Some Systematic Biases of Everyday Judgment

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Some Systematic Biases of Everyday Judgment Some systematic biases of everyday judgment http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1G1-19267325.html Home About Us Contact Us Help Search: Research Search tips Topics in the News: Some systematic biases of everyday judgment. From: Skeptical Inquirer | Date: 3/1/1997 | Author: Gilovich, Thomas Space: Final Frontier Print Digg del.icio.us Barack Obama 2008 Psychological research U.S. Attorney Firings shows that daily Sales Research Celebrities Largest behavior-based sales research project ever judgment and conducted. Misbehaving reasoning are biased in www.Huthwaite.com View more topics at predictable ways. Three Newser. common problems lead to judgment bias, Test in Dev. Psychology? namely, the 'compared Everyone Crams. You Can Cram Right. Browse Our Free Online Study Guides to what' problem, the www.CliffsNotes.com/DevPsych 'seek and ye shall find' problem and the selective memory problem. The scientific method was developed as an inferential safeguard for the inaccuracy of everyday judgment. Unfortunately, the scientific method is not widely taught and appreciated. Skeptics have long thought that everyday judgment and reasoning are biased in predictable ways. Psychological research on the subject conducted during the past quarter century largely confirms these suspicions. Two types of explanations are typically offered for the dubious beliefs that are dissected in SKEPTICAL INQUIRER. On one hand, there are motivational causes: Some beliefs are comforting, and so people embrace that comfort and convince 1 of 12 1/24/08 10:05 AM Some systematic biases of everyday judgment http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1G1-19267325.html themselves that a questionable proposition is true. Many types of religious beliefs, for example, are often explained this way. On the other hand, there are cognitive causes: faulty processes of reasoning and judgment that lead people to misevaluate the evidence of their everyday experience. The skeptical community is convinced that everyday judgment and reasoning leave much to be desired. Why are skeptics so unimpressed with the reasoning abilities and habits of the average person? Until recently, this pessimism was based on simple observation, often by those with a particularly keen eye for the foibles of human nature. Thus, skeptics often cite such thinkers as Francis Bacon, who stated: . all superstition is much the same whether it be that of astrology, dreams, omens, retributive judgment, or the like . [in that] the deluded believers observe events which are fulfilled, but neglect or pass over their failure, though it be much more common. (Bacon 1899/1620) John Stuart Mill and Bertrand Russell are two other classic scholars who, along with Bacon, are often quoted for their trenchant observations on the shortcomings of human judgment. It is also common to see similar quotes of more recent vintage - in SKEPTICAL INQUIRER and elsewhere - from the likes of Richard Feynman, Stephen Jay Gould, and Carl Sagan. During the past twenty-five years, a great deal of psychological Browse by research has dealt specifically with the quality of everyday alphabet: reasoning, and so it is now possible to go beyond simple A B C D E observation and arrive at a truly rigorous assessment of the shortcomings of everyday judgment. In so doing, we can F G H I J determine whether or not these scholars we all admire are K L M N O correct. Do people misevaluate evidence in the very ways and P Q R S T for the very reasons that Bacon, Russell, and others have claimed? Let us look at the research record and see. U V W X Y Z The "Compared to What?" Problem Need more Some of the common claims about the fallibility of human research? Get credible articles from reasoning stand up well to empirical scrutiny. For example, it is trusted sources at commonly argued that people have difficulty with what might HighBeam Research: be called the "compared to what" problem. That is, people are Newspaper archives often overly impressed with an absolute statistic without Magazine back issues 2 of 12 1/24/08 10:05 AM Some systematic biases of everyday judgment http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1G1-19267325.html recognizing that its true import can only be assessed by Academic journals comparison to some relevant baseline. Medical journals Nursing journals Psychology journals For instance, a 1986 article in Discover magazine (cited in Book reviews Dawes 1988) urges readers who fly in airplanes to "know And more! where the exits are and rehearse in your mind exactly how to get to them." Why? The article approvingly notes that someone who interviewed almost two hundred survivors of fatal airline accidents found that ". more than 90% had their escape routes mentally mapped out beforehand." Good for them, but note that whoever did the study cannot interview anyone who perished in an airplane