The Theory of Repression in Its Relation to Memory1

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Theory of Repression in Its Relation to Memory1 THE THEORY OF REPRESSION IN ITS RELATION TO MEMORY1. BY ERNEST JONES. THE following remarks represent in a sense a continuation of the discussion that took place at. the Durham meeting last July when papers were read on this subject by Pear, Wolf, Mitchell, and Loveday2, my object being to elucidate furthm certain points that were incompletely dealt with on that occasion and to raise certain other questions that are indirectly cognate to the same theme. The theory itself may be formulated in several different ways, the most comprehensive of which would run somewhat as follows: There exist in the mind certain inhibiting forces, which tend to exclude from consciousness all mental processes the presence of which would evoke there, either directly or through association, a feeling of “unpleasant- ness” (Unlust). It is, of course, evident that the efficacy of such forces is at best a relative one, for otherwise consciousness would never experience Urdust, but the thesis is maintained that, whenever this experience occurs, it is only because the action of the forces in question has first been neutralised by other tendencies and motives in the mind, whether volitional or not. It will be noticed that in the definition given the word “exclude” has been used as a common term for what some writers consider to be two independent processes, namely, on the one hand the expulsion from consciousness of an “ unpleasant’’ mental process, and on the other the tendency to prevent the return of this to consciousness on any subsequent occasion. As it seems possible to adopt one of these conceptions and not the other, they will be separately discussed, but I may remark that those who accept the theory of repression as here formulated are not much concerned to Read before the British Psychological Society, Jan. 3&h, 1916. Published in this Jozcrnnl, m. No. 2. J. of Psych. VIII 3 34 Theory of Repreasion in its ReZation to Memory make a sharp distinction between the two, considering it to be relatively unimportant. For them the inhibiting force of repression is a tendency in constant operation, independently of whether the “ unpleasant” mental process is of recent date or not, of whether it is at the time conscious or not, and even of whether it has ever been conscious or not, all matters which do not concern the main point at issue. The question of the relation of the inhibiting forces to consciousness and the unconscious is more obscure. Freud himself seems to place what he terms the “censorship”-an expression covering the sum total of the repressing forces in question-at the place of transition between the unconscious and the preconscious, with a less important one at the place of transition between the preconscious and consciousness. While it may be agreed that the action of repression is mainly exhibited at these points of junction, the evidence, in my opinion, induces one rather to picture the inhibiting tendencies as being distributed, in a streaming fashion, throughout the whole mind, conscious as well as unconscious, incrming in strength, however, as one proceeds from the level of consciousness to the lowest layers of the unconscious. The bearing that the theory of repression has on the problems of memory is a manifold one, and we may divide up the subject under the headings of registration, conservation, recollection, and recognition respectively. I. REGISTRATION. Practically all observers seem to agree that in attention there is a selective operation according to the pleasure-pain principle, whereby we attend to that which is pleasurable rather than to that which is not. (It is hardly necessary to say that no universality is claimed for this principle to the exclusion of others.) In so far, therefore, as variations in attention affect the registration of impressions, the process of exclusion, which is the essence of repression, must play a part. Ih action, however, is by no means always in the direction that it might at first sight be imagined to be. It might be thought that, if repression determines the ignoring of some unpleasant sight or sound, these, being less attended to, would undergo a fainter registration and would fade more readily than other impressions. One should exercise caution, however, before coming to this conclusion, apparent as it may seem. To say that a given idea is unpleasant is not at all the same as saying it is without interest or significance to the subject, in fact the reverse is more often the case. Any psycho-analytical ERNESTJONES 35 experience will provide numerous instances where an idea that has been presented to consciousness has been immediately ignored and its meaning not apprehended, but has nevertheless made a deep impression on the subject’s mind, and has been registered with unusual distinctness. It is quite