Interactions Between Attention and Emotion

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Interactions Between Attention and Emotion This article was originally published in Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial Reeck C., and Egner T. (2015) Interactions between Attention and Emotion. In: Arthur W. Toga, editor. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, vol. 3, pp. 269-274. Academic Press: Elsevier. Author's personal copy Interactions between Attention and Emotion C Reeck, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA T Egner, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA ã 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Authors’ note: Portions of this work were completed as part of the requirements for Crystal Reeck’s doctorate. Cognitive resources are inherently limited, such that individuals representations of stimuli that have previously (in either the cannot fully process all aspects of their environment at the same species’ or the individual’s history) been associated with time. Attention allocates processing resources to stimuli that are rewarding or aversive consequences. deemed most relevant to an individual’s goals and well-being, In addition to (and partly as a consequence of) their prior- facilitating adaptive behavior. As emotional stimuli have inher- itization in early sensory processing, affective stimuli are more ent value and biological or personal relevance to an individual, likely to hold attention and drive higher-level learning, by their very nature, they have a unique relationship with atten- decision-making, and response selection processes than affec- tion. (In this article, the terms “affect” and “emotion” are used tively neutral stimuli. During visual search, affectively salient interchangeably. However, it should be noted that the term targets are more rapidly detected (Fox, 2002; Hansen & “affect” is generally employed to refer to a broader class of states, Hansen, 1994; Ohman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001a; Ohman, including moods, motivations, and temperaments, than “emo- Lundqvist, & Esteves, 2001b). This advantage appears to be tion” typically encompasses.) Due to space limitations, this supported by speeded serial search (Eastwood, Smilek, & Meri- article primarily addresses interactions between attention and kle, 2001; Notebaert, Crombez, Van Damme, De Houwer, & the processing of negative affective stimuli, the latter having Theeuwes, 2011), and individuals are more likely both to been the main focus of the research literature and affording orient initially to affective stimuli in the environment and to the closest link to animal studies and the clinical domain. return their attention to those stimuli while sampling the Affective stimuli often gain privileged access to neural processing environment (Knight et al., 2007; LaBar, Mesulam, Gitelman, resources, making them both more likely to capture attention & Weintraub, 2000). Moreover, affective stimuli can also and more difficult to filter out if they are irrelevant to the current attract attention and improve processing of neutral stimuli goals. In addition to emotional stimuli exerting a strong exoge- presented at the same spatial locations (Phelps, Ling, & Car- nous (‘bottom-up’) pull on attention, endogenous (‘top-down’) rasco, 2006). Orienting to affectively salient stimuli can occur attentional settings can also shape affective responding to stim- even in the absence of conscious awareness of those stimuli, uli and modulate their capacity to influence perception, particularly for highly anxious individuals (Mogg & Bradley, decision making, and response selection. This article examines 1998). Emotional stimuli are also more likely to break through both of these aspects of the relationship between emotion and sources of distraction or interference to achieve awareness. attention and highlights the neural systems supporting their Perhaps one of the most famous examples of this phenome- interaction. non is the cocktail party effect (Cherry, 1953), wherein people are perfectly capable of ignoring other conversations at a cock- tail party while socializing but will end up orienting to one of Emotional Stimuli Engage Attention those background conversations if something emotionally rel- evant is uttered, such as their name. Similar affective intrusion Emotion alters neural representations of stimuli at early sen- effects have been observed using visual stimuli, with emotional sory processing stages, with emotional stimuli evoking greater images dominating awareness compared to neutral images in activation than neutral stimuli in relevant sensory regions: binocular rivalry paradigms (Alpers & Gerdes, 2007; Alpers & emotional images evoke greater activation in visual cortical Pauli, 2006; Alpers, Ruhleder, Walz, Muhlberger, & Pauli, regions (Lane, Chua, & Dolan, 1999; Morris et al., 1998; 2005) and breaking through interference generated during Sabatinelli, Bradley, Fitzsimmons, & Lang, 2005; Vuilleumier, continuous flash presentations (Yang, Zald, & Blake, 2007). Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2001), emotional sounds elicit With respect to the attentional blink (wherein targets presented greater activation in auditory processing areas (Grandjean immediately following the detection of a previous target are et al., 2005; Mitchell, Elliott, Barry, Cruttenden, & Woodruff, less likely to be reported), affectively salient stimuli are more 2003), and both pleasant and unpleasant tastes increase resistant to interference from previous targets and are more activation in gustatory processing regions (O’Doherty, Rolls, likely to be reported (Anderson, 2005; Lim, Padmala, & Pessoa, Francis, Bowtell, & McGlone, 2001). These biases can occur 2009). Representations of emotional stimuli in the environ- very early in sensory processing, emerging within 100 millisec- ment, therefore, appear to benefit from enhanced attentional onds after stimulus onset (Halgren, Raij, Marinkovic, access compared to neutral stimuli. Jousmaki, & Hari, 2000; Pizzagalli, Regard, & Lehmann, Several neural mechanisms appear to contribute to the 1999). Under biased-competition models (Desimone & potent influence that emotional stimuli exert on attention. Duncan, 1995; Miller & Cohen, 2001), external stimuli com- The amygdala plays a core role in processing affective stimuli. pete for representational processing resources. Thus, the Research involving animal models identifies two main proces- observed heightened activation in sensory regions likely sing routes for affective sensory inputs (LeDoux, 1994, 2000). reflects a competitive advantage that is granted to the sensory In addition to sensory inputs to the amygdala being channeled Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00243-8 269 Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, (2015), vol. 3, pp. 269-274 Author's personal copy 270 INTRODUCTION TO COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | Interactions between Attention and Emotion via their primary cortical areas, a second ‘low road’ provides Complementing findings from neuroimaging research, the amygdala rapid access to these representations, via a pulvi- damage to the amygdala removes attentional advantages for nar connection to the superior colliculus that bypasses cortical emotional stimuli (Phelps & LeDoux, 2005), including abol- sensory regions, enabling speeded processing of incoming ishing the ability of emotional stimuli to resist the attentional representations and facilitating rapid behavioral adjustments blink (Anderson & Phelps, 2001). Strikingly, attentional (LeDoux, 1994, 2000). There is some neuroimaging evidence advantages for emotional stimuli are observed even when neu- supporting a role for this direct amygdala input, such as the ral regions related to perceptual processing are damaged if the finding that the amygdala, pulvinar, and superior colliculus all amygdala remains intact. For instance, spatial neglect patients exhibit enhanced activation to fearful compared to neutral face have difficulty orienting their attention to the contralesional images of low spatial frequency (Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, hemifield due to parietal lobe damage. However, these patients & Dolan, 2003), consistent with the notion that coarse repre- can still detect emotionally expressive faces in the neglected sentations of visual stimuli are rapidly conveyed along this hemifield (Fox, 2002; Vuilleumier & Schwartz, 2001), and processing route. However, recent theories have called into these faces still evoke heightened activations in sensory proces- question whether this pathway is critical in human affective sing regions compared to neutral faces even when they are visual processing (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010). Nevertheless, the neglected (Vuilleumier et al., 2002). Converging evidence highly interconnected nature of the amygdala (LeDoux, 2000; from blindsight patients (who have lesions to the primary Young, Scannell, Burns, & Blakemore, 1994) makes it well visual cortex, removing
Recommended publications
  • Compare and Contrast Two Models Or Theories of One Cognitive Process with Reference to Research Studies
    ! The following sample is for the learning objective: Compare and contrast two models or theories of one cognitive process with reference to research studies. What is the question asking for? * A clear outline of two models of one cognitive process. The cognitive process may be memory, perception, decision-making, language or thinking. * Research is used to support the models as described. The research does not need to be outlined in a lot of detail, but underatanding of the role of research in supporting the models should be apparent.. * Both similarities and differences of the two models should be clearly outlined. Sample response The theory of memory is studied scientifically and several models have been developed to help The cognitive process describe and potentially explain how memory works. Two models that attempt to describe how (memory) and two models are memory works are the Multi-Store Model of Memory, developed by Atkinson & Shiffrin (1968), clearly identified. and the Working Memory Model of Memory, developed by Baddeley & Hitch (1974). The Multi-store model model explains that all memory is taken in through our senses; this is called sensory input. This information is enters our sensory memory, where if it is attended to, it will pass to short-term memory. If not attention is paid to it, it is displaced. Short-term memory Research. is limited in duration and capacity. According to Miller, STM can hold only 7 plus or minus 2 pieces of information. Short-term memory memory lasts for six to twelve seconds. When information in the short-term memory is rehearsed, it enters the long-term memory store in a process called “encoding.” When we recall information, it is retrieved from LTM and moved A satisfactory description of back into STM.