crash. Air travel being as scary as it is to so many people, perhaps 90 percent or more of those who died in airline crashes rehearsed their escape routes as well. Ninety percent sounds impressive because it is so close to 100 percent. But without a more pertinent comparison, it really does not mean much. Similarly, people are often impressed that, say, 30 percent of all infertile couples who adopt a child subsequently conceive. That is great news for that 30 percent to be sure, but what percentage of those who do not adopt likewise conceive? People likewise draw broad conclusions from a cancer patient who goes into remission after steadfastly practicing mental imagery. Again, excellent news for that individual, but might the cancer have gone into remission even if the person had not practiced mental imagery? This problem of failing to invoke a relevant baseline of comparison is particularly common when the class of data that requires inspection is inherently difficult to collect. Consider, for example, the commonly expressed opinion, "I can always tell that someone is wearing a hairpiece." Are such claims to be believed, or is it just that one can tell that someone is wearing a hairpiece . when it is obvious that he is wearing a hair-piece? After all, how can one tell whether some have gone undetected? The goal of a good hairpiece is to fool the public, and so the example is one of those cases in which the confirmations speak loudly while the disconfirmations remain silent. A similar asymmetry should give pause to those who have extreme confidence in their "gaydar," or their ability to detect whether someone is gay. Here, too, the confirmations announce themselves. When a person for whatever reason "seems gay" and it is later determined that he is, it is a salient 3 of 12 1/24/08 10:05 AM Some systematic biases of everyday judgment http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1G1-19267325.html triumph for one's skill at detection. But people who elude one's gaydar rarely go out of their way to announce, "By the way, I fooled you: I'm gay." At any rate, the notion that people have difficulty invoking relevant comparisons has received support from psychological research. Studies of everyday reasoning have shown that the logic and necessity of control groups, for example, is often lost on a large segment of even the educated population (Boring 1954; Einhorn and Hogarth 1978; Nisbett and Ross 1980). The "Seek and Ye Shall Find" Problem Another common claim that stands up well to empirical research is the idea that people do not assess hypotheses even-handedly. Rather, they tend to seek out confirmatory evidence for what they suspect to be true, a tendency that has the effect of "seek and ye shall find." A biased search for confirmatory information frequently turns up more apparent support for a hypothesis than is justified. This phenomenon has been demonstrated in numerous experiments explicitly designed to assess people's hypothesis-testing strategies (Skov and Sherman 1986; Snyder and Swann 1978). But it is so pervasive that it can also be seen in studies designed with an entirely different agenda in mind. One of my personal favorites is a study in which participants were given the following information (Shafir 1993): Imagine that you serve on the jury of an only-child sole-custody case following a relatively messy divorce. The facts of the case are complicated by ambiguous economic, social, and emotional considerations, and you decide to base your decision entirely on the following few observations. To which parent would you award sole custody of the child? Parent A: average income average health average working hours reasonable rapport with the child relatively stable social life Parent B: above-average income minor health 4 of 12 1/24/08 10:05 AM Some systematic biases of everyday judgment http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1G1-19267325.html problems lots of work-related travel very close relationship with the child extremely active social life Faced with this version of the problem, the majority of respondents chose to award custody to Parent B, the "mixed bag" parent who offers several advantages (above-average income), but also some disadvantages (health problems), in comparison to Parent A. In another version of the problem, however, a different group is asked to which parent they would deny custody of the child. Here, too, a majority selects Parent B. Parent B, then, is paradoxically deemed both more and less worthy of caring for the child. The result is paradoxical, that is, unless one takes into account people's tendencies to seek out confirming information. Asked which parent should be awarded the child, people look primarily for positive qualities that warrant being awarded the child - looking less vigilantly for negative characteristics that would lead one to favor the other parent. When asked which parent should be denied custody, on the other hand, people look primarily for negative qualities that would disqualify a parent.
