Experimental Psychology

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Experimental Psychology IDOL Institute of Distance and Online Learning ENHANCE YOUR QUALIFICATION, ADVANCE YOUR CAREER. 2 M.A.(Psy) PSYCHOLOGY EXPERIMENTAL EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY Course Code: MAP 602 Semester: First SLM Unit : 1-2 E-Lesson 1 https://images.app.goo.gl/wydmG6zjdkZR3N7M9 www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY OBJECTIVES INTRODUCTION 3 After studying this unit, you will be able to: Explain the basic concepts Experimental psychology examines relationships between human behavior and Discuss the design of experimental methods the mind. Elaborate the viewpoints of psychologists, Physiologists. The law states that the change in a stimulus that will be just noticeable is a constant ratio Discuss the design of experimental methods of the original stimulus. www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602MAP 602) INSTITUTEAll right OFare DISTANCE reserved ANDwith ONLINECU-IDOL LEARNING TOPICS TO BE COVERED 4 > Meaning and Scope of Experimental Psychology > History of Experimental Psychology EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY EXPERIMENTAL > Ernst Heinrich Weber > Gustav Theodor Fechner > Contribution of Wundt www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL MEANING AND SCOPE OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 5 Meaning of Experimental Psychology Scope of Experimental Psychology • Experimental psychology refers to work done • The scope of Experimental Psychology is wide by those who apply experimental methods to p ning with the invention of new tools and instru sychological study and the processes that und ments for experiments. erlie it. • Therefore, it is in the fitness of things that exp • Experimental psychologists employ human par erimental psychology constitutes compulsory p ticipants and animal subjects to study a great art of courses of psychology for the under-grad many topics, including sensation & perception, uate and post-graduate students in universities memory, cognition, learning, motivation, emoti everywhere in the world on; developmental processes, social psycholo gy, and the neural substrates of all of these. www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL NATURE OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 6 1. Experimental psychology is important because the findings discovered by psychologists play a vital role in our understanding of the human mind and behavior. 2. Experimental psychology is the scientific and empirical approach to the study of the mind. 3. The experimental approach means that tests are administered to participants, with both control and experimental conditions. 4. Experimental psychology is concerned with testing theories of human thoughts, feelings, actions, and beyond any aspect of being human that involves the mind. 5. This ultimately meant that mental perception is responsive to the material world – the mind doesn’t passively respond to a stimulus (if that was the case, there would be a linear relationship between the intensity of a stimulus and the actual perception of it), but is dynamically responsive to it. 6. Wilhelm Wundt is often credited with being “the father of experimental psychology” and is the founding point for many aspects of it. He began the first experimental psychology lab, scientific journal, and ultimately formalized the approach as a science. www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL HISTORY OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 7 CHARLES BELL • Charles Bell was a British physiologist, whose main contribution was research involving the nervous system. • He wrote a pamphlet summarizing his research on rabbits. • His research concluded that sensory nerves enter at the posterior (dorsal) roots of the spinal cord and motor nerves emerge from the anterior (ventral) roots of the spinal cord. ERNST HEINRICH WEBER • Weber was a German physician who is credited with being one of the founders of experimental psychology. • Weber's main interests were the sense of touch and kinesthesis. • His most memorable contribution to the field of experimental psychology is the suggestion that judgments of sensory differences are relative and not absolute. GUSTAV FECHNER: • Fechner published in 1860 what is considered to be the first work of experimental psychology, "Elemente der Psychophysik." • Some historians date the beginning of experimental psychology from the publication