and recommended reading

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

ꢀ of special interest ꢀꢀ of outstanding interest

1. de Vignemont F, Singer T: The empathic brain: how, when and

why? Trends Cogn Sci 2006, 10:435-441.

2. Singer T, Lamm C: The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann N Y

Acad Sci 2009, 1156:81-96.

3. Gallese V: The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity.

Psychopathology 2003, 36:171-180.

Recommended publications
  • Finding the Golden Mean: the Overuse, Underuse, and Optimal Use of Character Strengths

    Finding the Golden Mean: the Overuse, Underuse, and Optimal Use of Character Strengths

    Counselling Psychology Quarterly ISSN: 0951-5070 (Print) 1469-3674 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ccpq20 Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths Ryan M. Niemiec To cite this article: Ryan M. Niemiec (2019): Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths, Counselling Psychology Quarterly, DOI: 10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 Published online: 20 May 2019. Submit your article to this journal View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ccpq20 COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 ARTICLE Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths Ryan M. Niemiec VIA Institute on Character, Cincinnati, OH, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY The science of well-being has catalyzed a tremendous amount of Received 28 February 2019 research with no area more robust in application and impact than Accepted 8 May 2019 the science of character strengths. As the empirical links between KEYWORDS character strengths and positive outcomes rapidly grow, the research Character strengths; around strength imbalances and the use of strengths with problems strengths overuse; strengths and conflicts is nascent. The use of character strengths in understand- underuse; optimal use; ing and handling life suffering as well as emerging from it, is particularly second wave positive aligned within second wave positive psychology. Areas of particular psychology; golden mean promise include strengths overuse and strengths underuse, alongside its companion of strengths optimaluse.Thelatterisviewedasthe golden mean of character strengths which refers to the expression of the right combination of strengths, to the right degree, and in the right situation.
  • 1 the Development of Empathy: How, When, and Why Nicole M. Mcdonald & Daniel S. Messinger University of Miami Department Of

    1 the Development of Empathy: How, When, and Why Nicole M. Mcdonald & Daniel S. Messinger University of Miami Department Of

    1 The Development of Empathy: How, When, and Why Nicole M. McDonald & Daniel S. Messinger University of Miami Department of Psychology 5665 Ponce de Leon Dr. Coral Gables, FL 33146, USA 2 Empathy is a potential psychological motivator for helping others in distress. Empathy can be defined as the ability to feel or imagine another person’s emotional experience. The ability to empathize is an important part of social and emotional development, affecting an individual’s behavior toward others and the quality of social relationships. In this chapter, we begin by describing the development of empathy in children as they move toward becoming empathic adults. We then discuss biological and environmental processes that facilitate the development of empathy. Next, we discuss important social outcomes associated with empathic ability. Finally, we describe atypical empathy development, exploring the disorders of autism and psychopathy in an attempt to learn about the consequences of not having an intact ability to empathize. Development of Empathy in Children Early theorists suggested that young children were too egocentric or otherwise not cognitively able to experience empathy (Freud 1958; Piaget 1965). However, a multitude of studies have provided evidence that very young children are, in fact, capable of displaying a variety of rather sophisticated empathy related behaviors (Zahn-Waxler et al. 1979; Zahn-Waxler et al. 1992a; Zahn-Waxler et al. 1992b). Measuring constructs such as empathy in very young children does involve special challenges because of their limited verbal expressiveness. Nevertheless, young children also present a special opportunity to measure constructs such as empathy behaviorally, with less interference from concepts such as social desirability or skepticism.
  • Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes?

    Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes?

    Tennessee Advisory Commission on Intergovernmental Relations Staff Information Report January 2003 TACIR Publication Policy Staff Information Reports, Staff Briefs, Staff Technical Reports and Staff Working Papers and TACIR Fast Facts are issued to promote the mission and objectives of the Commission. These reports are intended to share information and research findings relevant to important public policy issues in an attempt to promote wider understanding. Only reports clearly labeled as Commission Reports represent the official position of the Commission. Others are informational. The Tennessee Advisory Commission on Intergovernmental Relations Suite 508, 226 Capitol Boulevard, Nashville,Tennessee 37243 (615) 741-3012 FAX (615) 532-2443 email: [email protected] website: www.state.tn.us/tacir Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes? A Staff Information Report The following staff have contributed to this report: Ed Young, Ph.D. Research Consultant Principal Author Harry A. Green, Ph.D. Executive Director Lynnisse Roehrich-Patrick, J.D. Director of Special Projects Project Manager, Editor Linda Joseph Publications Assistant Teresa Gibson Publications Assistant January 2003 ii Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes? Foreword In 1988, more than half of Tennessee’s school systems joined together and filed a lawsuit charging the state with failing to fund all systems, rich and poor, equitably, and in 1993, they won. In the meantime and after two years of deliberation, the Tennessee General Assembly adopted a new funding formula as part of the Education Improvement Act (EIA) of 1992. The plaintiffs were not entirely satisfied with that remedy and challenged it on three bases: it did not solve the problem of inequity among the systems in teachers’ salaries, it should be funded immediately and not phased in over six years as the legislature planned, and it did not provide adequate funds to bring all school buildings up to modern standards.
  • Zest and Work