Recommended publications
  • The Art of Thinking Clearly (577H) Sources.Indd
    THE ART OF THINKING CLEARLY A NOTE ON SOURCES Hundreds of studies have been conducted on the vast majority of cognitive and behav- ioural errors. In a scholarly work, the complete reference section would easily double the pages of this book. I have focused on the most important quotes, technical refer- ences, recommendations for further reading and comments. The knowledge encompassed in this book is based on the research carried out in the fields of cognitive and social psychology over the past three decades. SURVIVORSHIP BIAS Survivorship bias in funds and stock market indices, see: Edwin J. Elton, Martin J. Gruber & Christopher R. Blake, ‘Survivorship Bias and Mutual Fund Performance’, The Review of Financial Studies, volume 9, number 4, 1996, 1097–1120. Statistically relevant results by coincidence (self-selection), see: John P. A. Ioan- nidis, ‘Why Most Published Research Findings Are False’, PLoS Med 2(8), 2005, e124. SWIMMER’S BODY ILLUSION Nassim Nicholas Taleb, The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable (New York: Random House, 2007), 109–110. ‘Ideally, the comparison should be made between people who went to Harvard and people who were admitted to Harvard but chose instead to go to Podunk State. Unfortunately, this is likely to produce samples too small for statistical analysis.’ Thomas Sowell, Economic Facts and Fallacies (New York: Basic Books, 2008), 106. David Lykken & Auke Tellegen, ‘Happiness Is a Stochastic Phenomenon’, Psycho- logical Science, Vol.7, No. 3, May 1996, 189. In his book Good to Great, Jim Collins cites the CEO of Pitney Bowes, Dave Nassef: ‘I used to be in the Marines, and the Marines get a lot of credit for building people’s values.
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics and Biases the Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. In
    P1: FYX/FYX P2: FYX/UKS QC: FCH/UKS T1: FCH CB419-Gilovich CB419-Gilovich-FM May 30, 2002 12:3 HEURISTICS AND BIASES The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment Edited by THOMAS GILOVICH Cornell University DALE GRIFFIN Stanford University DANIEL KAHNEMAN Princeton University iii P1: FYX/FYX P2: FYX/UKS QC: FCH/UKS T1: FCH CB419-Gilovich CB419-Gilovich-FM May 30, 2002 12:3 published by the press syndicate of the university of cambridge The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom cambridge university press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA 477 Williamstown, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia Ruiz de Alarcon´ 13, 28014, Madrid, Spain Dock House, The Waterfront, Cape Town 8001, South Africa http://www.cambridge.org C Cambridge University Press 2002 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2002 Printed in the United States of America Typeface Palatino 9.75/12.5 pt. System LATEX2ε [TB] A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library. Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication data Heuristics and biases : the psychology of intuitive judgment / edited by Thomas Gilovich, Dale Griffin, Daniel Kahneman. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-521-79260-6 – ISBN 0-521-79679-2 (pbk.) 1. Judgment. 2. Reasoning (Psychology) 3. Critical thinking. I. Gilovich, Thomas. II. Griffin, Dale III. Kahneman, Daniel, 1934– BF447 .H48 2002 153.4 – dc21 2001037860 ISBN 0 521 79260 6 hardback ISBN 0 521 79679 2 paperback iv P1: FYX/FYX P2: FYX/UKS QC: FCH/UKS T1: FCH CB419-Gilovich CB419-Gilovich-FM May 30, 2002 12:3 Contents List of Contributors page xi Preface xv Introduction – Heuristics and Biases: Then and Now 1 Thomas Gilovich and Dale Griffin PART ONE.
    [Show full text]
  • How Should We Think About Americans' Beliefs About Economic Mobility?
    Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 13, No. 3, May 2018, pp. 297–304 How should we think about Americans’ beliefs about economic mobility? Shai Davidai∗ Thomas Gilovich† Abstract Recent evidence suggests that Americans’ beliefs about upward mobility are overly optimistic. Davidai & Gilovich (2015a), Kraus & Tan (2015), and Kraus (2015) all found that people overestimate the likelihood that a person might rise up the economic ladder, and underestimate the likelihood that they might fail to do so. However, using a different methodology, Chambers, Swan and Heesacker (2015) reported that Americans’ beliefs about mobility are much more pessimistic. Swan, Chambers, Heesacker and Nero (2017) provide a much-needed summary of these conflicting findings and question the utility of measuring population-level biases in judgments of inequality and mobility. We value their summary but argue that their conclusion is premature. By focusing on measures that best tap how laypeople naturally think about the distribution of income, we believe that researchers can draw meaningful conclusions about the public’s perceptions of economic mobility. When more ecologically representative measures are used, the consistent finding is that Americans overestimate the extent of upward mobility in the United States. To explain the divergent findings in the literature, we provide evidence that the methods used by Chambers et al. (2015) inadvertently primed participants to think about immobility rather than mobility. Finally, using a novel method to examine beliefs about economic
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Traffic-Light Labels and Time Pressure on Estimating
    THE EFFECT OF TRAFFIC-LIGHT LABELS AND TIME PRESSURE ON ESTIMATING KILOCALORIES AND CARBON FOOTPRINT OF FOOD Luca A. Panzonea, Falko F Sniehottab, Rob Comberc, Fred Lemked a School of Natural and Environmental Science, Newcastle University (UK); email: [email protected] b Institute for Health and Society, Newcastle University (UK); email: [email protected] c RISE Research Institute (Sweden); email: [email protected] d Vlerick Business School, Ghent University (Belgium). [email protected] Abstract Food consumption decisions require consumers to evaluate the characteristics of products. However, the literature has given limited attention to how consumers determine the impact of food on health (e.g., kilocalories) and on the environment (e.g., carbon footprint). In this exercise, 1,511 consumers categorised 43 food products as healthy/unhealthy and good/bad for the environment, and estimated their kilocalories and carbon footprint, which were known to the investigator. The task was performed either with no stimuli (a control group), under time pressure only, with traffic-light labels only, or both. Results show that traffic-light labels: 1) operate through improvements in knowledge, rather than facilitating information processing under pressure; 2) improve the ability to rank products by both kilocalories and carbon footprint, rather than the ability to use the metric; 3) reduce the threshold used to categorise products as unhealthy/bad for the environment, whilst raising the threshold used to classify products as good for the environment (but not healthy). Notably, traffic-light increase accuracy by reducing the response compression of the metric scale. The benefits of labels are particularly evident for carbon footprint.
    [Show full text]
  • The Spotlight Effect in Social Judgment: an Egocentric Bias in Estimates of the Salience of One's Own Actions and Appearance
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2000 by the Ainerican Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol. 78, No. 2, 211-222 0022-3514100/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.2.211 The Spotlight Effect in Social Judgment: An Egocentric Bias in Estimates of the Salience of One's Own Actions and Appearance Thomas Gilovich Victoria Husted Medvec Cornell University Northwestern University Kenneth Savitsky Williams College This research provides evidence that people overestimate the extent to which their actions and appearance are noted by others, a phenomenon dubbed the spotlight effect. In Studies 1 and 2, participants who were asked to don a T-shirt depicting either a flattering or potentially embarrassing image overestimated the number of observers who would be able to recall what was pictured on the shirt. In Study 3, participants in a group discussion overestimated how prominent their positive and negative utterances were to their fellow discussants. Studies 4 and 5 provide evidence supporting an anchoring-and-adjustment interpre- tation of the spotlight effect. In particular, people appear to anchor on their own rich phenomenological experience and then adjust--insufficiently--to take into account the perspective of others. The discussion focuses on the manifestations and implications of the spotlight effect across a host of everyday social phenomena. Most of us stand out in our own minds. Whether in the midst of view as extraordinary and memorable go unnoticed or underap- a personal triumph or an embarrassing mishap, we are usually preciated by others. The same is true of the actions we wish to quite focused on what is happening to us, its significance to our disown because they reflect poorly on our ability or character.