    [Show full text]
  • Mnemonics in a Mnutshell: 32 Aids to Psychiatric Diagnosis
    Mnemonics in a mnutshell: 32 aids to psychiatric diagnosis Clever, irreverent, or amusing, a mnemonic you remember is a lifelong learning tool ® Dowden Health Media rom SIG: E CAPS to CAGE and WWHHHHIMPS, mnemonics help practitioners and trainees recall Fimportant lists (suchCopyright as criteriaFor for depression,personal use only screening questions for alcoholism, or life-threatening causes of delirium, respectively). Mnemonics’ effi cacy rests on the principle that grouped information is easi- er to remember than individual points of data. Not everyone loves mnemonics, but recollecting diagnostic criteria is useful in clinical practice and research, on board examinations, and for insurance reimbursement. Thus, tools that assist in recalling di- agnostic criteria have a role in psychiatric practice and IMAGES teaching. JUPITER In this article, we present 32 mnemonics to help cli- © nicians diagnose: • affective disorders (Box 1, page 28)1,2 Jason P. Caplan, MD Assistant clinical professor of psychiatry • anxiety disorders (Box 2, page 29)3-6 Creighton University School of Medicine 7,8 • medication adverse effects (Box 3, page 29) Omaha, NE • personality disorders (Box 4, page 30)9-11 Chief of psychiatry • addiction disorders (Box 5, page 32)12,13 St. Joseph’s Hospital and Medical Center Phoenix, AZ • causes of delirium (Box 6, page 32).14 We also discuss how mnemonics improve one’s Theodore A. Stern, MD Professor of psychiatry memory, based on the principles of learning theory. Harvard Medical School Chief, psychiatric consultation service Massachusetts General Hospital How mnemonics work Boston, MA A mnemonic—from the Greek word “mnemonikos” (“of memory”)—links new data with previously learned information.
    [Show full text]
  • Post-Encoding Stress Does Not Enhance Memory Consolidation: the Role of Cortisol and Testosterone Reactivity
    brain sciences Article Post-Encoding Stress Does Not Enhance Memory Consolidation: The Role of Cortisol and Testosterone Reactivity Vanesa Hidalgo 1,* , Carolina Villada 2 and Alicia Salvador 3 1 Department of Psychology and Sociology, Area of Psychobiology, University of Zaragoza, 44003 Teruel, Spain 2 Department of Psychology, Division of Health Sciences, University of Guanajuato, Leon 37670, Mexico; [email protected] 3 Department of Psychobiology and IDOCAL, University of Valencia, 46010 Valencia, Spain; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +34-978-645-346 Received: 11 October 2020; Accepted: 15 December 2020; Published: 16 December 2020 Abstract: In contrast to the large body of research on the effects of stress-induced cortisol on memory consolidation in young people, far less attention has been devoted to understanding the effects of stress-induced testosterone on this memory phase. This study examined the psychobiological (i.e., anxiety, cortisol, and testosterone) response to the Maastricht Acute Stress Test and its impact on free recall and recognition for emotional and neutral material. Thirty-seven healthy young men and women were exposed to a stress (MAST) or control task post-encoding, and 24 h later, they had to recall the material previously learned. Results indicated that the MAST increased anxiety and cortisol levels, but it did not significantly change the testosterone levels. Post-encoding MAST did not affect memory consolidation for emotional and neutral pictures. Interestingly, however, cortisol reactivity was negatively related to free recall for negative low-arousal pictures, whereas testosterone reactivity was positively related to free recall for negative-high arousal and total pictures.
