A Cross-Linguistic Study on the Phonetics of Dorsal Obstruents

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Cross-Linguistic Study on the Phonetics of Dorsal Obstruents A cross-linguistic study on the phonetics of dorsal obstruents DISSERTATION zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades doctor philosophiae (Dr. phil.) im Fach Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft eingereicht an der Philosophischen Fakultät II Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin von Dipl. Psych. Christian Geng geboren am 08.11.1968 in Friedrichshafen Präsident der Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin: Prof. Dr. Dr. h.c. Christoph Markschies Dekan der Philosophischen Fakultät II: Prof. Dr. Dr. h.c. Christoph Markschies Gutachter: 1. Prof. Dr. Bernd Pompino-Marschall 2. Prof. Dr. Daniel Recasens eingereicht am: 20 November 2007 Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 23 Januar 2008 Abstract This work investigates place of articulation in a cross-linguistic perspective: While most articulatory and in particular perceptual studies within phonetics are refined to the study of the three major - labial, alveolar and velar - places of articulation, the present dissertation aims at the addition of the palatal place of articulation in obstruent production and percep- tion, with special focus on the Hungarian palatal stops. The self-limitation of phonetics to deal only with the three major stop places of articulation in part has practical reasons: Phonemes like the palatal Hungarian obstruents [c] and [é] are not members of the sound systems of languages like English which can be regarded as the drosophila of experimental phonetics. The guiding idea of this research was that the incorporation of such additional phonemes into the planning and design of the experimental studies might, by increasing the category density, drive the categories into a “tug of war” for phonetic “resources” - articulatory or perceptual spaces. Put differently, the architecture of the current disserta- tion is centered around ways in which the category distance defined in some phonetic - or potentially also phonological space - can be utilized to derive hypotheses which are best tested in a crosslinguistic design. Such designs can for example be helpful to test whether the phoneme inventory of a language leaves its traces in patterns of velar coarticulation. Concerning the so-called loops,1 there have been numerous publications dealing with the influences of speech physiology, aerodynamics, general movement principles or articula- tory biomechanics in a fairly systematic fashion, while no research efforts have been made so far to investigate possible influences of the system of linguistic contrasts of a given lan- guage. The same holds for the domain of speech perception: In speech perception, there are open questions which have not been answered yet or only been touched in a rudimen- tary fashion: How many stop place categories can be implemented on the basis of formant transitions alone? An arbitrary amount or is there an upper limit? In phonetic research on vowels similar questions are fairly common, in the domain of consonants, I am not aware of any empirical efforts. Experiments will be devised starting from opposite ends of the rope: speech production and speech perception - which do not necessarily have commensurable theoretical start- points. Working in both the domains of production and perception is costly and redundancy hard to avoid if one wants to arrive at a coherent theoretical treatment of the necessary conceptual ingredients. The solution chosen was to start with a theoretical introductory part (partI) with a separate treatment of (i) perceptual and (ii) articulatory matters. This main thread is split later into separate experimental chapters - chapters2 and3 - both of which already belong to the experimental part (partII). These separate additional theoret- ical chapters provide the theoretical building blocks which were deemed necessary for the experimental work on perception (chapter2) and articulation (chapter3). Also hypotheses are formulated in these sections. Take as an example justifying the repeated treatment of theoretical building blocks the description of the Gestural Approach: It is only described in very moderate depth in the introductory part (see section 2.2). It only contains the description of the lossless undamped spring-mass system but not of the damped system which forms an integral part of the actual movement generation device of Task Dynamics. This limitation was strategic in the sense that the only purpose in the introductory part was to highlight the alternative conception and 1“Loops” denote the loop-shaped trajectories of velar consonants during oral closure, in particular in front vowel context. ii role of time and timing in this particular theoretical apparatus - of Ecological Psychology2 - in comparison to more conventional phonologies. The treatment of perceptual theories can be seen somehow in analogy: While the introductory chapter just aimed to elaborate the approach aspired - in the form of a “Howto” for the application of a cross-linguistic usage of the CP paradigm for place of articulation studies (section 2.3), the information given there is supplemented by theories more closely related to the derivation of hypotheses like for example the locus theory of Sussman et al.