common, for example, for girls in the period of adolescence genuinely to ignore in an astounding manner all allusions to sex matters, which they meet with at every turn, in newspapers, in novels, and in daily life, and it is remarkable to observe later in a psycho-analysis the precision with which these impressions have been registered and their significance appreciated without the subject being at all consciously aware of it. It is also a commonplace observation in insanity that in a state of excitement or delirium certain expressions may be reproduced by a lady who certainly must have completely ignored them at the time of hearing them1. The only means of estimating the intensity of registration is by studying the conditions of subsequent recall, conditions which are themselves influenced by various other factors that are not easy to eliminate or allow for, but so far as the evidence goes it does not, in my opinion, favour the view that incoming impressions which are ignored through the action of repression are more faintly registered than others which have not been submitted to this action. Much might be said concerning the selective action of repression in regard to attention (and also to perception) that is of importance to the general theory of attention, but as the subject is not strictly germane to the problems of memory it must be passed over here with merely this short allusion. 11. CONSERVATION. The theory of repression, and the observations on which it is founded, has one very important contribution to make regarding the conservation or retention of memory-impressions. It is to the effect that the capacity of the mind for such retention is vastly greater than is generally supposed. It is ditlicult to lay too much emphasis on this point, one to which attention has repeatedly been called by medical psychologists, for even after a considerable experience of such data one is constantly being surprised at the truly extraordinary manner in which minute details may be revived after having apparently been completely for- gotten for many years, for half a century or longer. This applies both A well-known literary example is the case of Ophelia. 3-2 36 Theory qf Repres&on m $68 Relation to Nemory to significant, repressed memories and to those of a more trivial nature. With regard to the latter class I must say that I become increasingly sceptical about its supposed involvement in what Mr Pearl calls a, process of physiological decay, for delicate methods of investigation constantly succeed in demonstrating the continued presence in the mind of trivial elements that one might have imagined had disappeared long ago. At the same time, the distinction Mr Pear draws between the two classes of forgotten material, the significant and the insignificant, appears to me to be of great importance, and the explanation of the process of forgetting in the two cases raises some interesting questions, to which I shall return later in this paper. 111. RECOLLECTION. Professor Brough has somewhere remarked that psychologists may be divided into two schools as regards the subject of memory, com- prising those who hold that the facts in most need of explanation are those of remembering, and those who hold that they are the facts of forgetting. Psycho-analysts certainly belong to the latter school, which I imagine to-be the less numerous one. To us, and contrary to the usual opinion, it would seem more natural if everything were remembered, and it is the circumstance of a given mental impression not being capable of recall, while still in the mind, that demands an explanation. A consideration to which I would attach great importance is that the phenomenon of recall seems to be too often regarded as being induced by conscious volition, the fact being overlooked that in the majority of instances it proceeds quite spontaneously and independently of volition. If, now, the matter be thought of in other than volitional terms, the question is, not what is the mechanism whereby we recall a given idea when we want to, but rather how is it that such a relatively small number of ideas actually flow into consciousness, or have the capacity of ever entering consciousness? To this question Freud’s ’ theory of repression attempts to provide an answer. The main fact that this theory sets out to explain is that it is more difficult to recall an unpleasant memory than a pleasant one, other things being equal. The fact itself is, I think, beyond dispute, and has not been questioned by anyone who has seriously investigated the phenomena, either experimentally or clinically. According to the theory of repression, the reason why it is more difficult to recall an 1 Loc. cit. p 139. ERNESTJONES 37 unpleasant memory is because it is kept back from entering conscious- ness by the action of .certain inhibiting, “repressing” forces, the function of which is to guard consciousness, so far as possible, from the pain of disagreeable affects.