of "Elemente. www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL HISTORY OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 8 OSWALD KÜLPE • Oswald Külpe is the main founder of the Würzburg School in Germany. • He was a pupil of Wilhelm Wundt for about twelve years. Unlike Wundt, Külpe believed experiments were possible to test higher mental processes. WÜRZBURG SCHOOL •The work of the Würzburg School was a milestone in the development of experimental psychology. •The School was founded by a group of psychologists led by Oswald Külpe, and it provided an alternative to the structuralism of Edward Titchener and Wilhelm Wundt. George Trumbull Ladd •Experimental psychology was introduced into the United States by George Trumbull Ladd, who founded Yale University's psychological laboratory in 1879. In 1887 www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL UNDERSTANDING EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 9 . Experimental psychologists are interested in exploring theoretical questions, often by creating a hypothesis and then setting out to prove or disprove it through experimentation. .They study a wide range of behavioral topics among humans and animals, including sensation, perception, attention, memory, cognition and emotion. In a sense, all psychologists can be considered experimental psychologists since research is the foundation of the discipline, and many psychologists split their professional focus among research, patient care, teaching or program administration. Experimental psychologists, however, often devote their full attention to research its design, execution, analysis and dissemination. .Experimental psychologists use basic and applied research to explore questions about human and animal behavior. They often use their scientific findings to provide insights that create safer workplaces and transportation systems, improve teaching and learning methods, promote healthy child development and improve substance abuse treatment programs, to list a few examples www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL ERNST HEINRICH WEBER 10 Ernst Heinrich Weber, German anatomist and physiologist whose fundamental studies of the sense of touch introduced a concept that of the just-noticeable difference, the smallest difference perceivable between two similar stimuli that is important to psychology and sensory physiology Ernst Heinrich Weber was born on 24 June 1795 in Witte nberg, Saxony, Holy Roman Empire. At a young age, Weber became interested in physics and t he sciences after being heavily influenced by Ernst Chladni, a physicist often referred to as the “father of acoustics”. In 1811, he began medicine at the University of Wittenberg. He devoted his life to science and research. https://images.app.goo.gl/kWZhf15VDGv9s28XA www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL GUSTAV THEODOR FECHNER 11 Fechner was born at Groß Särchen, near Mukau, in Low er Lusatia. Fechner, along with Wilhelm Wundt and Hermann von H elmholtz, is recognized as one of the founders of modern experimental psychology His clearest contribution was the demonstration that because the mind was susceptible to measurement and mathematical treatment, psychology had the potential to become a quantified science. In 1970, the International Astronomical Union named a crater on the far side of the moon after Fechner. https://images.app.goo.gl/KEV3xiEvygR1ur1q7 www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL GUSTAV THEODOR FECHNER 12 Fechner Day • In 1985 the International Society for Psychophysics called its annual conference Fechner Day. • The conference is now scheduled to include 22 October to allow psychophysicists to celebrate the anniversary of Fechner's waking up on that day in 1850 with a new approach into how to study the mi nd. Family and later life Little is known of Fechner's later years, nor of the circumstances, cause, and manner of his death. www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL CONTRIBUTION OF WUNDT 13 Wundt's aim was to record thoughts and sensations, and to analyze them into their constituent elements, in much the same way as a chemist analyses chemical compounds, in order to get at the underlying structure. The school of psychology founded by Wundt is known as voluntarism, the process of organizing the mind. Wundt contributed to the development of psychology was to do his research in carefully controlled conditions, i.e. experimental methods. This encouraged other researchers such as the be