    [Show full text]
  • Insights from Psychology: Teaching Behavioral Legal Ethics As a Core Element of Professional Responsibility
    INSIGHTS FROM PSYCHOLOGY: TEACHING BEHAVIORAL LEGAL ETHICS AS A CORE ELEMENT OF PROFESSIONAL RESPONSIBILITY Tigran W. Eldred* 2016 MICH. ST. L. REV. 757 ABSTRACT The field of behavioral legal ethics—which draws on a large body of empirical research to explore how subtle and often unconscious psychological factors influence ethical decision-making by lawyers—has gained significant attention recently, including by many scholars who have called for a pedagogy that incorporates behavioral lessons into the professional responsibility curriculum. This Article provides one of the first comprehensive accounts of how law teachers can meet this challenge. Based on an approach that employs a variety of experiential techniques to immerse students in the contextual and emotional aspects of legal practice, it provides a detailed model of how to teach legal ethics from a behavioral perspective. Reflections on the approach, including the encouraging response expressed by students to this interdisciplinary method of instruction, are also discussed. TABLE OF CONTENTS I. INTRODUCTION ....................................................................... 758 II. TEACHING BEHAVIORAL LEGAL ETHICS ................................ 762 A. Understanding Behavioral Science ................................ 762 B. Teaching Methodology .................................................. 767 * Professor, New England Law | Boston. Special thanks to Lawrence Friedman, K. Babe Howell, Vivien Holmes, Leslie Levin, Catherine Gage O’Grady, Robert Prentice, Jordan Singer, Jean Sternlight, Jennifer Stumpf, Juliet Stumpf, and Molly Wilson for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this Article. Thanks also to the American Bar Association’s 41st National Conference on Professional Responsibility and CUNY Law School’s faculty colloquium, where earlier versions of this Article were presented; and to my excellent research assistants, Justin Amos and Julianna Zitz. 758 Michigan State Law Review 2016 1.
    [Show full text]
  • The Illusion of Transparency and the Alleviation of Speech Anxietyq
    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 618–625 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp The illusion of transparency and the alleviation of speech anxietyq Kenneth Savitskya,* and Thomas Gilovichb a Department of Psychology, Bronfman Science Center, Williams College, Williamstown, MA 01267, USA b Department of Psychology, Uris Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA Received 1 July 2002; revised 25 March 2003 Abstract Individuals often believe their internal states are more apparent to others than is actually the case, a phenomenon known as the illusion of transparency. In the domain of public speaking, for example, individuals who are nervous about delivering a public speech believe their nervousness is more apparent to their audience than it actually is, a finding we document in Study 1. We contend that the illusion of transparency can play a role in the self-exacerbating nature of speech anxiety, and show in Study 2 that an awareness of the illusion can improve the quality of a speakerÕs performance, from both the speakerÕs own perspective and in the eyes of observers. Discussion focuses on the application of these findings to the treatment of speech anxiety and other forms of social anxiety. Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved. I turn pale at the outset of a speech and quake in every limb and McFarland, 1987, 1991; Vorauer & Ross, 1999). This in all my soul. illusion derives from the difficulty people have in getting Cicero, De Oratore beyond their own phenomenological experience when Individuals often wish to conceal their internal states. attempting to determine how they appear to others.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessments of Bias 1 the Bias Blind Spot and Its Implications Lee Ross
    Assessments of Bias 1 The Bias Blind Spot and its Implications Lee Ross Department of Psychology Stanford University Joyce Ehrlinger Department of Psychology Florida State University Thomas Gilovich Department of Psychology Cornell University To appear in Contemporary Organizational Behavior in Action. Elsbach, K.D. & Kayes, A.B. (Eds.), Pearson Prentice Hall. Assessments of Bias 2 Everyday events often prompt us to assess the objectivity of opinions and judgments— our own, those of our friends and colleagues, and those of jurists, political leaders, and advocates for interest groups. Was our decision to promote a favored employee tainted by a desire to nurture a friendship or was it the result of an even-handed evaluation of her qualifications? Was the television pundit’s bleak assessment of national healthcare influenced by the political leanings of the network’s owners or was it a fair evaluation of the proposals on the table? Was a presidential decision to go to war a reflection of cool-headed assessments of national interest or the result of ideological blindness, unwarranted optimism, or personal anger? Indeed, are our assessments of whether our own or others’ judgments are biased themselves the product of our personal biases? A wealth of evidence suggests that people’s judgments are subject to a number of cognitive and motivational biases1. Individuals consistently rate themselves above average across a variety of domains2, take credit for their successes but explain away their failures3, and detect more support for deeply held beliefs than is objectively warranted4. Furthermore, people suffer from a “bias blind spot,” or the conviction that their own judgments are less susceptible to bias than the judgments of others5.
