The Illusion of Transparency and the Alleviation of Speech Anxietyq

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Illusion of Transparency and the Alleviation of Speech Anxietyq Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 618–625 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp The illusion of transparency and the alleviation of speech anxietyq Kenneth Savitskya,* and Thomas Gilovichb a Department of Psychology, Bronfman Science Center, Williams College, Williamstown, MA 01267, USA b Department of Psychology, Uris Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA Received 1 July 2002; revised 25 March 2003 Abstract Individuals often believe their internal states are more apparent to others than is actually the case, a phenomenon known as the illusion of transparency. In the domain of public speaking, for example, individuals who are nervous about delivering a public speech believe their nervousness is more apparent to their audience than it actually is, a finding we document in Study 1. We contend that the illusion of transparency can play a role in the self-exacerbating nature of speech anxiety, and show in Study 2 that an awareness of the illusion can improve the quality of a speakerÕs performance, from both the speakerÕs own perspective and in the eyes of observers. Discussion focuses on the application of these findings to the treatment of speech anxiety and other forms of social anxiety. Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved. I turn pale at the outset of a speech and quake in every limb and McFarland, 1987, 1991; Vorauer & Ross, 1999). This in all my soul. illusion derives from the difficulty people have in getting Cicero, De Oratore beyond their own phenomenological experience when Individuals often wish to conceal their internal states. attempting to determine how they appear to others. The Anxiety over approaching a potential romantic partner, adjustment one makes from the ‘‘anchor’’ of oneÕs own feelings of disgust over a disagreeable entreee served at a phenomenology, like adjustments to anchors generally, dinner party, or nervousness over delivering a public tends to be insufficient (Chapman & Johnson, 2002; speech—all are internal states one may wish, for a va- Epley & Gilovich, 2001, 2003; Jacowitz & Kahneman, riety of reasons, to keep private. 1995; Savitsky & MacIntosh, 2003). As a result, people How well can people conceal their internal states, exaggerate the extent to which their internal states and how well do they believe they can do so? Research ‘‘leak out’’ and overestimate the extent to which others suggests that individuals are typically better at dis- can detect their private feelings. guising their internal states than they believe—i.e., The illusion of transparency has been documented in people are prone to an illusion of transparency, or a a variety of domains and across a number of procedural belief that their thoughts, feelings, and emotions are variations (Gilovich & Savitsky, 1999). In one study, more apparent to others than is actually the case participants who were asked to tell lies believed they had (Gilovich, Savitsky, & Medvec, 1998; Miller & leaked more cues to their deception than they actually had and overestimated the extent to which others could detect their falsehoods. In another study, participants q Portions of this research were presented at the annual meeting of who sampled foul-tasting drinks in view of an observer the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, 1998. This research believed that their disgust was more apparent than was was supported by Research Grant MH45531 from the National actually the case (Gilovich et al., 1998). Other research Institute of Mental Health and SBR9319558 from the National Science has demonstrated that individuals overestimate the ex- Foundation. We thank Adi Bar-Lev and Lucy Berk for serving as experimenters in Study 2. tent to which their internal states are apparent to others * Corresponding author. Fax: 1-413-597-2085. in contexts as varied as negotiations (Van Boven, E-mail address: [email protected] (K. Savitsky). Gilovich, & Medvec, in press; Vorauer & Claude, 1998), 0022-1031/$ - see front matter Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00056-8 K. Savitsky, T. Gilovich / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 618–625 619 police interrogations (Kassin & Fong, 1999), and po- illusion of transparency, their anxiety can become self- tential emergencies (Gilovich et al., 1998). perpetuating. An individual who experiences some One purpose of the present investigation is to expand anxiety while giving a speech may believe it is more further the range of situations to which the illusion of apparent to the audience than is actually the case. This transparency applies by investigating a domain in which thought—that the audience is aware of just how nervous such a misconception can be both profound and con- he or she feels—may ironically serve to make the speaker sequential: public speaking. Many individuals report all the more nervous. The speaker may then believe that experiencing significant anxiety when called upon to this newfound nervousness is itself apparent to others, speak in public (Leary & Kowalski, 1995; McCroskey, leading to still more nervousness, concerns about leak- 1 1970, 1977)—anxiety that can interfere with oneÕs social age, and so on. life and education, lead to job absenteeism, and even Speech anxiety may thus be an example of what force one to change careers (Hohenstein, 1986; Monroe, Storms and McCaul (1976) have termed a ‘‘self-exac- Borzi, & Burrell, 1992; Stein, Walker, & Forde, 1996). erbating syndrome.’’ They note that the experience of These individuals may anchor on their internal sensa- certain conditions, such as stuttering, insomnia, or tions of anxiety, correct insufficiently for the fact that writerÕs block, can lead to an ironic increase in those others are less privy to those sensations than they are very conditions (cf., Ascher & Schotte, 1999; Wegner, themselves, and consequently overestimate the extent to 1997; Wegner, Broome, & Blumberg, 1997). We believe which their anxiety is apparent to onlookers. As Miller the illusion of transparency may lend a similar self- and McFarland (1991) note, ‘‘in anxiety-provoking sit- exacerbating quality to speech anxiety. At the same uations, it is often very difficult for people to believe time, however, the realization that oneÕs nervousness is that, despite feeling highly nervous, they do not appear less apparent than one thinks may be useful in allevi- highly nervous’’ (p. 310). ating speech anxiety: If individuals can be convinced Indeed, the literature on speech anxiety provides that their internal sensations are not manifested in some evidence consistent with this possibility. In par- their external appearance, one source of their anxiety ticular, several studies have shown discrepancies be- can be attenuated, allowing them to relax and even tween speakersÕ self-ratings and ratings provided by improving the quality of their performance. Thus, others. Behnke, Sawyer, and King (1987) found that speakers who know about the illusion of transparency public speakers felt more nervous than audience mem- may tend to give better speeches than speakers who do bers believed they did, and Rapee and Lim (1992) found not. that participants evaluated their own speeches more We put this speculation to empirical test in Study 2, negatively than they were evaluated by their audience. examining whether knowledge of the illusion of trans- In the only existing study that reports the key compar- parency can alleviate speech anxiety. But first we seek, in ison between how speakers thought they would be Study 1, to document the illusion of transparency in the evaluated by others and how others actually evaluated domain of public speaking. them, Mansell and Clark (1999) found that socially anxious individuals did indeed overestimate how anx- ious they appeared during an extemporaneous speech Study 1 performance, relative to the judgments of observers. Unfortunately, however, their report does not permit an Participants delivered extemporaneous speeches in assessment of whether this effect held true for their pairs. Each speaker then rated the degree to which he or sample as a whole. she appeared nervous during his or her speech, as well Given the uncertain nature of the existing evidence on as the extent to which his or her counterpart appeared this issue, our first goal in the present investigation was nervous. We expected speakers to overestimate the to collect unambiguous evidence of an illusion of transparency in the domain of public speaking. Our second goal was to explore a significant implication of 1 In fact, an individual need not even experience any bona fide the illusion of transparency—how it can exacerbate the nervousness to set this cycle in motion. Normal preparatory arousal, intensity of speech anxiety, and, more important, how stemming from the simple need to ‘‘get with it’’ and mobilize whatever cognitive and motor programs are required to perform the task at an awareness of the illusion (that is, an appreciation that hand, may be misattributed to pangs of anxiety and self-doubt. As oneÕs anxiety is typically not as apparent to others as one Olson (1988) notes, ‘‘the distinction between arousal caused by suspects) can help alleviate that anxiety. Many people Ôgearing up for a speechÕ and arousal caused by Ôspeech anxietyÕ is report that one component of their fear of public subtle’’ (p. 766). Thus, the individual may reason heuristically and speaking is the concern that they will ‘‘tremble, shake, or conclude, ‘‘If I am feeling these feelings of arousal, I must be nervous about giving this speech’’ (Savitsky, Medvec, Charlton, & Gilovich, show some other signs of anxiety’’ (Stein et al., 1996, p. 1998). The concern that such feelings are apparent to others, then, may 172). In other words, people are often nervous about give rise to a more genuine experience of anxiety, which the individual looking nervous. If public speakers are prone to an may think is also leaking out, and so on. 620 K. Savitsky, T. Gilovich / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39 (2003) 618–625 extent to which their nervousness was apparent, and order: first vs.