haviorists to follow the same experimental approach and be more scientific. https://images.app.goo.gl/9yFT9dWGho5aD9eTA www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved with CU-IDOL KEY WORDS/ABBREVIATIONS 14 Experimental Psychology - Experimental psychology examines relationships between human behavior and the mind. Experimental Method Design - Lab experiments are very common in psychology. Design of Experiments - Sound methodology is essential to the study of complex behavioral and mental processes. Modern Experimental psychology - Began with the adoption of experimental methods at the en d of 19th century. Physical stimuli - In phyisology, a stimulas is a dectable change. www.cuidol.in Unit-1-2 MAP602 All right are reserved
Recommended publications
  • III. Embodied Cognition A
    +(,121/,1( Citation: 35 Queen's L.J. 117 2009-2010 Content downloaded/printed from HeinOnline (http://heinonline.org) Wed Nov 17 15:51:32 2010 -- Your use of this HeinOnline PDF indicates your acceptance of HeinOnline's Terms and Conditions of the license agreement available at http://heinonline.org/HOL/License -- The search text of this PDF is generated from uncorrected OCR text. -- To obtain permission to use this article beyond the scope of your HeinOnline license, please use: https://www.copyright.com/ccc/basicSearch.do? &operation=go&searchType=0 &lastSearch=simple&all=on&titleOrStdNo=0316-778X Embodied Rationality BarbaraA. Spellman and Simone Schnall* Standard law and economics theory relies on the assumption that human beings act as ideal rationaldecision makers. However, significantpsychological researchhas undermined the view that individuals act completely rationally. The authors detail a recent approach to the human mind known as "embodied cognition", which maintains that mental processes are grounded in actual bodily states. This link between the mind and body is not captured in the standard view of the rationalhuman. Studying the mind in relation to the body can help us better understandand predict seemingly irrationalactions. The authors describe the precursors to the embodied cognition movement, and note that although embodied cognition is similar to earlierapproaches that consideredheuristics, it offers a more complete theory of human behaviour. They use embodied cognition as the basisfor an expanded notion of embodied rationality that goes beyond the domain of affect and actions into the domain ofjudgments. The concept of embodied rationalitycan be applied to reasoning and decision-makingprocesses central to Behavioural Law and Economics.
    [Show full text]
  • Weberˇs Planetary Model of the Atom
    Weber’s Planetary Model of the Atom Bearbeitet von Andre Koch Torres Assis, Gudrun Wolfschmidt, Karl Heinrich Wiederkehr 1. Auflage 2011. Taschenbuch. 184 S. Paperback ISBN 978 3 8424 0241 6 Format (B x L): 17 x 22 cm Weitere Fachgebiete > Physik, Astronomie > Physik Allgemein schnell und portofrei erhältlich bei Die Online-Fachbuchhandlung beck-shop.de ist spezialisiert auf Fachbücher, insbesondere Recht, Steuern und Wirtschaft. Im Sortiment finden Sie alle Medien (Bücher, Zeitschriften, CDs, eBooks, etc.) aller Verlage. Ergänzt wird das Programm durch Services wie Neuerscheinungsdienst oder Zusammenstellungen von Büchern zu Sonderpreisen. Der Shop führt mehr als 8 Millionen Produkte. Weber’s Planetary Model of the Atom Figure 0.1: Wilhelm Eduard Weber (1804–1891) Foto: Gudrun Wolfschmidt in der Sternwarte in Göttingen 2 Nuncius Hamburgensis Beiträge zur Geschichte der Naturwissenschaften Band 19 Andre Koch Torres Assis, Karl Heinrich Wiederkehr and Gudrun Wolfschmidt Weber’s Planetary Model of the Atom Ed. by Gudrun Wolfschmidt Hamburg: tredition science 2011 Nuncius Hamburgensis Beiträge zur Geschichte der Naturwissenschaften Hg. von Gudrun Wolfschmidt, Geschichte der Naturwissenschaften, Mathematik und Technik, Universität Hamburg – ISSN 1610-6164 Diese Reihe „Nuncius Hamburgensis“ wird gefördert von der Hans Schimank-Gedächtnisstiftung. Dieser Titel wurde inspiriert von „Sidereus Nuncius“ und von „Wandsbeker Bote“. Andre Koch Torres Assis, Karl Heinrich Wiederkehr and Gudrun Wolfschmidt: Weber’s Planetary Model of the Atom. Ed. by Gudrun Wolfschmidt. Nuncius Hamburgensis – Beiträge zur Geschichte der Naturwissenschaften, Band 19. Hamburg: tredition science 2011. Abbildung auf dem Cover vorne und Titelblatt: Wilhelm Weber (Kohlrausch, F. (Oswalds Klassiker Nr. 142) 1904, Frontispiz) Frontispiz: Wilhelm Weber (1804–1891) (Feyerabend 1933, nach S.