Recommended publications
  • Psycho-Analysis
    THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHO-ANALYSIS VOLUME X II OCTOBER 1931 PART 4 ORIGINAL PAPERS THE THERAPEUTIC EFFECT OF INEXACT INTERPRETATION : A CONTRIBUTION TO THE THEORY OF SUGGESTION BY EDWARD GLOVER LONDON Psycho-analytic interest in theories of cure is naturally directed for the most part to the curative processes occurring in analytic treatment : the therapeutic effect of other methods is, nowadays at any rate, more a matter of general psychological interest. In earlier times, of course, it was necessary to pay special attention to the theoretical significance of non-analytic psychotherapy. Statements were frequently bandied about that psycho-analysis was nothing more than camouflaged suggestion : moreover, the fact that analytic method was based on experiences derived from situations of rapport between physician and patient, as for example, in hypnosis, made some theoretical differentia­ tion desirable. Most discussions of the ‘ resolution of transference ’ can be regarded as contributions to this problem, affording a rough but serviceable distinction between analytic and other therapeutic methods. And the special studies of Freud (1) on group psychology, Ferenczi (2) on transference, Ernest Jones (3) on suggestion and auto-suggestion, Abraham (4) on Couéism and an unfinished study by Radö (5) on the processes of cure, have given a broader theoretical basis to this differentiation. Nevertheless we are periodically stimulated to reconsider the relations between different forms of psychotherapy, more particularly when any advance is made in analytic knowledge. When such advances occur we are bound to ask ourselves, ' what happened to our cases before we were in a position to turn this fresh knowledge to advantage ? ' Admittedly we would not be under this obligation had we not previously used terms such as ‘ cure ', ' thorough analysis etc., etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Lacan and Foulkes1
    Volume 1 (Issue 2), 2012 GROUP ACTS AND MISSED ENCOUNTER: LACAN AND FOULKES1 Erica Burman Prologue The theme of action outside speech typically does not gain positive treatment in psychoanalytical contexts, with all such action vulnerable to being designated “acting out”, or at best “acting in”. But the question “how to act” transcends such false oppositions between acting and not acting, to topicalise instead ethical responsibility and social engagement. In this paper these themes are explored in terms of the relations between Lacanian psychoanalytic and Foulkesian group analytic ideas. The assertion of the priority of one model over the other, or the assimilation of one to the other is not aimed at here, but rather this paper seeks to highlight, and perhaps indicate, some potential evaluations of how each approach attempts to engage with the question of action. While Lacanian psychoanalysis often draws a strong boundary around the analytic space, resisting the generalization of psychoanalytic phenomena outside the clinic, the social model of the psyche to which group analysts are committed, means that no such absolute distinctions can be maintained. Paradoxically, group analysts seem rather more reluctant than Lacanians to discuss questions of ethics, preferring to talk about questions of democracy, although both models share an understanding of analytical process in terms of the 1 This article is a version of a paper delivered at APPI’s 17th Annual Congress (2010) entitled; How to Act – Ethics and the Psychoanalytic Clinic in a Culture of Suppression and Demand. 22 23 Lacunae promotion of (if also the impossibility of) free speech. The challenge therefore, is to find ways of both analyzing and intervening in, the contexts that give rise to analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • A Counter-Theory of Transference
    A Counter-Theory of Transference John M. Shlien, Harvard University "Transference" is a fiction, invented and maintained by the therapist to protect himself from the consequences of his own behavior. To many, this assertion will seem an exaggeration, an outrage, an indictment. It is presented here as a serious hypothesis, charging a highly invested profession with the task of re-examining a fundamental concept in practice. It is not entirely new to consider transference as a defense. Even its proponents cast it among the defense mechanisms when they term it a "projection". But they mean that the defense is on the part of the patient. My assertion suggests a different type of defense; denial or distortion, and on the part of the therapist. Mine is not an official position in client-centered therapy. There is none. Carl Rogers has dealt with the subject succinctly, in about twenty pages (1951, pp. 198-217), a relatively brief treatment of a matter that has taken up volumes of the literature in the fleld.[1] "In client-centered therapy, this involved and persistent dependency relationship does not tend to develop" (p. 201), though such transference attitudes are evident in a considerable proportion of cases handled by client-centered therapists. Transference is not fostered or cultivated by this present-time oriented framework where intensive exploration of early childhood is not required, and where the therapist is visible and available for reality resting. While Rogers knows of the position taken here and has, I believe, been influenced by it since its first presentation in 1959, he has never treated the transference topic as an issue of dispute.
    [Show full text]
  • The Enigma of the Hour 100 Years of Psychoanalytic Thought
    The Enigma of the Hour 100 Years of Psychoanalytic Thought An exhibition to mark the centenary of The International Journal of Psychoanalysis curated by Simon Moretti with Goshka Macuga and Dana Birksted-Breen Freud Museum London Exhibition Guide On the occasion of the centenary of a return to disintegration of the death drive Linder, Goshka Macuga, Simon The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, the of Thanatos. In dialogue with the curators, exhibition The Enigma of the Hour: 100 Years the group of researchers and psychoanalysts of Psychoanalytic Thought presents archival explored in collaboration various aspects of the Moretti, Daniel Silver, Paloma material around specific themes, which touch history of the International Journal, the fruit of on the origins and life of The International which is exhibited in the Display Case in the Journal, alongside contemporary artworks. Exhibition Room and elaborated on in the Varga Weisz with additional Originally conceived by the Journal’s editor- Compendium to it. in-chief Dana Birksted-Breen and curated works by Duncan Grant, by artists Simon Moretti and Goshka Macuga The exhibition includes new commissions with Dana Birksted-Breen, the exhibition by Simon Moretti and Goshka Macuga, brings together themes central to both psycho- made in response to the themes and archives Barbara Ker-Seymer & John analysis and art: translation, transformation, chosen, as well as especially selected works temporality, the unconscious, metaphor and by their invited artists, Linder, Daniel Silver dreams. The theme of Oedipus, which was so and Paloma Varga Weisz, and loans from the Banting, Rodrigo Moynihan critical to Freud’s theorizing, with Oedipus British Psychoanalytic Society, and the Tate and the Sphinx from a painting by Ingres Gallery, including works by Duncan Grant, chosen as logo of the Journal, also appears Barbara Ker-Seymer with John Banting and as a leitmotif in the exhibition.