    [Show full text]
  • The Absent Minded Consumer
    The Absentminded Consumer John Ameriks, Andrew Caplin and John Leahy∗ March 2003 (Preliminary) Abstract We present a model of an absentminded consumer who does not keep constant track of his spending. The model generates a form of precautionary consumption, in which absentminded agents tend to consume more than attentive agents. We show that wealthy agents are more likely to be absentminded, whereas young and retired agents are more likely to pay attenition. The model presents new explanations for a relationship between spending and credit card use and for the decline in consumption at retirement. Key Words: JEL Classification: 1 Introduction Doyouknowhowmuchyouspentlastmonth,andwhatyouspentiton?Itis only if you answer this question in the affirmative that the classical life-cycle model of consumption applies to you. Otherwise, you are to some extent an absent-minded consumer. In this paper we develop a theory that applies to thoseofuswhofallintothiscategory. The concept of absentmindedness that we employ in our analysis was introduced by Rubinstein and Piccione [1997]. They define absentmindedness as the inability to distinguish between decision nodes that lie along the same branch of a decision tree. By identifying these distinct nodes with different ∗We would like to thank Mark Gertler, Per Krusell, and Ricardo Lagos for helpful comments. 1 levels of spending, we are able to model consumers who are uncertain as to how much they spend in any given period. The effect of this change in model structure is to make it difficult for consumers to equate the marginal utility of consumption with the marginal utility of wealth. We show that this innocent twist in a standard consumption model may not only provide insight into some existing puzzles in the consumption literature, but also shed light on otherwise puzzling results concerning linkages between financial planning and wealth accumulation (Ameriks, Caplin, and Leahy [2003]).
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Formation, Consolidation and Transformation
    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36 (2012) 1640–1645 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journa l homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev Review Memory formation, consolidation and transformation a,∗ b a a L. Nadel , A. Hupbach , R. Gomez , K. Newman-Smith a Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, United States b Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, United States a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Memory formation is a highly dynamic process. In this review we discuss traditional views of memory Received 29 August 2011 and offer some ideas about the nature of memory formation and transformation. We argue that memory Received in revised form 20 February 2012 traces are transformed over time in a number of ways, but that understanding these transformations Accepted 2 March 2012 requires careful analysis of the various representations and linkages that result from an experience. These transformations can involve: (1) the selective strengthening of only some, but not all, traces as a function Keywords: of synaptic rescaling, or some other process that can result in selective survival of some traces; (2) the Memory consolidation integration (or assimilation) of new information into existing knowledge stores; (3) the establishment Transformation Reconsolidation of new linkages within existing knowledge stores; and (4) the up-dating of an existing episodic memory. We relate these ideas to our own work on reconsolidation to provide some grounding to our speculations that we hope will spark some new thinking in an area that is in need of transformation.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory
    IFM LAB TUTORIAL SERIES # 6, COPYRIGHT c IFM LAB Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory Jiawei Zhang [email protected] Founder and Director Information Fusion and Mining Laboratory (First Version: May 2019; Revision: May 2019.) Abstract This is a follow-up tutorial article of [17] and [16], in this paper, we will introduce several important cognitive functions of the brain. Brain cognitive functions are the mental processes that allow us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information that we've received from external stimuli. This process allows us to understand and to relate to the world more effectively. Cognitive functions are brain-based skills we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex. They are related with the mechanisms of how we learn, remember, problem-solve, and pay attention, etc. To be more specific, in this paper, we will talk about the perception, attention and memory functions of the human brain. Several other brain cognitive functions, e.g., arousal, decision making, natural language, motor coordination, planning, problem solving and thinking, will be added to this paper in the later versions, respectively. Many of the materials used in this paper are from wikipedia and several other neuroscience introductory articles, which will be properly cited in this paper. This is the last of the three tutorial articles about the brain. The readers are suggested to read this paper after the previous two tutorial articles on brain structure and functions [17] as well as the brain basic neural units [16]. Keywords: The Brain; Cognitive Function; Consciousness; Attention; Learning; Memory Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Perception 3 2.1 Detailed Process of Perception .