(1998) in section 3.2. The division between articulation and perception pertains to the presentation of results: The results for perceptual experimentation (section6) are separated from the articulatory results (sections9, 10 and 11), i.e. presented together with their respective theoretical motivation. The results obtained are viewed in a more aggregate fashion in a separate part (part III) containing the general discussion which also concludes this dissertation. Summary of results - Perceptual Studies As mentioned, the research on the perception of place of articulation in oral stop conso- nants has almost exclusively focused the main places of articulation which are also most common in the sound inventories of the world’s languages, and the palatal place of articu- lation has been disregarded in this respect. One language that phonemically has a palatal stop is Hugarian. The aim was to compare a language with and without such a palatal stop phoneme in the inventory under deprived conditions. As a language without such a phoneme French was chosen because of its better match with Hungarian with respect to voicing im- plementation as compared to e.g. German. The deprivation mentioned was achieved by generating synthetic CV syllables as stimuli where V is the neutral vowel which deprives the listener of the possibility to make perceptual adjustments to vowel context. The listener was further deprived of burst information which was not synthesized. Results suggest that (i) territorial mapping of the responses in stimulus space for palatal and alveolar place together in a four-category language like Hungarian resembles that of the alveolar place alone in a three-category language like in the present case French, and (ii) the velar region of the territorial map for Hungarian is shrunk in comparison to the velar region of French. Summary of results - Articulatory Studies The dorsal obstruents of Hungarian and German were studied by means of Electromagnetic Articulography (EMA). The target material contained the velar and palatal stops in initial and medial position for Hungarian. For German, target material consisted in (a) medial palatal, velar and uvular fricative allophones and (b) initial and medial velar stops. Results are presented according to the division in (i) intralinguistic Hungarian, (ii) intralinguistic German findings, and (iii) crosslinguistic comparisons: ad (i) The Hungarian palatals are true dorsopalatals. The fronting of the Hungarian velar converges to a back palatal articulation. Palatals can exhibit large movement ampli- tudes during oral closure and also during the whole VCV sequence. This implies that contextual velar fronting is not optimally treated in the sense of an underspecifica- tion scenario such as that advocated by authors like Keating. Velar stop production in Hungarian shows some patterns of deviance from the patterns observed for other languages like German or English. These patterns partly run contrary to the claim that velar “loops” arise of biomechanical and/or aerodynamic origin exclusively (e.g Kent und Moll, 1972; Perrier et al., 2003). 2Articulatory Phonology is the particular instantiation of Ecological Psychology within Phonetics. iii ad (ii) Variation generated by the German fricative allophones is distinguishable from within- phoneme variation for the German stops. The stop variants exhibit less contextual variation. Interpretations in terms of phonological instantiation of phonemic or al- lophonic contrast or in terms of a particular articulatory control regime are rejected due to theoretical considerations. ad (iii) Crosslinguistic tongue shape comparisons of articulatory profiles indicate that Hun- garian velar stop production is in fact reactive to the presence of a phonemic palatal stop in Hungarian. This was evidenced by the crosslinguistic comparison of static tongue configurations as well as by kinematic analyses. iv Zusammenfassung Gegenstand dieser Arbeit sind crosslinguistische Differenzierungen der Artikulations- stelle: Während sich die - sowohl artikulatorisch wie auch perzeptuell ausgerichtete - ex- perimentalphonetische Forschungsliteratur zum Thema Plosive in den allermeisten Fällen auf die Untersuchung der drei Artikulationsstellen labial, alveolar und velar beschränkt hat, hat sich die vorliegende Arbeit zur Aufgabe gemacht, diese Beschränkung aufzuheben und eine vierte, palatale Artikulationsstelle in der experimentellen
Recommended publications
  • Phonetics and Phonology of Contrastive Palatal Affricates*
