Characteristic Tone Production F. Lewis Jones

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Characteristic Tone Production F. Lewis Jones CHARACTERISTIC TONE PRODUCTION F. LEWIS JONES Revisions and Updates by Bobby Keating For ASBDA Workshop “Do You Hear What I Hear” Essentially, characteristic tone quality serves as the basis of all musical performance. Attempting to achieve an acceptable level of musical performance without acceptable characteristic tone quality is like attempting to create a chocolate cake without using chocolate as an ingredient. Therefore, as a potential musician, no effort is more important than mastering the instrument in terms of producing the musical sound (tone) it is built to produce. Because of their importance, a separate discussion could be held to review the four basic elements of a musical tone (pitch, duration, intensity, timbre), but for the current discussion I will limit my remarks to the processes by which good characteristic tone quality can be achieved, I would recommend, however, that a study be made of these elements and of the overtone, or harmonic series that exists in nature and upon which so much depends, including instrument construction, tone quality (timbre), intonation and harmony. It is a fascinating phenomenon with which every musician needs to be familiar. A. CONCEPTS How would you describe the taste of a peach to a person who had never tasted one? It would be virtually impossible except to compare it to another fruit, but even that would be inaccurate and incomplete. The only sure way for a person to develop a concept of the taste of a peach is to taste one! Sounds simple, doesn’t it? How would you describe a clarinet tone to one who has never heard a clarinet sound before? Or trumpet? Or oboe? Or flute? Again, any attempt to explain would be inaccurate and incomplete. Yet time and time again students may be urged to perform with good tone quality on his/her instrument without ever having heard good tone quality. This is a deplorable situation which can be easily remedied. Find recordings of superb musicians. These recordings are everywhere. YouTube is full of great examples for your students. These recordings can be played before and after class so you can still get your full rehearsal time. In addition, with a little extra effort, live performers can be engaged to perform for or with your band. Every student should hear a concert by an advanced musician or musical group. Use college students if you live near a university. Bring in your friends, or former students. B. EQUIPMENT 1. Instruments Before acceptable tone quality can be achieved, the student must have an acceptable musical instrument – not necessarily an expensive one, but one that is serviceable or plays well. We have all heard of ISO’s (Instrument Shaped Objects) before and you should know to stay far away from them. Proper instrument care is essential for all instruments! It must be taught and constantly emphasized so that faulty equipment does not impede the progress of a student. Seek out advise from your fellow band directors for brand names that they recommend. They will give you a true evaluation of the instrument brands out there that work and sound best. 2. Mouthpieces Mismatching mouthpieces with instruments probably obstructs the achievement of good tone quality much more than is realized. On reed instruments the problem is compounded by also needing to match the reed with the mouthpiece, and the mouthpiece with the instrument. Directors and students should spend whatever time is necessary to learn the correct matching of instruments/mouthpieces/reeds. Saxophone mouthpieces are a real challenge for most band directors that don’t play it. If this is the case for you, please find a good sax player to explain the differences in tone quality. There are some really good “cross over” mouthpieces that can be used for both concert and jazz band, but please do not have that “jazz” sound in a “concert” setting if possible. With brass players it is a little bit easier to match a mouthpiece to the student. Generally, the larger mouthpieces get a fuller tone but are more difficult to play in the upper register and can cause endurance problems for your young players as well. The trick is to find that perfect match for the student, their tone, and their range. Usually, the stronger they get with their embouchure the bigger and deeper the mouthpiece can be. Depending on the genre, some players (and teachers) like a brighter sound. This can be a simple mouthpiece change. For instance, jazz band compared to concert band sound. Most professionals change mouthpieces to accommodate the style of music being played at the time. Rim size changes are usually not recommended, but the cup depth can be changed with very little adjustments. Shallow cups generally tend to be easier to play in upper register, but have a brighter tone quality to them which may not sound great in your concert band. Here are the mouthpieces I use with my middle school band students; Clarinet Vandoren B45, or Dolce Saxophone Rousseau 4R, Selmer C* - (Meyer 5M for Jazz players) Trumpet Bach 7C at first (or whatever is in the case), then switch to Bach 3C French Horn Bach 7 to start (or something smaller like Bach 11) Switch to Farkas MDC later if desired Trombone Bach 6 ½ AL, 12C Tuba Bach 18 or 24AW, then switch to a Conn Helleberg C. POSTURE A performer’s posture while playing is sometimes a neglected fundamental which makes an enormous difference in his or her ability to produce a characteristic tone quality. Good posture promotes physical readiness and mental alertness, two very important traits for musicians. Usually, the best posture is present while standing, so effort should be made while seated to emulate that upright position as much as possible. Generally, good posture while playing consists of the following; 1. Both feet on the floor with one placed slightly ahead of the other so that the person could stand up quickly without having to shift the feet. 2. The body (above the hips) should be in essentially the same position as it would be while standing. 