    [Show full text]
  • AMIT KUMAR 2110 Speedway, B6700, Austin, Texas, 78712 [email protected] | (973) 722-2510 Current Position
    AMIT KUMAR 2110 Speedway, B6700, Austin, Texas, 78712 [email protected] | (973) 722-2510 http://www.kumar-amit.com Current Position 2018- THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT AUSTIN, AUSTIN, TX Assistant Professor of Marketing and Psychology Education 2010-2015 CORNELL UNIVERSITY, ITHACA, NY Ph.D. in SociaL and PersonaLity Psychology Dissertation: “From Looking Backward to Looking Forward: On Consuming the Anticipation of ExperientiaL and MateriaL Purchases” Committee Chair: Tom GiLovicH Committee Members: MeLissa Ferguson and Dave Dunning 2004-2008 HARVARD UNIVERSITY, CAMBRIDGE, MA A.B. Magna Cum Laude with Highest Honors in Psychology (Secondary FieLd: Economics) Honors Thesis: “The Impact of Outcome VaLence on Perceived Duration” Advisor: Dan GiLbert Reader: Max Bazerman Honors and Awards 2017 University of Chicago Center for Decision Research Independent Research Grant ($3000) 2016 Society for ExperimentaL SociaL Psychology Dissertation Award FinaList 2011-2015 NationaL Science Foundation Pre-DoctoraL FeLLow ($96,000 + tuition supplement) 2014 CorneLL University TraveL Grant ($675) 2014 CorneLL University John S. Knight FeLLowship ($11,735) 2014 Judgment and Decision-Making SPSP Pre-Conference TraveL Award ($200) 2014 CorneLL University TraveL Grant ($440) 2013 FeLLow, Summer Institute in SociaL and PersonaLity Psychology 2013 Summer Institute in SociaL and PersonaLity Psychology TraveL Award ($300) 2013 Society for PersonaLity and SociaL Psychology Graduate Student TraveL Award ($500) 2013 Judgment and Decision-Making SPSP Pre-Conference
    [Show full text]
  • The Dark Side of Self- and Social Perception: Black Uniforms and Aggression in Professional Sports
    Journal of Pereonality and Social Psychology Copyrighl 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1988, Vol. 54, No. 1,74-85 0022-3514/88/$00.75 The Dark Side of Self- and Social Perception: Black Uniforms and Aggression in Professional Sports Mark G. Frank and Thomas Gilovich Cornell University Black is viewed as the color of evil and death in virtually all cultures. With this association in mind, we were interested in whether a cue as subtle as the color of a person's clothing might have a signifi- cant impact on his or her behavior. To test this possibility, we examined whether professional football and ice hockey teams that wear black uniforms are more aggressive than those that wear nonblack uniforms. An analysis of the penalty records of the National Football League and the National Hockey League indicate that teams with black uniforms in both sports ranked near the top of their leagues in penalties throughout the period of study. On those occasions when a team switched from nonblack to black uniforms, the switch was accompanied by an immediate increase in penalties. The results of two laboratory experiments indicate that this finding can be attributed to both social perception and self-perception processes—that is, to the biased judgments of referees and to the increased aggressiveness of the players themselves. Our discussion focuses on the theoretical implica- tions of these data for an understanding of the variable, or "situated," nature of the self. A convenient feature of the traditional American Western over, this association between black and evil is not strictly an film was the ease with which the viewer could distinguish the American or Western phenomenon, because college students in good guys from the bad guys: The bad guys wore the black hats.