    Working Papers of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory 2003, v.15, pp.130-193 Phonetics and Phonology of Contrastive Palatal Affricates* Amanda Miller-Ockhuizen and Draga Zec Serbian contains two classes of contrastive palatal affricates exemplified in tsar ‘gain’ vs. tar ‘magic’. In the phonological process of iotization, [t] patterns with [ts], and [k] with [t]. Articulatorily, [ts] is laminal, more front with compressed lips, while [t] is apical, more back with protruded lips. Acoustically, the affricates are distinguished by two prominent spectral peaks in the frication noise interval. [t]/[k] display lower frequency spectral peaks than [t]/[ts]. Different frequency ranges in the spectra of frication noise vs. stop bursts derive from constriction degree. Temporal acoustic attributes show that they behave as a class of affricates, different from both stops and fricatives. Phonetic differences in several articulatory attributes in the input contribute to cavity volume differences in the output, which result in higher frequency spectral peaks for the laminal affricate than the apical affricate. These differences define the natural classes observed in iotization and allow a phonetically accurate statement of the [t]/[ts] and [k]/[t] patterning. 1. Introduction Standard Serbian (formerly known as the eastern variant of Serbo-Croatian) possesses two classes of contrastive palatal affricates, each containing a voiced/voiceless pair. The difference between these classes can be broadly captured in terms of apical vs. laminal articulation. The apical class includes [t], which is voiceless, and [d], which is voiced. The members of the laminal class are [ts] and [dz], which are voiceless and voiced respectively.1 Articulatorily, the sounds in the laminal class are more front and produced with compressed lips, while the sounds in the apical class are more back and produced with protruded lips.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Phonetic and Inventory Constraints in the Spirantization of Intervocalic Voiced Stops: Comparing Two Different Measurements of Energy Change
    Effects of Phonetic and Inventory Constraints in the Spirantization of Intervocalic Voiced Stops: Comparing two Different Measurements of Energy Change. Marta Ortega-LLebaria University of Northern Colorado E-mail: [email protected] This paper extends the same hypothesis to the phenomenon ABSTRACT of spirantization by investigating the interaction of phonetic factors with inventory constraints. First, it This paper examines the effect of inventory constraints and examines whether the phonetic factors of stress and vowel the phonetic factors of stress and vowel context in the context, which had an effect in the lenition of Spanish lenition of English and Spanish intervocalic voiced stops. intervocalic /g/ [3], had also an effect in English Five native speakers of American English and five native intervocalic /g/ and in Spanish and English intervocalic /b/. speakers of Caribbean Spanish were recorded saying Secondly, this paper studies the interaction of inventory bi-syllabic words containing intervocalic /b/ and /g/. The constraints with phonetic factors. Inventory constraints intervocalic consonants were evaluated according to two were aimed to preserve the system of sound contrasts of a measures of energy, i.e. RMS ratio, and speed of consonant language while phonetic factors provided contexts that release. Repeated Measures ANOVAS in each measure favored consonant lenition. Hypothetically, a consonant indicated that for both languages, /b/ and /g/ were most will become lenited in a favorable context only if the lenited in trochee words, and that /g/ was most spirantized resulting sound will not impair a contrast of the language. when flanked by /i/ and /u/ vowels. Inventory constraints For example, in English, /b/ contrasts with the voiced moderated the degree of consonant lenition displayed in the fricative /v/ while in Spanish, it does not.
    [Show full text]
  • Using 'North Wind and the Sun' Texts to Sample Phoneme Inventories
    Blowing in the wind: Using ‘North Wind and the Sun’ texts to sample phoneme inventories Louise Baird ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University [email protected] Nicholas Evans ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University [email protected] Simon J. Greenhill ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University & Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History [email protected] Language documentation faces a persistent and pervasive problem: How much material is enough to represent a language fully? How much text would we need to sample the full phoneme inventory of a language? In the phonetic/phonemic domain, what proportion of the phoneme inventory can we expect to sample in a text of a given length? Answering these questions in a quantifiable way is tricky, but asking them is necessary. The cumulative col- lection of Illustrative Texts published in the Illustration series in this journal over more than four decades (mostly renditions of the ‘North Wind and the Sun’) gives us an ideal dataset for pursuing these questions. Here we investigate a tractable subset of the above questions, namely: What proportion of a language’s phoneme inventory do these texts enable us to recover, in the minimal sense of having at least one allophone of each phoneme? We find that, even with this low bar, only three languages (Modern Greek, Shipibo and the Treger dialect of Breton) attest all phonemes in these texts.