3. Move in the chair so that only 6 to 8 inches of the front of the chair is occupied. 4. Try to think of balancing the spine as one would balance an upright board in the palm of your hand, using as little energy as possible to stay in that position. 5. Keep the head in such a position that the throat will be open and the flow of air will not be obstructed. D. AIR Obviously, in any discussion concerning wind instruments, there must be an emphasis placed on the use of the air and this is especially necessary when dealing with characteristic tone production. Wind instrument performance without the required volume and intensity of air flow is next to impossible - like trying to play a string instrument with no strings or with rubber bands instead of strings. Below and the components of the process of using proper air control; THIS IS THE AIR YOU CONTROL 1. Inhalation Based on the experience of evaluating/adjudicating at MPA’s, I believe that this is probably the most overlooked of all of the component fundamentals, and yet the most essential because without a sufficient supply of air on hand, not enough air can be used to produce the proper tone, control the instruments pitch, or sustain and complete a musical phrase. Some visible means of determining the amount of air being inhaled must be used since otherwise it will depend on how much the thinks her or she is inhaling. Simple devices can help you measure the amount of air the student is inhaling. Take a plastic bag, such as a newspaper wrapper, using a rubber band to attach it to a ¾ inch PVC pipe 4-5 inches in length. Have the student fill the bag with air using as many breaths that it takes, then try to breath in as much of the air from the bag as possible in one inhalation attempt. This will help you measure how much air your student is taking in with one breath. Using breathing techniques easily found on YouTube, you can help the students increase the amount of air they inhale. 2. Diaphragm We tend to talk about the use of muscles, but seldom do we understand what it is and exactly how it is used in the tone production process. Specific knowledge and practical application are vital if adequate control of a wind instrument is ever to be established. Identifying the muscle used for good air control is very simple. Have the student while sitting down put knees together and bend over to touch their toes. Taking a big breath will make their “core” push against the thighs. This is the diaphragm muscle working!!! Developing good breath control is not complicated, (even though it is often presented in a confusing manner). Good breath support can easily be developed by following these simple procedures: Open the mouth and throat like a “yawn”, take a deep breath without making too much of a sound (because sound comes from a closed throat). Then blow the air out with the same openness of the throat. Do this without the instrument, then with the mouthpiece alone, and finally with the instrument attached, using the same column of air for all three. 3. Expulsion/Exhalation So often we speak of blowing air into the instrument, but I believe that a better concept is to think of blowing air, or wind through the instrument. In this way we are trying to get the air to affect the entire length of the instruments tubing, not just the portion nearest to the mouthpiece, thus making it possible to obtain the necessary tone production.
Recommended publications
  • Shepard, 1982
    Psychological Review VOLUME 89 NUMBER 4 JULY 1 9 8 2 Geometrical Approximations to the Structure of Musical Pitch Roger N. Shepard Stanford University ' Rectilinear scales of pitch can account for the similarity of tones close together in frequency but not for the heightened relations at special intervals, such as the octave or perfect fifth, that arise when the tones are interpreted musically. In- creasingly adequate a c c o u n t s of musical pitch are provided by increasingly gen- eralized, geometrically regular helical structures: a simple helix, a double helix, and a double helix wound around a torus in four dimensions or around a higher order helical cylinder in five dimensions. A two-dimensional "melodic map" o f these double-helical structures provides for optimally compact representations of musical scales and melodies. A two-dimensional "harmonic map," obtained by an affine transformation of the melodic map, provides for optimally compact representations of chords and harmonic relations; moreover, it is isomorphic to the toroidal structure that Krumhansl and Kessler (1982) show to represent the • psychological relations among musical keys. A piece of music, just as any other acous- the musical experience. Because the ear is tic stimulus, can be physically described in responsive to frequencies up to 20 kHz or terms of two time-varying pressure waves, more, at a sampling rate of two pressure one incident at each ear. This level of anal- values per cycle per ear, the physical spec- ysis has, however, little correspondence to ification of a half-hour symphony requires well in excess of a hundred million numbers.