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory
    IFM LAB TUTORIAL SERIES # 6, COPYRIGHT c IFM LAB Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory Jiawei Zhang [email protected] Founder and Director Information Fusion and Mining Laboratory (First Version: May 2019; Revision: May 2019.) Abstract This is a follow-up tutorial article of [17] and [16], in this paper, we will introduce several important cognitive functions of the brain. Brain cognitive functions are the mental processes that allow us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information that we've received from external stimuli. This process allows us to understand and to relate to the world more effectively. Cognitive functions are brain-based skills we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex. They are related with the mechanisms of how we learn, remember, problem-solve, and pay attention, etc. To be more specific, in this paper, we will talk about the perception, attention and memory functions of the human brain. Several other brain cognitive functions, e.g., arousal, decision making, natural language, motor coordination, planning, problem solving and thinking, will be added to this paper in the later versions, respectively. Many of the materials used in this paper are from wikipedia and several other neuroscience introductory articles, which will be properly cited in this paper. This is the last of the three tutorial articles about the brain. The readers are suggested to read this paper after the previous two tutorial articles on brain structure and functions [17] as well as the brain basic neural units [16]. Keywords: The Brain; Cognitive Function; Consciousness; Attention; Learning; Memory Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Perception 3 2.1 Detailed Process of Perception .
    [Show full text]
  • Everyday Attention Failures: an Individual Differences Investigation
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: © 2012 American Psychological Association Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0028075 2012, Vol. 38, No. 6, 1765–1772 RESEARCH REPORT Everyday Attention Failures: An Individual Differences Investigation Nash Unsworth and Brittany D. McMillan Gene A. Brewer University of Oregon Arizona State University Gregory J. Spillers University of Oregon The present study examined individual differences in everyday attention failures. Undergraduate students completed various cognitive ability measures in the laboratory and recorded everyday attention failures in a diary over the course of a week. The majority of attention failures were failures of distraction or mind wandering in educational contexts (in class or while studying). Latent variable techniques were used to perform analyses, and the results suggested that individual differences in working memory capacity and attention control were related to some but not all everyday attention failures. Furthermore, everyday attention failures predicted SAT scores and partially accounted for the relation between cognitive abilities and SAT scores. These results provide important evidence for individual differences in everyday attention failures as well as for the ecological validity of laboratory measures of working memory capacity and attention control. Keywords: attention control, attention failures, working memory capacity, individual differences, SAT Our ability to focus and sustain attention is a hallmark of a Despite the importance of everyday attention failures to a highly functioning attentional system. This attentional system al- number of domains, work examining these failures remains lows us to perform both highly important as well as mundane relatively scarce (relative to experimental studies of laboratory tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Models of Working Memory
    MODELS OF WORKING MEMORY Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control Edited by AKIRA MIYAKE AND PRITI SHAH PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http: //www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA http: //www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia © Cambridge University Press 1999 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 1999 Printed in the United States of America Typeface Stone Serif 9/12 pt. System QuarkXpress™ [HT] A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Models of working memory : mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control / edited by Akira Miyake, Priti Shah. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and indexes. ISBN 0-521-58325-X. – ISBN 0-521-58721-2 (pbk.) 1. Short-term memory. I. Miyake, Akira, 1966– . II. Shah, Priti, 1968– . BF378.S54M63 1999 153.1´3 – dc21 98–35134 CIP ISBN 0 521 58325 X hardback ISBN 0 521 58721 2 paperback 1 Models of Working Memory An Introduction PRITI SHAH AND AKIRA MIYAKE Working memory plays an essential role in complex cognition. Everyday cog- nitive tasks – such as reading a newspaper article, calculating the appropriate amount to tip in a restaurant, mentally rearranging furniture in one’s living room to create space for a new sofa, and comparing and contrasting various attributes of different apartments to decide which to rent – often involve mul- tiple steps with intermediate results that need to be kept in mind temporarily to accomplish the task at hand successfully.