    [Show full text]
  • The Grammar of Coarticulation Edward Flemming Department of Linguistics & Philosophy, MIT
    The Grammar of Coarticulation Edward Flemming Department of Linguistics & Philosophy, MIT 1. Introduction It is well established that coarticulatory patterns are language-specific and therefore must be specified in the grammars of languages (e.g. Beddor, Harnsberger & Lindemann, 2002, Clumeck, 1976, Huffman, 1988, Keating & Cohn, 1988, Magen, 1984, Manuel, 1990, Manuel & Krakow, 1984, Oh, 2002), but there is less consensus on the nature of the grammar of coarticulation. Here we use evidence from the typology of coarticulatory patterns to argue for a model based on weighted constraints. Through two case studies we see that coarticulation shows typological variation that is parallel in many respects to phonological typology: there are common or universal cross-linguistic patterns, such as F0 transitions between adjacent tones, that point to universal constraints, such as physiological limitations on rates of F0 change, but language- specific variation in the details of coarticulation, e.g. variation in the timing of F0 transitions, shows that languages differ in their responses to these constraints. I propose that this is due to interaction between conflicting constraints: constraints on rate of change interact with perceptually motivated constraints requiring the realization of phonetic targets. These constraints can conflict, with conflicts being resolved by constraint prioritization. The relative priority of constraints can differ from language to language resulting in language-specific patterns of coarticulation. This is essentially the same approach to the analysis of typology that has been successful in Optimality Theoretic analyses of phonology (Prince & Smolensky, 2004). This account of coarticulation has implications for analyses that attempt to explain phonological generalizations in terms of phonologization of coarticulation.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparative Analysis of Consonants in Chinese and Arabic Phonetics
    Journal of Literature and Art Studies, November 2019, Vol. 9, No. 11, 1188-1193 doi: 10.17265/2159-5836/2019.11.010 D DAVID PUBLISHING A Comparative Analysis of Consonants in Chinese and Arabic Phonetics FAYROUZ ABDELSAMIE ELSAYED MOHAMED School of International Chinese Studies (SICS) of East China Normal University (ECNU), Shanghai , China Arabic and Chinese are one of the most difficult languages to learn in the world. The mark of Arabic is Pinyin characters plus pinyin symbols. On the other hand, the mark of Chinese is ideographic square shaped characters; each pronunciation of a Chinese character is generally a syllable. There are great differences between Arabic and Chinese consonants, so this paper mainly discusses the similarities and differences between the two languages and their respective characteristics through the comparative analysis of Chinese and Arabic consonants. Keywords: Chinese, Arabic, pronunciation, consonant, comparison A Comparative Analysis of Chinese and Arabic Consonants In order to make a comparative analysis of Chinese and Arabic consonants, first of all, we need to have a quite comprehensive and clear understanding of the features of each consonant pronunciation of the two languages. All consonant phonemes in Chinese and Arabic are included in Table 2 below. The Chinese phonetic symbol is Pinyin, which uses the International Latin alphabets. It has both consonant and vowel letters, while Arabic is phonetic alphabets transcription and the phonetic symbol of consonants is Arabic letters itself. Chinese language has 22 consonant phonemes; among them [ŋ] can only be used as a tail vowel, [n] can be used as both initial and final, and the rest can only be used as initials.
    [Show full text]
  • Description of Acoustic and Articulatory Parameters of Vowels in Mapudungun Speakers
    Int. J. Odontostomat., 14(2):205-212, 2020. Description of Acoustic and Articulatory Parameters of Vowels in Mapudungun Speakers. Pilot Study Descripción de Parámetros Acusticos y Articulatorios de las Vocales en Hablantes de Mapudungun. Estudio Piloto Giannina Álvarez1; Magaly Ruiz2; Alain Arias1,3,4; María Florencia Lezcano1,3 & Ramón Fuentes1,3 ÁlvareZ, G.; RUIZ, M.; ARIAS, A.; LEZCANO, M. F. & FUENTES, R. Description of acoustic and articulatory parame- ters of vowels in Mapudungun speakers. Pilot study. Int. J. Odontostomat., 14(2):205-212, 2020. ABSTRACT: Mapudungun is a language used by Mapuche people in some regions of Chile and Argentina. The aim of this study was to describe the vowel phonemes with regard to the articulatory parameters (position of the tongue with respect to the palate and jaw opening) and acoustic parameters (f0, F1, F2 and F3) in Mapudungun speakers in the Region of La Araucanía. The vocalic phonemes of Mapudungun are six, where the first five are similar to those used in Spanish (/a e i o u/), to which is added a sixth vowel (/ɨ/) with its vocalic allophones (/ɨ/) and [Ә]. Three Mapudungun speakers were evaluated. The tongue movements were collected by Electromagnetic Articulography 3D and the data were processed with MATLAB and PRAAT software. It was possible to describe the trajectory of each third of the tongue during the production of the vowels. It was observed that the sixth vowel /Ә/ had minimal jaw opening during its pronunciation. In addition, the characteristic of /Ә/ as an unrounded mid-central vowel was corroborated. In this study, the tongue of mapudungun speakers was in a more posterior position than the found in other studies.