    [Show full text]
  • Docunint Mori I
    DOCUNINT MORI I. 00.175 695 SE 020 603 *OTROS Schaaf, Willias L., Ed. TITLE Reprint Series: Mathematics and Music. RS-8. INSTITUTION Stanford Univ., Calif. School Mathematics Study Group. SKINS AGENCY National Science Foundation, Washington, D.C. DATE 67 28p.: For related documents, see SE 028 676-640 EDRS PRICE RF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Curriculum: Enrichment: *Fine Arts: *Instruction: Mathesatics Education: *Music: *Rustier Concepts: Secondary Education: *Secondary School Mathematics: Supplementary Reading Materials IDENTIFIERS *$chool Mathematics Study Group ABSTRACT This is one in a series of SBSG supplesentary and enrichment pamphlets for high school students. This series makes available expository articles which appeared in a variety of athematical periodicals. Topics covered include: (1) the two most original creations of the human spirit: (2) mathematics of music: (3) numbers and the music of the east and west: and (4) Sebastian and the Wolf. (BP) *********************************************************************** Reproductions snpplied by EDRS are the best that cam be made from the original document. *********************************************************************** "PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS U S DE PE* /NE NT Of MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED SY EOUCATION WELFAIE NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF IEDUCAT1ON THISLX)( 'API NT HAS141 IN WI P410 Oti(10 4 MA, Yv AS WI t1,4- 0 4 tiONI TH4 Pi 40SON 4)44 fwftAN,IA t.(1N 041.G114. AT,sp, .1 po,sos 1)1 1 ev MW OP4NIONS STA 'Fp LX) NE.IT NI SSI144,t Y RIPWI. TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES SI NT (7$$ M A NAIhj N',1,11)11E 01 INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)." E.),,f T P(1,1140N ,1 V 0 1967 by The Board cf Trustees of theLeland Stanford AU rights reeerved Junior University Prieted ia the UnitedStates of Anverka Financial support for tbe SchoolMatbernatks Study provided by the Group has been Nasional ScienceFoundation.
    [Show full text]
  • Physics 101 Physics
    PhysicsPhysics 101101 LectureLecture 2121 DopplerDoppler EffectEffect LoudnessLoudness HumanHuman HearingHearing InterferenceInterference ofof SoundSound WavesWaves ReflectionReflection && RefractionRefraction ofof SoundSound Quiz: Monday Oct. 18; Chaps. 16,17,18(as covered in class),19 CR/NC Deadline Oct. 19 1/32 DemoDemo:: DopplerDoppler ShiftShift Hear frequency as higher when whistle is moving towards you and hear it as lower when moving away from you. Summary: Source & observer approaching, fobs>fs Source & observer separating, fobs<fs Higher Shorter Wavelength Frequency Lower Frequency Longer Wavelength 13-Oct-10 2/32 LoudnessLoudness && AmplitudeAmplitude Loudness depends on amplitude of pressure and density variations in sound waves. 13-Oct-10 3/32 deciBelsdeciBels (dB) (dB) Loudness of sound depends on the amplitude of pressure variation in the sound wave. Loudness is measured in deciBels (dB), which is a logarithmic scale (since our perception of loudness varies logarithmically). From the threshold of hearing (0 dB) to the threshold of pain (120 dB), the pressure amplitude is a million times higher. At the threshold of pain (120 db), the pressure variation is still only about 10 Pascals, which is one ten thousandth of atmospheric pressure. 4/32 5/32 Human Hearing 6/32 7/32 HearingHearing LossLoss The hair cells that line the cochlea are a delicate and vulnerable part of the ear. Repeated or sustained exposure to loud noise destroys the neurons in this region. Once destroyed, the hair cells are not replaced, and the sound frequencies interpreted by them are no longer heard. Hair cells that respond to high frequency sound are very vulnerable to destruction, and loss of these neurons typically produces difficulty understanding human voices.