    Zest and Work

    Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 30, 161–172 (2009) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/job.584 Zest and work CHRISTOPHER PETERSON1*, NANSOOK PARK2, NICHOLAS HALL3 AND MARTIN E.P.SELIGMAN 3 1University of Michigan, Michigan, U.S.A. 2University of Rhode Island, Rhode Island, U.S.A. 3University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania, U.S.A. Summary Zest is a positive trait reflecting a person’s approach to life with anticipation, energy, and excitement. In the present study, 9803 currently employed adult respondents to an Internet site completed measures of dispositional zest, orientation to work as a calling, and satisfaction with work and life in general. Across all occupations, zest predicted the stance that work was a calling (r ¼.39), as well as work satisfaction (r ¼.46) and general life satisfaction (r ¼.53). Zest deserves further attention from organizational scholars, especially how it can be encouraged in the workplace. Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Your work is to discover your work, and then with all of your heart to give yourself to it.—the Buddha Introduction Recent years have seen a widespread call for the study of work organizations in which people can be well and do well (Cameron, Dutton, & Quinn, 2003; Gardner, Csikszentmihalyi, & Damon, 2001; Luthans, 2003; Wright, 2003). The emergence of the positive perspective within organizational psychology has brought new attention to the venerable topic of work satisfaction (Hoppock, 1935). Satisfaction with the work that one does is seen not just as a contributor to good performance and increased profitability but as a worthy end in its own right (Heslin, 2005).
  • Cognition, Affect, and Learning —The Role of Emotions in Learning

    Cognition, Affect, and Learning —The Role of Emotions in Learning

    How People Learn: Cognition, Affect, and Learning —The Role of Emotions in Learning Barry Kort Ph.D. and Robert Reilly Ed.D. {kort, reilly}@media.mit.edu formerly MIT Media Lab Draft as of date January 2, 2019 Learning is the quintessential emotional experience. Our species, Homo Sapiens, are the beings who think. We are also the beings who learn, and the beings who simultaneously experience a rich spectrum of affective emotional states, including a selected suite of emotional states specifically and directly related to learning. This proposal reviews previously published research and theoretical models relating emotions to learning and cognition and presents ideas and proposals for extending that research and reducing it to practice. Our perspective The concept of affect in learning (i.e., emotions in learning) is the same pedagogy applied by an athletic coach at a sporting event. A coach recognizes the affective state of an athlete, and, for example, exhorts that athlete toward increased performance (e.g., raises the level of enthusiasm), or, redirects a frustrated athlete to a productive affective state (e.g., instills confidence, or pride). A coach recognizes that an athlete’s affective state is a critical factor during performance; and, when appropriate, a coach will intervene with a meaningful strategy or tactic. Athletic coaches are skilled at recognizing affective states and intervening appropriately. Educators can have the same impact on a learner by understanding a learner’s affective state and intervening with appropriate strategies or tactics that will meaningfully manage and guide a person’s learning journey. There are several learning theories and a great deal of neuroscience/affective research.
  • Risk Aversion and Emotions

    Risk Aversion and Emotions

    bs_bs_banner Pacific Economic Review, 19: 3 (2014) pp. 296–312 doi: 10.1111/1468-0106.12067 RISK AVERSION AND EMOTIONS YEN NGUYEN Tilburg University CHARLES N. NOUSSAIR* Tilburg University Abstract. We consider the relationship between emotions and decision-making under risk. Specifi- cally, we examine the emotional correlates of risk-averse decisions. In our experiment, individuals’ facial expressions are monitored with face reading software, as they are presented with risky lotteries. We then correlate these facial expressions with subsequent decisions in risky choice tasks. We find that a more positive emotional state is positively correlated with greater risk taking. The strength of a number of emotions, including fear, happiness, anger and surprise, is positively correlated with risk aversion. JEL Classification Code: C9 1. INTRODUCTION The mechanisms that underpin choice under risk have been a primary focus of research in economics, psychology and neuroscience. The traditional approach in economics has been to postulate the existence of a utility function, possessing the expected utility property, that represents an individual’s preferences, and to note that concavity implies a preference for relatively safe lotteries, while convexity corresponds to a preference for relatively risky lotteries. Neuroscien- tists have documented the physiological correlates of risky decision-making. Psychologists and behavioural economists stress the role of emotions such as happiness or fear in guiding individuals’ choices in risky situations (see e.g. Forgas, 1995; Loewenstein and Lerner, 2003; Coget et al., 2011). The focus of the work reported here is to better understand the connection between emotions, which are ultimately psychological terms describing profiles of physiological responses, and the level of risk aversion exhibited in financial decision-making tasks.
  • Altering the Trajectory of Affect and Affect Regulation: the Impact of Compassion Training