    [Show full text]
  • Fechner's Inner Psychophysics Viewed From
    FECHNER’S INNER PSYCHOPHYSICS VIEWED FROM BOTH A HERBARTIAN AND A FECHNERIAN PERSPECTIVE David J. Murray Department of Psychology, Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario and Christina A. Bandomir Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario Abstract Following brief summaries of the overlapping lives of Herbart, E. H. Weber, Fechner, and Helmholtz, it is shown that Herbart and Fechner agreed about the nature of mental science in three particular ways. First, both adopted ideas about force and energy from the physical sciences; Herbart’s interplay of Vorstellungen assumed that equilibrium states were attained when opposing forces balanced out, while Fechner’s notion of brain oscillations was based on the idea that to every action there corresponded a reaction. Second, both assumed that Vorstellungen could be assigned magnitudes, but Fechner was far more concerned than was Herbart with the problem of how to measure those magnitudes. Third, the threshold of consciousness was defined in mathematical terms by Herbart, whereas the absolute threshold associated with a sensation magnitude was speculated about in physiological terms by Fechner. In order to show both how Herbart and Fechner tried to base their respective theories of mind on the physics available to them, it will be useful first to outline their respective biographies, along with those of E. H. Weber and of H. von Helmholtz. This will enable us to gain an overview of how the lives of these pioneer psychologists overlapped, and, more important, to relate their life-stories to one of the most important innovations in the history of physics, namely, the enunciation of the principle of the conservation of energy.
    [Show full text]
  • Hermann Lotze
    Hermann Lotze An Intellectual Biography WILLIAM R. WOODWARD University of New Hampshire Kg Cambridge UNIVERSITY PRESS Contents List of Figures page ix Preface xiii List of Abbreviations xix Introduction: A Scientific Intellectual Biography between Biedermeier and Modern Cosmopolitan Thought i PART I YOUTH IN BIEDERMEIER 1 Ancestry and Education of a Cultural Reformer (1817-1834) 2.1 2 Education in Medical Thought and Practice: Working Explanations (1834-1838) 44 3 Education in Philosophy: The Mathematical Construction of Space(1834-1839) 67 4 A Gestalt Metaphysics: Laws, Events, and Values (1838-1841) 9° 5 Applying Hypotheses in Pathology and Therapy (1838-1842) 123 6 The Dual Model of Explanation and Speculation (1838-1843) 147 PART II EMERGING BOURGEOIS LIBERALISM 7 Levels of Physiological Explanation (1843-1851) 173 8 The Physical-Mental Mechanism: An Alternative to Psychophysics (1846-1852) 202 vii viii Contents 9 Inner Migration or Disguised Reform? Political Interests of Philosophical Anthropology (1851-1864) 228 10 From the Evolution of Culture to the Human Sciences (1852-1858) 252 11 A Feminist Turn in Secular Theology (1858-1864) 284 PART III THE SYSTEM IN THE BISMARCK PERIOD 12 From an Aesthetics of Everyday Life to Dilthey's Lived Experience (1864-1868) 323 13 Between Objectivism and Relativism: Logic as Theory of Inquiry (1868-1874) 352 14 The Metaphysical Foundations of Matter and Mind (1874-1879) 378 15 The Personal Is the Political: A Cosmopolitan Ethics (1864-1881) 406 Postscript: Historiographic Lessons of Lotze Research 435 Appendix 1 Chronology of Hermann Lotze's Life 449 Appendix 2 Publications and Published Letters of Hermann Lotze 4 5 z Appendix 3 Unpublished Sources 460 Appendix 4 Dissertations with Lotze's Evaluative Remarks (Promotionsschriften and Habilitationsschriften) 465 Index 475 Figures o.i Lotze at age twenty-six in Leipzig.