Recommended publications
  • The Art of Thinking Clearly (577H) Sources.Indd
    THE ART OF THINKING CLEARLY A NOTE ON SOURCES Hundreds of studies have been conducted on the vast majority of cognitive and behav- ioural errors. In a scholarly work, the complete reference section would easily double the pages of this book. I have focused on the most important quotes, technical refer- ences, recommendations for further reading and comments. The knowledge encompassed in this book is based on the research carried out in the fields of cognitive and social psychology over the past three decades. SURVIVORSHIP BIAS Survivorship bias in funds and stock market indices, see: Edwin J. Elton, Martin J. Gruber & Christopher R. Blake, ‘Survivorship Bias and Mutual Fund Performance’, The Review of Financial Studies, volume 9, number 4, 1996, 1097–1120. Statistically relevant results by coincidence (self-selection), see: John P. A. Ioan- nidis, ‘Why Most Published Research Findings Are False’, PLoS Med 2(8), 2005, e124. SWIMMER’S BODY ILLUSION Nassim Nicholas Taleb, The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable (New York: Random House, 2007), 109–110. ‘Ideally, the comparison should be made between people who went to Harvard and people who were admitted to Harvard but chose instead to go to Podunk State. Unfortunately, this is likely to produce samples too small for statistical analysis.’ Thomas Sowell, Economic Facts and Fallacies (New York: Basic Books, 2008), 106. David Lykken & Auke Tellegen, ‘Happiness Is a Stochastic Phenomenon’, Psycho- logical Science, Vol.7, No. 3, May 1996, 189. In his book Good to Great, Jim Collins cites the CEO of Pitney Bowes, Dave Nassef: ‘I used to be in the Marines, and the Marines get a lot of credit for building people’s values.
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics and Biases the Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. In
    P1: FYX/FYX P2: FYX/UKS QC: FCH/UKS T1: FCH CB419-Gilovich CB419-Gilovich-FM May 30, 2002 12:3 HEURISTICS AND BIASES The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment Edited by THOMAS GILOVICH Cornell University DALE GRIFFIN Stanford University DANIEL KAHNEMAN Princeton University iii P1: FYX/FYX P2: FYX/UKS QC: FCH/UKS T1: FCH CB419-Gilovich CB419-Gilovich-FM May 30, 2002 12:3 published by the press syndicate of the university of cambridge The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom cambridge university press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA 477 Williamstown, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia Ruiz de Alarcon´ 13, 28014, Madrid, Spain Dock House, The Waterfront, Cape Town 8001, South Africa http://www.cambridge.org C Cambridge University Press 2002 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2002 Printed in the United States of America Typeface Palatino 9.75/12.5 pt. System LATEX2ε [TB] A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library. Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication data Heuristics and biases : the psychology of intuitive judgment / edited by Thomas Gilovich, Dale Griffin, Daniel Kahneman. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-521-79260-6 – ISBN 0-521-79679-2 (pbk.) 1. Judgment. 2. Reasoning (Psychology) 3. Critical thinking. I. Gilovich, Thomas. II. Griffin, Dale III. Kahneman, Daniel, 1934– BF447 .H48 2002 153.4 – dc21 2001037860 ISBN 0 521 79260 6 hardback ISBN 0 521 79679 2 paperback iv P1: FYX/FYX P2: FYX/UKS QC: FCH/UKS T1: FCH CB419-Gilovich CB419-Gilovich-FM May 30, 2002 12:3 Contents List of Contributors page xi Preface xv Introduction – Heuristics and Biases: Then and Now 1 Thomas Gilovich and Dale Griffin PART ONE.