    [Show full text]
  • Sine Waves and Simple Acoustic Phenomena in Experimental Music - with Special Reference to the Work of La Monte Young and Alvin Lucier
    Sine Waves and Simple Acoustic Phenomena in Experimental Music - with Special Reference to the Work of La Monte Young and Alvin Lucier Peter John Blamey Doctor of Philosophy University of Western Sydney 2008 Acknowledgements I would like to thank my principal supervisor Dr Chris Fleming for his generosity, guidance, good humour and invaluable assistance in researching and writing this thesis (and also for his willingness to participate in productive digressions on just about any subject). I would also like to thank the other members of my supervisory panel - Dr Caleb Kelly and Professor Julian Knowles - for all of their encouragement and advice. Statement of Authentication The work presented in this thesis is, to the best of my knowledge and belief, original except as acknowledged in the text. I hereby declare that I have not submitted this material, either in full or in part, for a degree at this or any other institution. .......................................................... (Signature) Table of Contents Abstract..................................................................................................................iii Introduction: Simple sounds, simple shapes, complex notions.............................1 Signs of sines....................................................................................................................4 Acoustics, aesthetics, and transduction........................................................................6 The acoustic and the auditory......................................................................................10
    [Show full text]
  • A Biological Rationale for Musical Consonance Daniel L
    PERSPECTIVE PERSPECTIVE A biological rationale for musical consonance Daniel L. Bowlinga,1 and Dale Purvesb,1 aDepartment of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, 1090 Vienna, Austria; and bDuke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708 Edited by Solomon H. Snyder, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, and approved June 25, 2015 (received for review March 25, 2015) The basis of musical consonance has been debated for centuries without resolution. Three interpretations have been considered: (i) that consonance derives from the mathematical simplicity of small integer ratios; (ii) that consonance derives from the physical absence of interference between harmonic spectra; and (iii) that consonance derives from the advantages of recognizing biological vocalization and human vocalization in particular. Whereas the mathematical and physical explanations are at odds with the evidence that has now accumu- lated, biology provides a plausible explanation for this central issue in music and audition. consonance | biology | music | audition | vocalization Why we humans hear some tone combina- perfect fifth (3:2), and the perfect fourth revolution in the 17th century, which in- tions as relatively attractive (consonance) (4:3), ratios that all had spiritual and cos- troduced a physical understanding of musi- and others as less attractive (dissonance) has mological significance in Pythagorean phi- cal tones. The science of sound attracted been debated for over 2,000 years (1–4). losophy (9, 10). many scholars of that era, including Vincenzo These perceptual differences form the basis The mathematical range of Pythagorean and Galileo Galilei, Renee Descartes, and of melody when tones are played sequen- consonance was extended in the Renaissance later Daniel Bernoulli and Leonard Euler.
    [Show full text]
  • A Study to Explore the Effects of Sound Vibrations on Consciousness
    International Journal of Social Work and Human Services Practice Horizon Research Publishing Vol.6. No.3 July, 2018, pp. 75-88 A Study to Explore the Effects of Sound Vibrations on Consciousness Meera Raghu Independent Researcher, New Zealand Abstract Sound is a form of energy produced by cause happiness, joy, courage or calmness, dissonant vibrations caused by movement of particles. Sound can intervals can cause tension, anger, fear or sadness, thereby travel through solids (such as metal, wood, membranes), affecting the emotional aspect of consciousness. liquids (water) and gases (air). The sound vibrations that reach our ear are produced by the movement of particles in Keywords Consciousness, Sound, Vibration, Music, the air surrounding the source of sound. The movement or Emotion, Chladni, Pattern, Energy vibration of particles produces waves of sound. Sound waves are longitudinal and travel in the direction of propagation of vibrations. The pitch of sound is related directly to its frequency, which is given by the number of Introduction vibrations or cycles per second. The higher the pitch of sound, the higher is its frequency, and the lower the pitch, Sound is everywhere. There is perpetual movement and the lower is its frequency. Human ear can hear sounds of action in the world around us, and this produces a variety of frequencies ranging from 20 – 20,000 cycles per second (or sounds, such as those coming from Nature, from animals, Hertz – Hz). Sound waves can be visually seen and studied those generated by humans in the form of speech or music, using ‘Chladni’ plates, which was devised and those that are generated by vehicles, machines, gadgets that experimented by Ernst Chladni, a famous physicist with a are used for comfort, leisure and convenience.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perceptual Representation of Timbre