    [Show full text]
  • Carl Gustav Jung's Pivotal Encounter with Sigmund Freud During Their Journey to America
    Swiss American Historical Society Review Volume 54 Number 2 Article 4 6-2018 The Psychological Odyssey of 1909: Carl Gustav Jung's Pivotal Encounter with Sigmund Freud during their Journey to America William E. Herman Axel Fair-Schulz Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/sahs_review Part of the European History Commons, and the European Languages and Societies Commons Recommended Citation Herman, William E. and Fair-Schulz, Axel (2018) "The Psychological Odyssey of 1909: Carl Gustav Jung's Pivotal Encounter with Sigmund Freud during their Journey to America," Swiss American Historical Society Review: Vol. 54 : No. 2 , Article 4. Available at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/sahs_review/vol54/iss2/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Swiss American Historical Society Review by an authorized editor of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Herman and Fair-Schulz: The Psychological Odyssey of 1909: The Psychological Odyssey of 1909: Carl Gustav Jung's Pivotal Encounter with Sigmund Freud during their Journey to America by William E. Herman and Axel Fair-Schulz The year 1909 proved decisive for our relationship. - Carl Gustav Jung's autobiography. Memories, Dreams, Reflections (1961) M any volumes in the scholarly literature explore the complex evolution of the relationship between Carl Gustav Jung and Sigmund Freud as well as the eventual split between these two influential contributors to psychoanalytic thought and more generally to the field of psychology and other academic fields/professions. The events that transpired during the seven-week journey from Europe to America and back in the autumn of 1909 would serve as a catalyst to not only re-direct the lives of Jung and Freud along different paths, but also re-shape the roadmap of psychoanalytic thinking, clinical applications, and psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction: Jung, New York, 1912 Sonu Shamdasani
    Copyrighted Material IntroductIon: Jung, neW York, 1912 Sonu Shamdasani September 28, 1912. the New York Times featured a full-page inter- view with Jung on the problems confronting america, with a por- trait photo entitled “america facing Its Most tragic Moment”— the first prominent feature of psychoanalysis in the Times. It was Jung, the Times correctly reported, who “brought dr. freud to the recognition of the older school of psychology.” the Times went on to say, “[H]is classrooms are crowded with students eager to under- stand what seems to many to be an almost miraculous treatment. His clinics are crowded with medical cases which have baffled other doctors, and he is here in america to lecture on his subject.” Jung was the man of the hour. aged thirty-seven, he had just com- pleted a five-hundred-page magnum opus, Transformations and Sym‑ bols of the Libido, the second installment of which had just appeared in print. following his first visit to america in 1909, it was he, and not freud, who had been invited back by Smith ely Jelliffe to lec- ture on psychoanalysis in the new international extension course in medicine at fordham university, where he would also be awarded his second honorary degree (others invited included the psychiatrist William alanson White and the neurologist Henry Head). Jung’s initial title for his lectures was “Mental Mechanisms in Health and disease.” By the time he got to composing them, the title had become simply “the theory of Psychoanalysis.” Jung com- menced his introduction to the lectures by indicating that he in- tended to outline his attitude to freud’s guiding principles, noting that a reader would likely react with astonishment that it had taken him ten years to do so.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief History of the British Psychoanalytical Society
    A BRIEF HISTORY OF THE BRITISH PSYCHOANALYTICAL SOCIETY Ken Robinson When Ernest Jones set about establishing psychoanalysis in Britain, two intertwining tasks faced him: establishing the reputation of psychoanalysis as a respectable pursuit and defining an identity for it as a discipline that was distinct from but related to cognate disciplines. This latter concern with identity would remain central to the development of the British Society for decades to come, though its inflection would shift as the Society sought first to mark out British psychoanalysis as having its own character within the International Psychoanalytical Association, and then to find a way of holding together warring identities within the Society. Establishing