    [Show full text]
  • How Should We Think About Americans' Beliefs About Economic Mobility?
    Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 13, No. 3, May 2018, pp. 297–304 How should we think about Americans’ beliefs about economic mobility? Shai Davidai∗ Thomas Gilovich† Abstract Recent evidence suggests that Americans’ beliefs about upward mobility are overly optimistic. Davidai & Gilovich (2015a), Kraus & Tan (2015), and Kraus (2015) all found that people overestimate the likelihood that a person might rise up the economic ladder, and underestimate the likelihood that they might fail to do so. However, using a different methodology, Chambers, Swan and Heesacker (2015) reported that Americans’ beliefs about mobility are much more pessimistic. Swan, Chambers, Heesacker and Nero (2017) provide a much-needed summary of these conflicting findings and question the utility of measuring population-level biases in judgments of inequality and mobility. We value their summary but argue that their conclusion is premature. By focusing on measures that best tap how laypeople naturally think about the distribution of income, we believe that researchers can draw meaningful conclusions about the public’s perceptions of economic mobility. When more ecologically representative measures are used, the consistent finding is that Americans overestimate the extent of upward mobility in the United States. To explain the divergent findings in the literature, we provide evidence that the methods used by Chambers et al. (2015) inadvertently primed participants to think about immobility rather than mobility. Finally, using a novel method to examine beliefs about economic
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Traffic-Light Labels and Time Pressure on Estimating
    THE EFFECT OF TRAFFIC-LIGHT LABELS AND TIME PRESSURE ON ESTIMATING KILOCALORIES AND CARBON FOOTPRINT OF FOOD Luca A. Panzonea, Falko F Sniehottab, Rob Comberc, Fred Lemked a School of Natural and Environmental Science, Newcastle University (UK); email: [email protected] b Institute for Health and Society, Newcastle University (UK); email: [email protected] c RISE Research Institute (Sweden); email: [email protected] d Vlerick Business School, Ghent University (Belgium). [email protected] Abstract Food consumption decisions require consumers to evaluate the characteristics of products. However, the literature has given limited attention to how consumers determine the impact of food on health (e.g., kilocalories) and on the environment (e.g., carbon footprint). In this exercise, 1,511 consumers categorised 43 food products as healthy/unhealthy and good/bad for the environment, and estimated their kilocalories and carbon footprint, which were known to the investigator. The task was performed either with no stimuli (a control group), under time pressure only, with traffic-light labels only, or both. Results show that traffic-light labels: 1) operate through improvements in knowledge, rather than facilitating information processing under pressure; 2) improve the ability to rank products by both kilocalories and carbon footprint, rather than the ability to use the metric; 3) reduce the threshold used to categorise products as unhealthy/bad for the environment, whilst raising the threshold used to classify products as good for the environment (but not healthy). Notably, traffic-light increase accuracy by reducing the response compression of the metric scale. The benefits of labels are particularly evident for carbon footprint.
    [Show full text]
  • The Spotlight Effect in Social Judgment: an Egocentric Bias in Estimates of the Salience of One's Own Actions and Appearance
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2000 by the Ainerican Psychological Association, Inc. 2000, Vol. 78, No. 2, 211-222 0022-3514100/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.2.211 The Spotlight Effect in Social Judgment: An Egocentric Bias in Estimates of the Salience of One's Own Actions and Appearance Thomas Gilovich Victoria Husted Medvec Cornell University Northwestern University Kenneth Savitsky Williams College This research provides evidence that people overestimate the extent to which their actions and appearance are noted by others, a phenomenon dubbed the spotlight effect. In Studies 1 and 2, participants who were asked to don a T-shirt depicting either a flattering or potentially embarrassing image overestimated the number of observers who would be able to recall what was pictured on the shirt. In Study 3, participants in a group discussion overestimated how prominent their positive and negative utterances were to their fellow discussants. Studies 4 and 5 provide evidence supporting an anchoring-and-adjustment interpre- tation of the spotlight effect. In particular, people appear to anchor on their own rich phenomenological experience and then adjust--insufficiently--to take into account the perspective of others. The discussion focuses on the manifestations and implications of the spotlight effect across a host of everyday social phenomena. Most of us stand out in our own minds. Whether in the midst of view as extraordinary and memorable go unnoticed or underap- a personal triumph or an embarrassing mishap, we are usually preciated by others. The same is true of the actions we wish to quite focused on what is happening to us, its significance to our disown because they reflect poorly on our ability or character.