    [Show full text]
  • Grapheme-To-Phoneme Transcription in Hungarian
    International Journal of Computational Linguistics and Applications vol. 7, no. 1, 2016, pp. 161–173 Received 08/02/2016, accepted 07/03/2016, final 20/06/2016 ISSN 0976-0962, http://ijcla.bahripublications.com Grapheme-to-Phoneme Transcription in Hungarian ATTILA NOVÁK1 AND BORBÁLA SIKLÓSI2 1 MTA-PPKE Hungarian Language Technology Research Group, Hungary 2 Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Hungary ABSTRACT A crucial component of text-to-speech systems is the one respon- sible for the transcription of the written text to its phonemic rep- resentation. although the complexity of the relation between the written and spoken form of languages varies, most languages have their regular and irregular phonological set of rules. In this paper, we present a system for the phonemic transcription of Hungarian. Beside the implementation of rules describing default letter-to-phoneme correspondences and morphophonological al- ternations, the tool incorporates the knowledge of a Hungarian morphological analyzer in order to be able to detect compound and other morpheme boundaries, and it contains a rich lexicon of entries with irregular pronunciation. It is shown that the system performs well even on texts containing a high number of foreign names. 1 INTRODUCTION In the research reported about in this study, our goal was to imple- ment a system that can automatically transform written Hungarian to its phonemic representation. The original intent of the system This is a pre-print version of the paper, before proper formatting and copyediting by the editorial staff. 162 ATTILA NOVÁK AND BORBÁLA SIKLÓSI was to transcribe a database of Hungarian geographic terms. How- ever, due to certain design decisions, our system proved to perform well also on texts containing a high ratio of foreign names and suf- fixed forms.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Santa Barbara Dotawo: a Journal of Nubian Studies
    UC Santa Barbara Dotawo: A Journal of Nubian Studies Title The Consonant System of Abu Jinuk (Kordofan Nubian) Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3kf38308 Journal Dotawo: A Journal of Nubian Studies, 2(1) Author Alshareef, Waleed Publication Date 2015-06-01 DOI 10.5070/D62110020 License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ 4.0 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California The Consonant System of 155 Abu Jinuk (Kordofan Nubian) Waleed Alshareef 1. Introduction Abu Jinuk is a Kordofan Nubian language mainly spoken in the northwestern Nuba Mountains of Sudan. Kordofan Nubian is a branch of the Nubian language family. According to Rilly,1 Nubian belongs to the northern East Sudanic subgroup which is part of the East Sudanic branch of the Nilo-Saharan phylum. According to the Sultan of the Abu Jinuk tribe, the population in 2010 was 5,896 of whom 3,556 speakers live in the Nuba Moun- tains and 2,340 are scattered in different towns of Sudan.2 Accord- ing to the informants, the people call themselves and their language [d̪εkla] meaning “the great grandfather.” The Arabic term “Abu Ji- nuk,” by which they are known in linguistic literature, is the name of their mountain. By the non-Arab neighboring groups, the Abu Jinuk people are called [εlεk], which means “the explorers.” Abu Jinuk is an undescribed language. No linguistic studies have been devoted to the phonology of this language. Therefore, exam- ining the consonant system of Abu Jinuk is thought to be the first linguistic investigation of this language.
    [Show full text]
  • The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk
    Background Methodology and Predictions Results Conclusions References The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk Andrew Garre & Tyler Lau University of California, Berkeley [email protected] [email protected] Society for the Study of the Indigenous Languages of the Americas (SSILA) 2018 Salt Lake City, UT, USA January 6, 2018 Andrew Garre & Tyler Lau The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk Background Methodology and Predictions Results Conclusions References Acknowledgements Many thanks to the following: • Karuk master speakers Sonny Davis and the late Lucille Albers, Charlie Thom, and especially Vina Smith; • research collaborators LuLu Alexander, Tamara Alexander, Crystal Richardson, and Florrine Super (in Yreka) and Erik H. Maier, Line Mikkelsen, and Clare Sandy (at Berkeley); and • Susan Lin and the audience at UC Berkeley’s Phonetics and Phonology Forum for insightful comments and suggestions. Data in this talk is drawn from Ararahi’urípih, a Karuk dictionary and text corpus (http://linguistics.berkeley.edu/~karuk). Andrew Garre & Tyler Lau The Emergence of Consonant-Vowel Metathesis in Karuk Background Methodology and Predictions Metathesis Results Karuk Conclusions References Overview • Karuk V1CV2 sequences show much coarticulation of V1 into V2 w j j /uCi/ ! [uC i], /iCa/ ! [iC a], /iCu/ ! [iC u] (all high V1) • We argue that this coarticulation is a source of CV metathesis along lines that are phonologized in other languages. • Goals • To figure out the environments in which this process occurs •
    [Show full text]
  • Issue 31.2.Indd