Psychoanalysis: The London Society Ernest Jones’ diary for 1913 contains the simple entry for October 30: “Ψα meeting. Psycho-med. dinner” (Archives of the British Psychoanalytical Society, hereafter Archives). This was the first meeting of the London Psychoanalytical Society. In early August Jones had returned to London from ignominious exile in Canada after damaging accusations of inappropriate sexual conduct in relation to children. Having spent time in London and Europe the previous year, he now returned permanently, via Budapest where from June he had received analysis from Ferenczi. Once in London he wasted no time in beginning practice as a psychoanalyst, seeing his first patient on the 14th August (Diary 1913, Archives), though he would soon take a brief break to participate in what would turn out to be a troublesome Munich Congress in September (for Jones’s biography generally, see Maddox [2006]). Jones came back to a London that showed a growing interest in unconscious phenomena and abnormal psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Recent Advances in Psychoanalysis1
    RECENT ADVANCES IN PSYCHO-ANALYSIS’. BY ERNEST JONES. THE progress made in psycho-analytic knowledge during the past five or six years has been, in spite of the great external hindrances, very considerable, and in the attempt to present it one is met at the outset by two special di5culties. In the first place, the later researches have shown that most of the problems in question are a’good deal more complex than was perhaps at first realised, though such researches have naturally had to be based on the earlier work; it is therefore impossible to expound them without presupposing a knowledge of this earlier work, and I trust that this unavoidable fact will be borne in mind by those who find some of what follows too abstruse or abstract. The second digculty in exposition is a more technical one, and is due to the multi- plicity and variety of the contributions made during the past few years, which makes it hard to group or arrange them in any clear way. This difficulty I have dealt with mainly by the simple procedures of omission and selection. I shall not, for instance, touch on any branch of applied psycho-analysis except in the purely clinical field, and even here there are many interesting contributions with which I shall not be able to deal, among them being, to my regret, the valuable series of Pierce Clark’s on the subject of epilepsy(1). In the narrower field itself thus circumscribed no general review of the literature will be attempted, this being now fortunately accessible elsewhere(2),and I shall merely aim at calling attention to a few of what I consider to be the main respects in which advance in our knowledge has been made.
    [Show full text]
  • War and the Practice of Psychotherapy: the UK Experience 1939–1960
    Medical History, 2004, 48: 493–510 War and the Practice of Psychotherapy: The UK Experience 1939–1960 EDGAR JONES* During the Second World War, it is argued, ‘‘the neuroses of battle’’ not only deepened an understanding of ‘‘psychopathological mechanisms’’,1 but also created opportunities for the practice of psychotherapy, while its perceived efficacy led to a broader acceptance within medicine and society once peace had returned.2 This recognition is contrasted with the aftermath of the First World War when a network of outpatient clinics, set up by the Ministry of Pensions to treat veterans with shell shock, were closed within a few years in response to financial pressures and doubts about their therapeutic value. In the private sector, psychoanalysis under the leadership of Ernest Jones remained an idiosyncratic activity confined largely to the affluent middle classes of London.3 According to Gregorio Kohon, ‘‘it was strongly opposed by the general public, the Church, the medical and psychiatric establishment, and the press’’.4 The Medico-Psychological Clinic of London, originally set up in 1913, offered psychotherapy on three afternoons a week in premises at 30 Brunswick Square under the direction of Dr James Glover. However, it closed in 1923 after Glover and his brother Edward had both become psychoanalysts.5 As the First World War drew to a close, Maurice Craig helped to persuade Sir Ernest Cassel to fund a hospital for ‘Functional and Nervous Disorders’ at Penshurst, Kent, to treat neuroses in the civilian population.6 Although moved to permanent premises near Richmond, it remained small- scale and at the time no attempt was made to establish a network of similar institutions throughout the UK.
    [Show full text]
  • Classical Psychoanalytic Technique-A Causal Connection?