    [Show full text]
  • When Effortful Thinking Influences Judgmental Anchoring
    Journal of Behavioral Decision Making J. Behav. Dec. Making, 18: 199–212 (2005) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/bdm.495 When Effortful Thinking Influences Judgmental Anchoring: Differential Effects of Forewarning and Incentives on Self-generated and Externally Provided Anchors NICHOLAS EPLEY1* and THOMAS GILOVICH2 1University of Chicago, USA 2Cornell University,USA ABSTRACT Two experiments examined the impact of financial incentives and forewarnings on judgmental anchoring effects, or the tendency for judgments of uncertain qualities to be biased in the direction of salient anchor values. Previous research has found no effect of either manipulation on the magnitude of anchoring effects. We argue, however, that anchoring effects are produced by multiple mechanisms—one involving an effortful process of adjustment from ‘‘self-generated’’ anchors, and another involving the biased recruitment of anchor-consistent information from ‘‘externally provided’’ anchors— and that only the former should be influenced by incentives and forewarning. Two stu- dies confirmed these predictions, showing that responses to ‘‘self-generated’’ anchors are influenced by both incentives and forewarnings whereas responses to ‘‘externally provided’’ anchors are not. Discussion focuses on the implications of these effects for debiasing efforts. Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. key words anchoring; bias; incentives; effortful thought INTRODUCTION The fatal tendency of mankind to leave off thinking about a thing when it is no longer doubtful is the cause of half their errors. (J. S. Mill, 1859/1975, On liberty, p. 105) Although one may argue with Mill’s figure, it is hard to argue with his conclusion. Everyday life confronts us with a wide range of complicated problems, and at least some of the errors in thinking about these * Correspondence to: Nicholas Epley, University of Chicago, Graduate School of Business, 5807 South Woodlawn Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics and Biases at the Bargaining Table Russell Korobkin
    Marquette Law Review Volume 87 Article 17 Issue 4 Special Issue Heuristics: Heuristics and Biases at the Bargaining Table Russell Korobkin Chris Guthrie Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.law.marquette.edu/mulr Part of the Law Commons Repository Citation Russell Korobkin and Chris Guthrie, Heuristics: Heuristics and Biases at the Bargaining Table, 87 Marq. L. Rev. (2004). Available at: http://scholarship.law.marquette.edu/mulr/vol87/iss4/17 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals at Marquette Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Marquette Law Review by an authorized administrator of Marquette Law Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. HEURISTICS AND BIASES AT THE BARGAINING TABLE RUSSELL KOROBKIN* & CHRIS GUTHRIE** I. INTRODUCTION Negotiation is an inherently interpersonal activity that nonetheless requires each participant to make individualjudgments and decisions. Each negotiator must evaluate a proposed agreement, assess its value and the value of alternative courses of action-such as continuing to negotiate or pursuing an alternative transaction-and ultimately choose whether to accept or reject the proposal. The interdisciplinary field of "decision theory" offers both a normative account (how should individuals act) and descriptive accounts (how do individuals act) of decision making in the negotiation context. According to the normative model, negotiators should compare the subjective expected value of an agreement to the subjective expected value of non-agreement, taking into account such factors as risks, differential transaction costs, and reputational and relational consequences of each possible course of action.' Once a negotiator has calculated the expected value of each course of action,2 the negotiator should then select the one that promises the greatest return.
    [Show full text]