    [Show full text]
  • Insights from Psychology: Teaching Behavioral Legal Ethics As a Core Element of Professional Responsibility
    INSIGHTS FROM PSYCHOLOGY: TEACHING BEHAVIORAL LEGAL ETHICS AS A CORE ELEMENT OF PROFESSIONAL RESPONSIBILITY Tigran W. Eldred* 2016 MICH. ST. L. REV. 757 ABSTRACT The field of behavioral legal ethics—which draws on a large body of empirical research to explore how subtle and often unconscious psychological factors influence ethical decision-making by lawyers—has gained significant attention recently, including by many scholars who have called for a pedagogy that incorporates behavioral lessons into the professional responsibility curriculum. This Article provides one of the first comprehensive accounts of how law teachers can meet this challenge. Based on an approach that employs a variety of experiential techniques to immerse students in the contextual and emotional aspects of legal practice, it provides a detailed model of how to teach legal ethics from a behavioral perspective. Reflections on the approach, including the encouraging response expressed by students to this interdisciplinary method of instruction, are also discussed. TABLE OF CONTENTS I. INTRODUCTION ....................................................................... 758 II. TEACHING BEHAVIORAL LEGAL ETHICS ................................ 762 A. Understanding Behavioral Science ................................ 762 B. Teaching Methodology .................................................. 767 * Professor, New England Law | Boston. Special thanks to Lawrence Friedman, K. Babe Howell, Vivien Holmes, Leslie Levin, Catherine Gage O’Grady, Robert Prentice, Jordan Singer, Jean Sternlight, Jennifer Stumpf, Juliet Stumpf, and Molly Wilson for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this Article. Thanks also to the American Bar Association’s 41st National Conference on Professional Responsibility and CUNY Law School’s faculty colloquium, where earlier versions of this Article were presented; and to my excellent research assistants, Justin Amos and Julianna Zitz. 758 Michigan State Law Review 2016 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessments of Bias 1 the Bias Blind Spot and Its Implications Lee Ross
    Assessments of Bias 1 The Bias Blind Spot and its Implications Lee Ross Department of Psychology Stanford University Joyce Ehrlinger Department of Psychology Florida State University Thomas Gilovich Department of Psychology Cornell University To appear in Contemporary Organizational Behavior in Action. Elsbach, K.D. & Kayes, A.B. (Eds.), Pearson Prentice Hall. Assessments of Bias 2 Everyday events often prompt us to assess the objectivity of opinions and judgments— our own, those of our friends and colleagues, and those of jurists, political leaders, and advocates for interest groups. Was our decision to promote a favored employee tainted by a desire to nurture a friendship or was it the result of an even-handed evaluation of her qualifications? Was the television pundit’s bleak assessment of national healthcare influenced by the political leanings of the network’s owners or was it a fair evaluation of the proposals on the table? Was a presidential decision to go to war a reflection of cool-headed assessments of national interest or the result of ideological blindness, unwarranted optimism, or personal anger? Indeed, are our assessments of whether our own or others’ judgments are biased themselves the product of our personal biases? A wealth of evidence suggests that people’s judgments are subject to a number of cognitive and motivational biases1. Individuals consistently rate themselves above average across a variety of domains2, take credit for their successes but explain away their failures3, and detect more support for deeply held beliefs than is objectively warranted4. Furthermore, people suffer from a “bias blind spot,” or the conviction that their own judgments are less susceptible to bias than the judgments of others5.