    Altering the Trajectory of Affect and Affect Regulation: the Impact of Compassion Training

    Mindfulness DOI 10.1007/s12671-017-0773-3 ORIGINAL PAPER Altering the Trajectory of Affect and Affect Regulation: the Impact of Compassion Training Hooria Jazaieri1 & Kelly McGonigal2 & Ihno A. Lee3 & Thupten Jinpa 2 & James R. Doty2,4 & James J. Gross3 & Philippe R. Goldin5 # Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017 Abstract A growing literature has begun to document the lesser use of expressive suppression and greater acceptance effects of compassion training on a variety of important inter- when experiencing stress/anxiety. These results suggest that personal behaviors (e.g., helping behavior). What is not yet interventions such as compassion training may help modulate well understood, however, is what impact compassion training specific affective states and modify the use of and self-efficacy has on affect and affect regulation. To examine this issue, we for specific regulatory strategies. implemented a 9-week compassion training program in which 51 adults provided twice-daily ratings of four affective states Keywords Affect . Affect regulation . Affective trajectories . (anxiety, calm, fatigue, alertness) as well as their desire and Affect dynamics . Self-efficacy . Compassion . Diary study . capability to regulate these affective states. In addition, partic- Experience sampling . Ecological momentary assessment . ipants provided weekly responses regarding five specific reg- Multilevel analysis ulatory strategies. Analysis of day-to-day trajectories of affec- tive experience showed a decrease in anxiety and increase in calmness. Day-to-day trajectories of affect regulation demon- One commonly cited definition of compassion is a Bfeeling strated that participants were more likely to choose to accept that arises in witnessing another’s suffering and that motivates and thus not influence or modulate affective experience (as a subsequent desire to help^ (Goetz et al.
  • Understanding and Watering the Seeds of Compassion

    Understanding and Watering the Seeds of Compassion

    Research in Human Development, 12: 280–287, 2015 Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1542-7609 print / 1542-7617 online DOI: 10.1080/15427609.2015.1068060 Understanding and Watering the Seeds of Compassion Mark T. Greenberg and Christa Turksma Pennsylvania State University The authors call for a broad agenda to create comprehensive models and research on the devel- opment and promotion of empathy and compassion. This wish would lead to a new level of 5 developmental/ecological understanding of the growth of compassion as well as effective policies and practices and interventions that nurture caring, compassion, and service to others in our schools and communities. The authors define empathy and compassion, briefly discuss the outline of early developmental processes, and call for basic research on these essential aspects of human develop- ment. The authors conclude by discussing the need to develop new ways to promote empathy and 10 compassion in families, schools and communities. Our wish is for a life-span developmental psychology to make greater contributions to building a compassionate world society. This calls for a broad agenda to create comprehensive models and research on the development and promotion of emotional awareness, empathy, and com- passion. The practical outcome of such a comprehensive program would include (1) clearer 15 developmental/ecological models of the growth of compassion; (2) effective policies, programs, and practices that support the development of caring, compassion, and service to others in our schools and communities; and (3) children, youth, and adults who show greater service to others as well as caring about the preservation of the earth’s ecology.
  • It's Complicated: a Literature Review of Happiness and the Big Five

    It's Complicated: a Literature Review of Happiness and the Big Five

    Bengtson 1 Lilly Bengtson PSYC330 It’s Complicated: A Review of Literature on Happiness and the Big Five One of the great quests of an individual’s life is often to find happiness. But what does “happiness” mean? Can it even be “found?” These questions and more have been addressed with the growth of the positive psychology movement, a modern attempt to examine happiness from a scientific perspective. A natural first step in the study of happiness is evaluating exactly who is happy, and why. While a great many factors influence one’s satisfaction with life, personality is an especially relevant contributor to consider. Personality factors influence how people see the world, how they behave, and how they move through life, so it follows that these same factors would strongly influence one’s ultimate failure or success in achieving happiness. The Five Factor model of personality is a tried-and-true trait model which breaks personality down into five basic components: extraversion, openness, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and neuroticism. This empirically-validated model has been combined with the relatively recent positive psychology movement to study how personality traits affect individuals’ overall happiness. The Big Five traits of neuroticism and extraversion have been shown to correlate strongly with measures of individual happiness, but this effect is moderated by both internal and external factors of an individual’s life circumstances. In order to study the relationship between personality traits and happiness, one must first establish exactly how to evaluate this concept. A common measure for happiness is subjective well-being, which can be broken down into individual scales of life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect.
  • Education in a Pandemic: the Disparate Impacts of COVID-19 on America's Students (PDF)