    Akanlig-Pare, G../Legon Journal of the Humanities Vol. 31.2 (2020) DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.4314/ljh.v31i2 .3 Palatalization in Central Bùlì George Akanlig-Pare Senior Lecturer University of Ghana, Legon Email: [email protected] Submitted: May 6, 2020/Accepted: November 20, 2020/Published: January 28, 2021 Abstract Palatalization is a process through which non-palatal consonants acquire palatality, either through a shift in place of articulation from a non-palatal region to the hard palate or through the superimposition of palatal qualities on a non-palatal consonant. In both cases, there is a front, non-low vowel or a palatal glide that triggers the process. In this paper, I examine the palatalization phenomena in Bùlì using Feature Geometry within the non- linear generative phonological framework. I argue that both full and secondary palatalization occur in Buli. The paper further explains that, the long high front vowel /i:/, triggers the formation of a palato-alveolar aff ricate which is realized in the Central dialect of Bùlì, where the Northern and Central dialects retain the derived palatal stop. Keywords: Palatalization, Bùlì, Feature Geometry, synchronic, diachronic Introduction Although linguists generally agree that palatalization is a process through which non-palatal consonants acquire palatality, they diff er in their accounts of the phonological processes that characterize it. As Halle (2005, p.1) states, “… palatalization raises numerous theoretical questions about which there is at present no agreement among phonologists”. Cross linguistic surveys conducted on the process reveal a number of issues that lead to the present state of aff airs.
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Sounds Can Symbolise the Felt Heaviness of Obje
    CORRESPONDENCES AND SYMBOLISM Cross-Sensory Correspondences in Language: Vowel Sounds can Symbolise the Felt Heaviness of Objects Peter Walker1,2 & Caroline Regina Parameswaran2 1 Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK 2 Department of Psychology, Sunway University, Malaysia Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Peter Walker, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, UK e-mail: [email protected] tel: +44 (0) 1524 593163 fax: +44 (0) 1524 593744 1 CORRESPONDENCES AND SYMBOLISM Abstract In sound symbolism, a word's sound induces expectations about the nature of a salient aspect of the word's referent. Walker (2016a) proposed that cross-sensory correspondences can be the source of these expectations and the present study assessed three implications flowing from this proposal. First, sound symbolism will embrace a wide range of referent features, including heaviness. Second, any feature of a word's sound able to symbolise one aspect of the word's referent will also be able to symbolise corresponding aspects of the referent (e.g., a sound feature symbolising visual pointiness will also symbolise lightness in weight). Third, sound symbolism will be independent of the sensory modality through which a word's referent is encoded (e.g., whether heaviness is felt or seen). Adults judged which of two contrasting novel words was most appropriate as a name for the heavier or lighter of two otherwise identical hidden novel objects they were holding in their hands. The alternative words contrasted in their vowels and/or consonants, one or both of which were known to symbolise visual pointiness. Though the plosive or continuant nature of the consonants did not influence the judged appropriateness of a word to symbolise the heaviness of its referent, back/open vowels, compared to front/close vowels, were judged to symbolise felt heaviness.
    [Show full text]
  • SSC: the Science of Talking
    SSC: The Science of Talking (for year 1 students of medicine) Week 3: Sounds of the World’s Languages (vowels and consonants) Michael Ashby, Senior Lecturer in Phonetics, UCL PLIN1101 Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology A Lecture 4 page 1 Vowel Description Essential reading: Ashby & Maidment, Chapter 5 4.1 Aim: To introduce the basics of vowel description and the main characteristics of the vowels of RP English. 4.2 Definition of vowel: Vowels are produced without any major obstruction of the airflow; the intra-oral pressure stays low, and vowels are therefore sonorant sounds. Vowels are normally voiced. Vowels are articulated by raising some part of the tongue body (that is the front or the back of the tongue notnot the tip or blade) towards the roof of the oral cavity (see Figure 1). 4.3 Front vowels are produced by raising the front of the tongue towards the hard palate. Back vowels are produced by raising the back of the tongue towards the soft palate. Central vowels are produced by raising the centre part of the tongue towards the junction of the hard and soft palates. 4.4 The height of a vowel refers to the degree of raising of the relevant part of the tongue. If the tongue is raised so as to be close to the roof of the oral cavity then a close or high vowel is produced. If the tongue is only slightly raised, so that there is a wide gap between its highest point and the roof of the oral cavity, then an open or lowlowlow vowel results.
    [Show full text]