    [Show full text]
  • AMIT KUMAR 2110 Speedway, B6700, Austin, Texas, 78712 [email protected] | (973) 722-2510 Current Position
    AMIT KUMAR 2110 Speedway, B6700, Austin, Texas, 78712 [email protected] | (973) 722-2510 http://www.kumar-amit.com Current Position 2018- THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT AUSTIN, AUSTIN, TX Assistant Professor of Marketing and Psychology Education 2010-2015 CORNELL UNIVERSITY, ITHACA, NY Ph.D. in SociaL and PersonaLity Psychology Dissertation: “From Looking Backward to Looking Forward: On Consuming the Anticipation of ExperientiaL and MateriaL Purchases” Committee Chair: Tom GiLovicH Committee Members: MeLissa Ferguson and Dave Dunning 2004-2008 HARVARD UNIVERSITY, CAMBRIDGE, MA A.B. Magna Cum Laude with Highest Honors in Psychology (Secondary FieLd: Economics) Honors Thesis: “The Impact of Outcome VaLence on Perceived Duration” Advisor: Dan GiLbert Reader: Max Bazerman Honors and Awards 2017 University of Chicago Center for Decision Research Independent Research Grant ($3000) 2016 Society for ExperimentaL SociaL Psychology Dissertation Award FinaList 2011-2015 NationaL Science Foundation Pre-DoctoraL FeLLow ($96,000 + tuition supplement) 2014 CorneLL University TraveL Grant ($675) 2014 CorneLL University John S. Knight FeLLowship ($11,735) 2014 Judgment and Decision-Making SPSP Pre-Conference TraveL Award ($200) 2014 CorneLL University TraveL Grant ($440) 2013 FeLLow, Summer Institute in SociaL and PersonaLity Psychology 2013 Summer Institute in SociaL and PersonaLity Psychology TraveL Award ($300) 2013 Society for PersonaLity and SociaL Psychology Graduate Student TraveL Award ($500) 2013 Judgment and Decision-Making SPSP Pre-Conference
    [Show full text]
  • The Dark Side of Self- and Social Perception: Black Uniforms and Aggression in Professional Sports
    Journal of Pereonality and Social Psychology Copyrighl 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1988, Vol. 54, No. 1,74-85 0022-3514/88/$00.75 The Dark Side of Self- and Social Perception: Black Uniforms and Aggression in Professional Sports Mark G. Frank and Thomas Gilovich Cornell University Black is viewed as the color of evil and death in virtually all cultures. With this association in mind, we were interested in whether a cue as subtle as the color of a person's clothing might have a signifi- cant impact on his or her behavior. To test this possibility, we examined whether professional football and ice hockey teams that wear black uniforms are more aggressive than those that wear nonblack uniforms. An analysis of the penalty records of the National Football League and the National Hockey League indicate that teams with black uniforms in both sports ranked near the top of their leagues in penalties throughout the period of study. On those occasions when a team switched from nonblack to black uniforms, the switch was accompanied by an immediate increase in penalties. The results of two laboratory experiments indicate that this finding can be attributed to both social perception and self-perception processes—that is, to the biased judgments of referees and to the increased aggressiveness of the players themselves. Our discussion focuses on the theoretical implica- tions of these data for an understanding of the variable, or "situated," nature of the self. A convenient feature of the traditional American Western over, this association between black and evil is not strictly an film was the ease with which the viewer could distinguish the American or Western phenomenon, because college students in good guys from the bad guys: The bad guys wore the black hats.