    Education in a Pandemic: the Disparate Impacts of COVID-19 on America's Students (PDF)

    Education in a Pandemic: The Disparate Impacts of COVID-19 on America’s Students OFFICE FOR CIVIL RIGHTS Message from the Acting Assistant Secretary In his Executive Order on Supporting the Reopening and Continuing Operation of Schools and Early Childhood Education Providers, President Biden expressed our nation’s commitment to students across the country: “Every student in America deserves a high-quality education in a safe environment.”1 Over the course of the COVID-19 pandemic, so many students have found new ways to continue learning in this challenging time and countless teachers, staff, faculty, administrators, and institutional leaders, along with students’ families, have gone above and beyond to support our students at all educational levels. Yet, this promise of a safe, high-quality education was already out of reach for many students long before the COVID-19 pandemic and could slip further away if we do not act collectively and with attention to equal opportunity for all students. Against this backdrop, this Report responds to President Biden’s Executive Order, and, specifically, to this call to the Assistant Secretary for Civil Rights in the Department of Education: [T]o deliver a report as soon as practicable on the disparate impacts of COVID-19 on students in elementary, secondary, and higher education, including those attending historically black colleges and universities, and Tribal colleges and universities, Hispanic- serving institutions, and other minority-serving institutions.2 The Department’s Office for Civil Rights
  • Scientific Evaluation of Animal Emotions: Brief History and Recent New Zealand Contributions

    Scientific Evaluation of Animal Emotions: Brief History and Recent New Zealand Contributions

    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Open Journal Systems at the Victoria University of Wellington Library Scientific evaluation of animal emotions: Brief history and recent New Zealand contributions Beausoleil, N.J., Stratton, R.B., Guesgen, M.J., Sutherland, M.A., Johnson, C.B. Abstract The idea of animals having emotions was once rejected as being anthropomorphic and unscientific. However, with society’s changing views and advances in scientific knowledge and technology, the idea of animal emotions is becoming more accepted. Emotions are subjective internal experiences that can’t be measured directly. Animal welfare scientists must infer emotions by measuring the behavioural, physiological and neurobiological components of emotional experience. In this paper, we describe innovative ways in which these indicators have been used by New Zealand scientists to facilitate a more holistic understanding of the emotions and welfare of animals. Introduction From a scientific perspective, emotion is defined as an innate response to an event or situation (internal or external) that comprises behavioural, physiological, subjective (the feeling) and cognitive (subsequent decision-making) components.1 Emotions are the result of complex processing, by the nervous system, of sensory information gathered from within and outside the animal’s body. The capture and processing of this sensory information are influenced by the biology of the animal species, as well as by individual factors such as the animal’s genetic predispositions, life stage, sex, previous experience, learning and memory.2 The emotion or emotions resulting from these processes of mental evaluation are thus uniquely personal to the individual, but can be broadly characterized by their valence (pleasant or unpleasant) and the degree of arousal generated.3 While the behavioural, physiological and, in some cases, cognitive components of an emotional response can be scientifically evaluated using observable indicators, the subjective component cannot.
  • Measuring Farm Animal Emotions—Sensor-Based Approaches

    Measuring Farm Animal Emotions—Sensor-Based Approaches

    sensors Review Measuring Farm Animal Emotions—Sensor-Based Approaches Suresh Neethirajan * , Inonge Reimert and Bas Kemp Adaptation Physiology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University & Research, 6700 AH Wageningen, The Netherlands; [email protected] (I.R.); [email protected] (B.K.) * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +31-068-343-2700 Abstract: Understanding animal emotions is a key to unlocking methods for improving animal welfare. Currently there are no ‘benchmarks’ or any scientific assessments available for measuring and quantifying the emotional responses of farm animals. Using sensors to collect biometric data as a means of measuring animal emotions is a topic of growing interest in agricultural technology. Here we reviewed several aspects of the use of sensor-based approaches in monitoring animal emotions, beginning with an introduction on animal emotions. Then we reviewed some of the available technological systems for analyzing animal emotions. These systems include a variety of sensors, the algorithms used to process biometric data taken from these sensors, facial expression, and sound analysis. We conclude that a single emotional expression measurement based on either the facial feature of animals or the physiological functions cannot show accurately the farm animal’s emotional changes, and hence compound expression recognition measurement is required. We propose some novel ways to combine sensor technologies through sensor fusion into efficient systems for monitoring and measuring the animals’ compound expression of emotions. Finally, we explore future perspectives in the field, including challenges and opportunities. Keywords: animal emotions; animal welfare; behavior; sensors; precision livestock farming; farm animals; animal-based measures Citation: Neethirajan, S.; Reimert, I.; 1.