    Chapter 2 The Perceptual Representation of Timbre Stephen McAdams Abstract Timbre is a complex auditory attribute that is extracted from a fused auditory event. Its perceptual representation has been explored as a multidimen- sional attribute whose different dimensions can be related to abstract spectral, tem- poral, and spectrotemporal properties of the audio signal, although previous knowledge of the sound source itself also plays a role. Perceptual dimensions can also be related to acoustic properties that directly carry information about the mechanical processes of a sound source, including its geometry (size, shape), its material composition, and the way it is set into vibration. Another conception of timbre is as a spectromorphology encompassing time-varying frequency and ampli- tude behaviors, as well as spectral and temporal modulations. In all musical sound sources, timbre covaries with fundamental frequency (pitch) and playing effort (loudness, dynamic level) and displays strong interactions with these parameters. Keywords Acoustic damping · Acoustic scale · Audio descriptors · Auditory event · Multidimensional scaling · Musical dynamics · Musical instrument · Pitch · Playing effort · Psychomechanics · Sound source geometry · Sounding object 2.1 Introduction Timbre may be considered as a complex auditory attribute, or as a set of attributes, of a perceptually fused sound event in addition to those of pitch, loudness, per- ceived duration, and spatial position. It can be derived from an event produced by a single sound source or from the perceptual blending of several sound sources. Timbre is a perceptual property, not a physical one. It depends very strongly on the acoustic properties of sound events, which in turn depend on the mechanical nature of vibrating objects and the transformation of the waves created as they propagate S.
    [Show full text]
  • The Sounds of Music: Science of Musical Scales∗ 1
    SERIES ARTICLE The Sounds of Music: Science of Musical Scales∗ 1. Human Perception of Sound Sushan Konar Both, human appreciation of music and musical genres tran- scend time and space. The universality of musical genres and associated musical scales is intimately linked to the physics of sound, and the special characteristics of human acoustic sensitivity. In this series of articles, we examine the science underlying the development of the heptatonic scale, one of the most prevalent scales of the modern musical genres, both western and Indian. Sushan Konar works on stellar compact objects. She Introduction also writes popular science articles and maintains a Fossil records indicate that the appreciation of music goes back weekly astrophysics-related blog called Monday Musings. to the dawn of human sentience, and some of the musical scales in use today could also be as ancient. This universality of musi- cal scales likely owes its existence to an amazing circularity (or periodicity) inherent in human sensitivity to sound frequencies. Most musical scales are specific to a particular genre of music, and there exists quite a number of them. However, the ‘hepta- 1 1 tonic’ scale happens to have a dominating presence in the world Having seven base notes. music scene today. It is interesting to see how this has more to do with the physics of sound and the physiology of human auditory perception than history. We shall devote this first article in the se- ries to understand the specialities of human response to acoustic frequencies. Human ear is a remarkable organ in many ways. The range of hearing spans three orders of magnitude in frequency, extending Keywords from ∼20 Hz to ∼20,000 Hz (Figure 1) even though the sensitivity String vibration, beat frequencies, consonance-dissonance, pitch, tone.
    [Show full text]
  • Mathematics and Music
    Mathematics and Music David Wright April 8, 2009 2 i About the Author David Wright is professor of mathematics at Washington University in St. Louis, where he currently serves as Chair of the Mathematics Department. He received his Ph.D. in Mathematics from Columbia University, New York City. A leading researcher in the fields of affine algebraic geometry and polynomial automorphisms, he has produced landmark publications in these areas and has been an invited speaker at numerous international mathematics conferences. He designed and teaches a university course in Mathematics and Music, the notes from which formed the beginnings of this book. As a musician, David is an arranger and composer of vocal music, where his work often integrates the close harmony style called barbershop harmony with jazz, blues, gospel, country, doo-wop, and contemporary a cappella. He is Associate Director of the St. Charles Ambassadors of Harmony, an award winning male chorus of 160 singers. He also serves as a musical consultant and arranger for numerous other vocal ensembles. He is active in the Bar- bershop Harmony Society and was inducted into its Hall of Fame in 2008. As arranger and music historian David has been featured in national radio and TV broadcasts at home and abroad, and has authored several articles on vocal harmony. ii Contents Introduction v 1 Basic Concepts 1 2 Horizontal Structure 17 3 Harmony and Related Numerology 31 4 Ratios and Musical Intervals 45 5 Logarithms and Musical Intervals 53 6 Chromatic Scales 61 7 Octave Identification 69 8 Properties of Integers 87 9 The Integers as Intervals 97 10 Timbre and Periodic Functions 105 11 Rational Numbers As Intervals 125 12 Rational Tuning 139 iii iv CONTENTS Introduction The author’s perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • Perspectives on Memory for Musical Timbre