    [Show full text]
  • When Effortful Thinking Influences Judgmental Anchoring
    Journal of Behavioral Decision Making J. Behav. Dec. Making, 18: 199–212 (2005) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/bdm.495 When Effortful Thinking Influences Judgmental Anchoring: Differential Effects of Forewarning and Incentives on Self-generated and Externally Provided Anchors NICHOLAS EPLEY1* and THOMAS GILOVICH2 1University of Chicago, USA 2Cornell University,USA ABSTRACT Two experiments examined the impact of financial incentives and forewarnings on judgmental anchoring effects, or the tendency for judgments of uncertain qualities to be biased in the direction of salient anchor values. Previous research has found no effect of either manipulation on the magnitude of anchoring effects. We argue, however, that anchoring effects are produced by multiple mechanisms—one involving an effortful process of adjustment from ‘‘self-generated’’ anchors, and another involving the biased recruitment of anchor-consistent information from ‘‘externally provided’’ anchors— and that only the former should be influenced by incentives and forewarning. Two stu- dies confirmed these predictions, showing that responses to ‘‘self-generated’’ anchors are influenced by both incentives and forewarnings whereas responses to ‘‘externally provided’’ anchors are not. Discussion focuses on the implications of these effects for debiasing efforts. Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. key words anchoring; bias; incentives; effortful thought INTRODUCTION The fatal tendency of mankind to leave off thinking about a thing when it is no longer doubtful is the cause of half their errors. (J. S. Mill, 1859/1975, On liberty, p. 105) Although one may argue with Mill’s figure, it is hard to argue with his conclusion. Everyday life confronts us with a wide range of complicated problems, and at least some of the errors in thinking about these * Correspondence to: Nicholas Epley, University of Chicago, Graduate School of Business, 5807 South Woodlawn Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics and Biases at the Bargaining Table Russell Korobkin
    Marquette Law Review Volume 87 Article 17 Issue 4 Special Issue Heuristics: Heuristics and Biases at the Bargaining Table Russell Korobkin Chris Guthrie Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.law.marquette.edu/mulr Part of the Law Commons Repository Citation Russell Korobkin and Chris Guthrie, Heuristics: Heuristics and Biases at the Bargaining Table, 87 Marq. L. Rev. (2004). Available at: http://scholarship.law.marquette.edu/mulr/vol87/iss4/17 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals at Marquette Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Marquette Law Review by an authorized administrator of Marquette Law Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. HEURISTICS AND BIASES AT THE BARGAINING TABLE RUSSELL KOROBKIN* & CHRIS GUTHRIE** I. INTRODUCTION Negotiation is an inherently interpersonal activity that nonetheless requires each participant to make individualjudgments and decisions. Each negotiator must evaluate a proposed agreement, assess its value and the value of alternative courses of action-such as continuing to negotiate or pursuing an alternative transaction-and ultimately choose whether to accept or reject the proposal. The interdisciplinary field of "decision theory" offers both a normative account (how should individuals act) and descriptive accounts (how do individuals act) of decision making in the negotiation context. According to the normative model, negotiators should compare the subjective expected value of an agreement to the subjective expected value of non-agreement, taking into account such factors as risks, differential transaction costs, and reputational and relational consequences of each possible course of action.' Once a negotiator has calculated the expected value of each course of action,2 the negotiator should then select the one that promises the greatest return.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception and Misperception of Bias in Human Judgment
    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.1 Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment Emily Pronin Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Green Hall, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA Human judgment and decision making is distorted by an outcomes and to disregard evidence that threatens their array of cognitive, perceptual and motivational biases. self-esteem. Recent evidence suggests that people tend to recognize Despite the well-documented role of self-enhancement (and even overestimate) the operation of bias in human (or ego-protective) bias in human judgment, people rarely judgment – except when that bias is their own. Aside recognize their susceptibility to it. Although they rate from the general motive to self-enhance, two primary themselves as ‘better than average’ on a wide range of sources of this ‘bias blind spot’ have been identified. traits and abilities, most people also claim that their overly One involves people’s heavy weighting of introspective positive self-views are objectively true [10,11]. Moreover, evidence when assessing their own bias, despite the their unwarranted claims of objectivity persist even when tendency for bias to occur nonconsciously. The other they are informed about the prevalence of the bias and involves people’s conviction that their perceptions invited to acknowledge its influence [11]. In another exam- directly reflect reality, and that those who see things ple, people are biased to make themselves feel better after differently are therefore biased. People’s tendency to negative things happen to them (from heartbreaks to job deny their own bias, even while recognizing bias in rejections) but they are unaware of this ego-protective bias.
    [Show full text]