    [Show full text]
  • Pain” in the Modern Neurosciences: a Historical Account of the Visualization Technologies Used in the Development of an “Algesiogenic Pathology”, 1850 to 2000
    Brain Sci. 2015, 5, 521-545; doi:10.3390/brainsci5040521 OPEN ACCESS brain sciences ISSN 2076-3425 www.mdpi.com/journal/brainsci/ Review Objectifying “Pain” in the Modern Neurosciences: A Historical Account of the Visualization Technologies Used in the Development of an “Algesiogenic Pathology”, 1850 to 2000 Frank W. Stahnisch Department of Community Health Sciences & Department of History, The University of Calgary, 3280 Hospital Drive NW, Calgary T2N 4Z6, AB, Canada; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-403-210-6290. Academic Editor: Patrick W. Stroman Received: 31 August 2015 / Accepted: 9 November 2015 / Published: 17 November 2015 Abstract: Particularly with the fundamental works of the Leipzig school of experimental psychophysiology (between the 1850s and 1880s), the modern neurosciences witnessed an increasing interest in attempts to objectify “pain” as a bodily signal and physiological value. This development has led to refined psychological test repertoires and new clinical measurement techniques, which became progressively paired with imaging approaches and sophisticated theories about neuropathological pain etiology. With the advent of electroencephalography since the middle of the 20th century, and through the use of brain stimulation technologies and modern neuroimaging, the chosen scientific route towards an ever more refined “objectification” of pain phenomena took firm root in Western medicine. This article provides a broad overview of landmark events and key imaging technologies, which represent the long developmental path of a field that could be called “algesiogenic pathology.” Keywords: pain; history of medicine; Leipzig; Montreal; New York; nineteenth century; precursors to functional neuroimaging of pain; twentieth century “The past of a present-day science is not the same thing as that science in the past.” (Georges Canguilhem) [1] 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Phd in Philosophy
    PhD in Philosophy CYCLE XXXII COORDINATOR Prof. Adriano Fabris Hermann Lotze on the Mind–Body Problem and the 19th Century Philosophy and Psychology: With Special Attention to William James Academic Discipline (SSD) M-FIL/06 Doctoral Candidate Supervisors Michele Vagnetti Prof. Alessandro Pagnini Apl. Prof. Dr. Nikolay Milkov Coordinator Prof. Fabris Adriano Years 2017/2020 CONTENTS 1. Preliminary remarks on Hermann Lotze’s metaphysics 1 1.1. Lotze on science and metaphysics 1 1.2. Lotze’s atomism 6 1.3. Philosophy of space: metaphysics and geometry 12 2. Physiological and psychological studies in the first half of the 19th century 26 2.1. The study of the nervous system 26 2.2. Hermann Lotze on phrenology and the question of the location of the mind 35 2.3. Three scientific psychologists 39 2.3.1. Ernst Heinrich Weber 39 2.3.2. Gustav Theodor Fechner 44 2.3.3. Alfred Wilhelm Volkmann 53 3. The reductionist approach to the mind–body problem and its critics 54 3.1. Hermann von Helmholtz 56 3.2. The dispute between Büchner and Lotze 60 3.3. Two further scholars 64 3.3.1. J. F. Herbart 64 3.3.2. J. F. Fries 69 4. Hermann Lotze on the relation between body and mind 78 4.1. Pilosophical psychology and perspectivism 86 4.2. Physiological psychology between materialism and mentalism 94 4.3. The psycho-physical mechanism: the occasionalist way 103 I 4.4. The psycho-physical mechanism: the interaction 110 4.5. Lotze’s theory of local signs 113 4.6.
    [Show full text]
  • HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY: VOLUME 1, HISTORY of PSYCHOLOGY
    HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY: VOLUME 1, HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Donald K. Freedheim Irving B. Weiner John Wiley & Sons, Inc. HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY HANDBOOK of PSYCHOLOGY VOLUME 1 HISTORY OF PSYCHOLOGY Donald K. Freedheim Volume Editor Irving B. Weiner Editor-in-Chief John Wiley & Sons, Inc. This book is printed on acid-free paper. ➇ Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. All rights reserved. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 750-4470, or on the web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008, e-mail: [email protected]. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials.