    [Show full text]
  • Absent-Mindedness: Lapses of Conscious Awareness and Everyday Cognitive Failures
    Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitiv... 1 of 13 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.11.009 Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 578–592 Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures James Allan Cheyne, Jonathan S. A. Carriere, Daniel Smilek Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Canada Abstract A brief self-report scale was developed to assess everyday performance failures arising directly or primarily from brief failures of sustained attention (attention-related cognitive errors—ARCES). The ARCES was found to be associated with a more direct measure of propensity to attention lapses (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale—MAAS) and to errors on an existing behavioral measure of sustained attention (Sustained Attention to Response Task—SART). Although the ARCES and MAAS were highly correlated, structural modelling revealed the ARCES was more directly related to SART errors and the MAAS to SART RTs, which have been hypothesized to directly reflect the lapses of attention that lead to SART errors. Thus, the MAAS and SART RTs appear to directly reflect attention lapses, whereas the ARCES and SART errors reflect the mistakes these lapses are thought to cause. Boredom proneness was also assessed by the BPS, as a separate consequence of a propensity to attention lapses. Although the ARCES was significantly associated with the BPS, this association was entirely accounted for by the MAAS, suggesting that performance errors and boredom are separate consequences of lapses in attention. A tendency to even extraordinarily brief attention lapses on the order of milliseconds may have far-reaching consequences not only for safe and efficient task performance but also for sustaining the motivation to persist in and enjoy these tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Interactions Between Attention and Memory Marvin M Chun and Nicholas B Turk-Browne
    Interactions between attention and memory Marvin M Chun and Nicholas B Turk-Browne Attention and memory cannot operate without each other. In form of selective attention to internal representations this review, we discuss two lines of recent evidence that [1,2]. support this interdependence. First, memory has a limited capacity, and thus attention determines what will be Classic psychologists such as William James stated long encoded. Division of attention during encoding prevents the ago that ‘we cannot deny that an object once attended to formation of conscious memories, although the role of attention will remain in the memory, while one inattentively in formation of unconscious memories is more complex. Such allowed to pass will leave no traces behind’ [3]. More memories can be encoded even when there is another recently, leading neuroscientists such as Eric Kandel have concurrent task, but the stimuli that are to be encoded must be stated that one of the most important problems for 21st selected from among other competing stimuli. Second, century neuroscience is to understand how attention memory from past experience guides what should be attended. regulates the processes that stabilize experiential mem- Brain areas that are important for memory, such as the ories [4]. Here, we review studies from the past two years hippocampus and medial temporal lobe structures, are that reveal progress towards understanding the inter- recruited in attention tasks, and memory directly affects frontal- actions between attention and memory in neural systems. parietal networks involved in spatial orienting. Thus, exploring the interactions between attention and memory can provide Attention at encoding new insights into these fundamental topics of cognitive A major question in many people’s minds is how to neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence from Taboo Stroop, Lexical Decision, and Immediate Memory Tasks
    Memory & Cognition 2004, 32 (3), 474–488 Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks DONALD G. MACKAY, MEREDITH SHAFTO, JENNIFER K. TAYLOR, DIANE E. MARIAN, LISE ABRAMS, and JENNIFER R. DYER University of California, Los Angeles, California This article reports five experiments demonstrating theoretically coherent effects of emotion on memory and attention. Experiments 1–3 demonstrated three taboo Stroop effects that occur when peo- ple name the color of taboo words. One effect is longer color-naming times for taboo than for neutral words, an effect that diminishes with word repetition. The second effect is superior recall of taboo words in surprise memory tests following color naming. The third effect is better recognition memory for colors consistently associated with taboo words rather than with neutral words. None of these ef- fects was due to retrieval factors, attentional disengagement processes, response inhibition, or strate- gic attention shifts. Experiments 4 and 5 demonstrated that taboo words impair immediate recall of the preceding and succeeding words in rapidly presented lists but do not impair lexical decision times. We argue that taboo words trigger specific emotional reactions that facilitate the binding of taboo word meaning to salient contextual aspects, such as occurrence in a task and font color in taboo Stroop tasks. This study demonstrates six interrelated effects of emo- readily observable with blocked, but not with randomly in- tion on attention and memory. The main one is the taboo termixed, emotional and unemotional words; see Richards, Stroop effect: When people name the color of randomly French, Johnson, Naparstek, & Williams, 1992), and vari- intermixed taboo and neutral words, color-naming times able (e.g., holding for some types of clinical traits and are longer for taboo than for neutral words (Siegrist, 1995).
    [Show full text]