    Perspectives on Memory for Musical Timbre Kai Kristof Siedenburg Music Technology Area Department of Music Research Schulich School of Music McGill University Montreal, Canada March 2016 A thesis submitted to McGill University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. c 2016 Kai Siedenburg 2016/03/29 bong bing bang bung bäng für Eliza ii Contents Abstract/Résumé .................................. vi Acknowledgments ................................. x Contribution of authors .............................. xii List of Figures ................................... xvii List of Tables .................................... xx List of Acronyms .................................. xxi 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Ideas and questions ............................. 1 1.2 Methods ................................... 8 1.3 Thesis outline ................................ 9 I Background 13 2 Three conceptual distinctions for timbre 15 2.1 Introduction ................................. 15 2.2Soundeventvs.timbre........................... 16 2.3 Qualitative vs. source timbre ........................ 18 2.4 Timbre on different scales of detail .................... 19 2.5 Conclusion .................................. 21 3 A review of research on memory for timbre 23 3.1 Introduction ................................. 23 3.2 Basic concepts in memory ......................... 25 3.3 Key findings in auditory memory ..................... 28 Contents iii 3.3.1 Auditory sensory memory ..................... 29 3.3.2 Memory for noise .........................
    [Show full text]
  • Absolute Pitch
    Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1993 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1993. Vol. 113. No. 2. 345-361 0033-2909/93/53.00 Absolute Pitch Annie H. Takeuchi and Stewart H. Hulse Absolute pitch (AP) is the ability to identify a tone's pitch or to produce a tone at a particular pitch without the use of an external reference pitch. AP exists in varying degrees among people generally described as AP possessors. AP possessors vary not only in the accuracy with which they can identify pitches but also in their ability to produce pitches absolutely and in their ability to identify tones of various timbres and in various pitch registers. AP possessors' memory for pitches is mediated by verbal pitch names; they do not have superior memory for pitches per se. Although the etiology of AP is not yet completely understood, evidence points toward the early-learning theory. This theory states that AP can be learned by anyone during a limited period early in development, up to about age 6, after which a general developmental shift from perceiving individual features to perceiving relations among features makes AP difficult or impossible to acquire. Absolute pitch (AP) is the ability to identify the pitch of a work with units of a single tone, whereas composers without AP musical tone or to produce a musical tone at a given pitch must work with pitch relations that require a minimum unit of without the use of an external reference pitch. Most humans two tones. process musical pitch relatively rather than absolutely. They Psychologists have puzzled over several questions about AP process the melodic and harmonic relations among pitches in- for over 100 years.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychological Representation of Musical Pitch in a Tonal Context
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 11, 346-374 (1979) The Psychological Representation of Musical Pitch in a Tonal Context CAROL L. KRUMHANSL Stanford University In this series of experiments, evidence was found for a complex psychological representation of musical pitch. The results of a scaling study, in which subjects judged the similarities between pairs of tones presented in an explicitly tonal context, suggest that musical listeners extract a pattern of relationships among tones that is determined not only by pitch height and chroma, but also by member- ship in the major triad chord and the diatonic scale associated with the tonal system of the context. Multidimensional scaling of the similarity ratings gave a three-dimensional conical structure around which the tones were ordered ac- cording to pitch height. The major triad components formed a closely related cluster near the vertex of the cone; the remaining diatonic scale tones formed a less closely related subset farther from the vertex; and, the nondiatonic tones, still farther from the vertex, were widely dispersed. The results also suggest that, in the psychological representation, tones less closely related to the tonality are less stable than tones closely related to the tonality, and that the representation incor- porates the tendency for unstable tones to move toward the more stable tones in time, reflecting the dynamic character of musical tones. In the similarity ratings of the scaling study, tones less related to the tonality were judged more similar to tones more related to the tonality than the reverse temporal order. Furthermore, in a delayed recognition task memory performance for nondiatonic tones was less accurate than for diatonic tones, and nondiatonic tones were more often confused with diatonic tones than diatonic tones were confused with nondiatonic tones.
    [Show full text]