    [Show full text]
  • Life and Work
    1 LIFE AND WORK . Ancestry and Early Years GUSTAV THEODOR FECHNER was born on April , in Gross-Särchen, a village situated on the Neisse river in the southeastern region of Lower Lusatia.¹ His father, Samuel Traugott Fechner (–), had been a pastor there (prob- ably Lutheran) since , as his father before him had also been. Fechner’s mother Dorothea Fechner (–), née Fischer, descended likewise from a regional pastoral family. From childhood on Fechner himself, the second of five children, was meant to join the clergy, too. His older brother Eduard Clemens (–) became an artist and moved to Paris in , where he later died. The three younger siblings were Fechner’s sisters Emilie, Clementine, and Mathilde. Fechner’s father has been described as a typical pastor of enlightened times: of a zealous nature, yet open-minded for progress. He was the first in his region to have a lightning rod mounted on the church; he upset the congregation by not wearing a wig during sermons; he had his children vaccinated, and he was a passionate fruit-grower. His young children were taught Latin—at the age of three, little Theo (Fechner’s nickname) spoke Latin as fluently as he did German. Fechner’s mother was affectionate, cheerful, friendly, and poetic, a woman who gathered a social circle around herself in all of life’s situations. Following their father’s premature death in , both sons were sent for a Copyright © Michael Heidelberger from Nature From Within History few years to their maternal uncle, also a pastor, in Wurzen and Ranis in Thuringia. In Gustav Theodor was enrolled in secondary school in Sorau (now called Zary), a town near the village where he was born; later he spent two years at the School of the Cross in Dresden, where the Fechner children were reunited with their mother.
    [Show full text]
  • Making Sense of Temperature in Early Psychophysics
    International Journal of Communication 8 (2014), Feature 2513–2517 1932–8036/2014FEA0002 Making Sense of Temperature in Early Psychophysics ALICE R. CHRISTENSEN Princeton University, USA How do human bodies sense the hotness and coldness of things? Not like a thermometer does, answers Ernst Heinrich Weber in Tastsinn und Gemeingefühl (Sense of Touch and Common Feeling) (1846). A thermometer registers the “heat of the mercury” at any given moment, while the human body instead registers changes from its “zero point” (the temperature of the body or skin) as positive and negative magnitudes. Sense of Touch and Common Feeling was originally published as part of a series of articles on physiological topics edited by Rudolph Wagner. The text expands considerably upon Weber’s earlier Latin treatise on the tactile sense, De Tactu (1834), and is, generally speaking, more theoretically ambitious than the earlier work. While the first half of Sense of Touch presents research on the effects of weight, pressure, and temperature on the nervous system of the skin, the second half of the text interrogates the connection between mind and body in the context of coanaesthesis (i.e., generalized feelings that cannot be attributed primarily to an external source such as pain, hunger, etc.).1 It would be hard to overstate the importance of Weber’s 1846 text for the subsequent development of the disciplines of experimental psychology and sensory physiology (Ross & Murray, 1996). Indeed, Gustav Fechner referred to his former mentor Weber as the “father of psychophysics” (1860, p. v), stating that in the “classic investigations” of Sense of Touch, Weber had laid out the basic categories of psychophysical inquiry.
    [Show full text]
  • Scientific Psychology in the 18Th Century
    PPSXXX10.1177/1745691616635601Schwarz, PfisterEmpirical Psychology in the 18th Century 635601research-article2016 Perspectives on Psychological Science 2016, Vol. 11(3) 399 –407 Scientific Psychology in the 18th © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Century: A Historical Rediscovery DOI: 10.1177/1745691616635601 pps.sagepub.com Katharina A. Schwarz and Roland Pfister Department of Psychology, University of Würzburg Abstract As early as 1783, the almost forgotten philosopher, metaphysicist, and psychologist Ferdinand Ueberwasser (1752– 1812) designated himself “Professor für empirische Psychologie und Logik” (professor of empirical psychology and logic) at the University of Münster, Germany. His position was initiated and supported by the minister and educational reformer Franz von Fürstenberg (1729–1810), who considered psychology a core scientific discipline that should be taught at each school and university. At the end of the 18th century, then, psychology seems to have been on the verge of becoming an independent academic discipline, about 100 years before Wilhelm Wundt founded the discipline’s first official laboratory. It seems surprising that Ueberwasser’s writings—including a seminal textbook on empirical psychology—have been almost entirely overlooked in most historical accounts. We focus on this important founding moment of psychological science and on the circumstances that eventually brought this seminal development to a halt. Keywords history of psychology, 18th century, psychology, philosophy The advent of psychology is traditionally considered to Ueberwasser (1752–1812) designated himself “Professor für have occurred in the late 19th century, with Wilhelm empirische Psychologie und Logik”—professor of empirical Wundt founding the discipline’s first official laboratory in psychology and logic—at the University of Münster, 1879 (Boring, 1929).