    Freud’s Trials Before Michelangelo’s Moses and the Classical Psychoanalytic Technique-a Causal Connection? Robert L. Lippman, Ph.D. How often have I mounted the steep steps from the unlovely Corso Cavour to the lonely piazza where the deserted church stands, and have essayed to support the angry scorn of the hero's [Moses'] glance! Sometimes I have crept cautiously out of the half-gloom of the interior . (Freud, “The Moses of Michelangelo,” 1914, 213.) The decisive part of the work is achieved by creating in the patient’s relation to the doctor--in ‘the transference’--new editions of old conflicts. (Freud, 1916- 1917, 454). The psychoanalyst as an individual must be unknown to the patient...Insofar as is humanly possible, he remains a peg on which the patient can hang conscious and unconscious fantasies.[By maintaining throughout his “ analytic incognito"], the psychoanalyst makes of himself a neutral sample of all humanity in the patient's emotional life. This is what gives value to the study of the origins of the patient's feelings. Toward the shadowy image of the psychoanalyst, the patient experiences anxiety, anger, hate, affection, jealousy, and the like... By unearthing the original sources of these feelings in early childhood, the analysis [of the transference] makes it possible to eliminate them, or at least to lessen their intensity and their influence....(Kubie,1975, 100-1). Freud’s first mention of transference—the key instrument of psychoanalysis-- appears in 1905, in his Postscript to Fragment of an analysis of a Case of Hysteria: ...it is only after the transference has been resolved that a patient arrives at a sense of conviction of the validity of the connections which have been constructed during the analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 This Is an Accepted Manuscript of an Article
    This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in the Transactional Analysis Journal, Vol.46, #2, 2016, 136-148. available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/0362153714559921 In Conflict and Community: A century of coming to terms with working and living in groups William F. Cornell Working draft submitted to Transactional Analysis Journal, 8/15 You do not know what wars are going on Down there where the spirit meets the bone (Miller Williams, 1999) Abstract: This article examines the century-long pairing of international wars with periods of resurgent interest in group psychotherapy and community based treatment. The group-centered theories of Freud, Burrow, Bion, Foulkes, and Berne are discussed and used to examine the qualities of group process and structure that enhance the capacity of members to work with conflict, difference, and societal projection. Key words: group psychotherapy, group-analysis, transactional analysis, Freud, Burrow, Bion, Foulkes, Berne, war, community War: The rending and reviving of societal relations There is a powerful and rather surprising pairing of the outbreak of major wars followed by experiments in group and community treatment. Though the international violence over the past century has often severed civilized ties, these periods of violent slaughter have then often engendered efforts of redress and restoration through group and community life. Through my many years of teaching and training in the mental health field, I have come to see the importance of studying the historical contexts within which the theories that inform our practices developed. I have also come to believe that the theories that form our work are efforts of marginalized individuals and their communities to make the unbearable bearable and to create spaces and places of belonging.
    [Show full text]
  • The Groupanalytic Orientation in the Training of Psychotherapists: the Teachings of S
    The groupanalytic orientation in the training of psychotherapists: the teachings of S. H. Foulkes1 Juan Campos Summary The author examines four aspects of Foulkes as a teacher: 1. the integration of his psychiatric and psychoanalytic training; 2. his experience as a teacher; 3. the training institutions he inspired and promoted; and 4. the experience of the ones who were his disciples. Comparing and relating the conditions of training in psychoanalysis and group analysis, the arguments are generated on which to base Foulkes’ conviction that group analysis is the best method for making effective the revolutionary discoveries of psychoanalysis on a wider front in psychotherapy and in training. 1 Campos, J. (1986) La orientación grupoanalítica en la formación de psicoterapeutas: el magisterio de S.H. Foulkes. En Martí Tusquets, J. L. y Sadne, L. (Ed.) La Formación en Psicoterapia de Grupo y Psicodrama (23-41). Barcelona: Argot. 1 S. H. Foulkes is one of the few psychoanalysts that are excused of saying «mea culpa» to which Balint invites them for their carelessness and negligence in the development of group psychotherapies.i His contribution at the theoretical level, Group Analysis, is only comparable to the contribution that he made in this field as an organizer of therapeutic services and educational programs and as a therapist of psychotherapists and group therapists. His educational activity moves in three directions: the training of psychoanalysts, the training of psychotherapists in the military and civil hospitals where he had clinical responsibility, and training of group psychotherapists —specifically group analysts. Balint suggests that it would be very interesting to make a historical —and psychopathological— study of why, in spite of Freud’s insinuations, psychoanalysis showed disinterest in the field of group psychotherapies.
    [Show full text]