    [Show full text]
  • Contents More Information
    Cambridge University Press 978-1-107-10989-6 — A History of Modern Psychology Per Saugstad Table of Contents More Information Contents List of Illustrations page xvii Foreword xix 1 Introduction 1 A Short Characterization of Present-Day Psychology 1 The Present Approach to the Study of the History of Psychology 2 How Did Empirical Psychology Originate? 2 Edwin Boring’s Book on the History of Psychology 3 History as University Politics 3 Philosophy and Scientiic Empirical Psychology 4 Sigmund Koch’s View of the History of Psychology 5 Expansion in Natural Science 7 Where Did Psychology Originate? 7 The Emergence of Scientiic Psychology 8 Why Is Psychology Changing? 10 Progress in Empirical Psychology 11 No Simple Criteria for the Assessment of Progress in Empirical Research 12 Plan for the Book 13 2 The Scientiic and Intellectual Environment of the Mid-1800s 14 A New View of Body and Soul 14 Immediate Experience: The Soul Detached from the Body 15 Progress in the Study of the Brain 16 The Concept of the Relex in Philosophy and Early Physiology 17 Positivism and British Empiricist Philosophy 19 Auguste Comte (1798–1857): A Society Governed by Science 20 Science and Society 21 John Stuart Mill (1806–1873) 21 Scientiic Thinking During the 1800s: Mechanism and Positivism 25 The Emergence of Mechanistic Views in Psychology 25 Ernst Mach (1838–1916) 26 The Scientiic Ideal of Later Positivism: Accurate Observation and Description 27 © in this web service Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-1-107-10989-6
    [Show full text]
  • Stumpf, Hornbostel, and Comparative Musicology in Berlin
    Melting Musics, Fusing Sounds Stumpf, Hornbostel, and Comparative Musicology in Berlin R M Naturalism and musical otherness Observations on the peculiarities that distinguish the music of various popula- tions always raised the interest of philosophers and musicians. Ancient Greeks used to assign ethnic names such as Doric, Phrygian, Lydian, etc., to their differ- ent scales, but curiosity as to the variety of the exotic musical systems remarkably increased in the modern age. Geographic discoveries face the Europeans with puzzling forms of musical otherness, and some attention begins to be paid to the repertory of the domestic barbarians living in the countryside of cultivated Europe. Yet, it is only by the beginning of the twentieth century that a systematic discipline with genuine scientific ambitions eventually begins its process of insti- tutional recognition under the name of comparative musicology. A major role in this process is played by Carl Stumpf and his pupil Erich Moritz von Hornbostel, who initiated the so-called Berlin School of vergleichende Musikwissenschaft [Fig. ]. As they clearly recognized, technological progress has an important part in this development. Until then, the study of exotic music had relied upon the haphazard and amateurish transcriptions into the Western no- tational system made by missionary fathers or musically educated travelers. /e possibility of recording music on Edison phonographic cylinders permanently changed this state of affairs. Primitive as they may appear today, phonographic recordings allow preservation, promote objectivity and provide a general idea of timbre and other performative characteristics of the recorded items. Yet, intellectual factors must be also considered in the development of com- parative musicology.
    [Show full text]