A Feasibility Study of Hypercorrection in The

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Feasibility Study of Hypercorrection in The I IS PRESTIGIOUS: A FEASIBILITY STUDY OF HYPERCORRECTION IN THE ACCUSATIVE CASE IN AMERICAN ENGLISH A RESEARCH PAPER SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS BY BENJAMIN D. BISHOP GUILHERME DUARTE GARCIA - ADVISOR BALL STATE UNIVERSITY MUNCIE, INDIANA MAY 2020 1 I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 2 Contents 1 Abstract 4 2 Introduction 4 2.1 Linguistic Viruses . 4 2.2 Hypercorrection . 5 2.3 Identity in L1 English Hypercorrection . 7 2.4 Instructional Implications . 9 2.5 Objective . 10 3 Methods 12 4 Experiment 1 13 4.1 Methodology, Experiment 1 . 13 4.2 Results, Experiment 1 . 15 4.3 Discussion, Experiment 1 . 17 4.3.1 Hypercorrection and grouping . 17 4.3.2 Hypercorrection and time in residence . 18 4.3.3 Hypercorrection and age . 19 4.3.4 Hypercorrection and degree . 19 4.4 Overall Discussion . 19 5 Experiment 2 20 5.1 Methodology Experiment 2 . 20 5.2 Results, Experiment 2 . 21 5.3 Discussion, Experiment 2 . 23 5.3.1 Positional Import . 23 5.3.2 Education . 24 5.3.3 Age . 24 I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 3 6 General Discussion 25 6.1 Degree and hypercorrection in culture . 25 6.2 Age and hypercorrection in culture . 26 6.3 The linguistic virus' structure . 27 6.3.1 The Linguistic Virus' Internal Structure . 27 6.3.2 The Linguistic Virus' External Placement . 28 6.3.3 The Linguistic Virus' Virulence . 28 6.4 Implications for instruction . 28 7 Conclusions 29 8 Implications for Future Research 30 I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 4 1 Abstract Hypercorrection is an overgeneralization of a prestige semantic struc- ture (Piattelli-Palmarini and Uriagereka, 2004). In this study on the feasi- bility of the study of hypercorrection in English, I discuss the implications of hypercorrection of first-person pronouns within noun pairs, such as \Bill and me," versus \Bill and I", in terminal and medial positions. Results show that speakers hypercorrect to an extent which is significant to draw some conclusions and warrant a further study, as well as showing that age appears to have a significant effect on the frequency of hypercorrection. 2 Introduction Hypercorrection is often demonstrated as being an intrinsic factor of those from a nonstandard dialect of English(DeCamp, 1972). This prescrip- tive approach to the use of hypercorrection therefore implies that the act is a mistake made by those who are not as skilled in the language as a well- educated native speaker. The following section will provide some degree of background of this topic, which will be followed by the methodology for the study undertaken, and the results, all of which come together to ask: How often do native speakers of American English hypercorrect, what factors in- fluence hypercorrection, and is it something which should be discussed in English Education? As such, the study will be looking into the extent to which First-Language (L1) speakers of American English hypercorrect ut- terances, specifically looking at those which change from the first-person accusative case into a nominative form. 2.1 Linguistic Viruses A linguistic virus, here defined as a summarization of Piattelli-Palmarini and Uriagereka (2004) and Lasnik and Sobin (2000), would be a prestige form I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 5 of a language which emerges as a grammatically incorrect construction. A phonetic example of this would be the pronunciation of the word \forte" as [fOôteI], rather than the phonologically- and historically-driven pronunciation [fOôt]. The former, given its spelling and french-esque pronunciation, could easily be perceived as the more prestigious, given the tendency of latinate languages to be so determined in English-speaking communities (Lutz, 2013). In order to understand hypercorrection we must first regard the con- cept of a linguistic virus and how, precisely, one may come to be. To begin, the universal grammar version of a linguistic virus takes it as a biological left- over, in the strain of so-called junk DNA (Ponting and Lunter, 2006; Piattelli- Palmarini and Uriagereka, 2004). This concept essentially posits that lan- guage, upon the emergence of a different linguistic system in a speaker, leaves behind its lexical and semantic footprint, as a sort of interlanguage (Kasper and Blum-Kulka, 1993). This causes the speaker of a language to be reliant upon their original linguistic system in order to produce language in a Sec- ond Language (L2), this leading to a system-wide interlanguage (Selinker and Rutherford, 2013). This is certainly one manner through which a lin- guistic virus may emerge. However, a truer one would be best looked at less as a result of maintaining linguistic information by one speaker, and more along the lines of a grammatically incorrect piece of language passed from one speaker to another, either vertically or horizontally, by way of a com- munity of practice and prestige speech (Piattelli-Palmarini and Uriagereka, 2004; Lasnik and Sobin, 2000). 2.2 Hypercorrection A linguistic virus hereforth shall refer to the phenomenon of mainte- nance of form and structure of a phrase or word based upon not its gram- matically correct position and conjugation, but upon the method through which it operates in accordance to special rules which allow it an exceptional place wherein rules of grammar or orthography need not apply. I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 6 Hypercorrection is often defined as being an aspect of L2 English speakers, these speakers overapplying rules and concepts which were given to them in order to correct their language into one which is far beyond what the rules of English actually permit (DeCamp, 1972). This overapplication of a rule serves as the basis for our concept here. Take, for example, an L1 English speaker who determines, whether consciously or unconsciously, that \I" is a more socially advanced use of the first-person pronoun than is \me." This belief then translates into a consis- tent use of the believed privileged form of the pronoun, even in situations when it would be grammatically correct to use the less-privileged form. An example of this would be the use of \were" in order to create a subjunctive sentence which is not intended as such, though processed as being a more advanced and \prestigious" form of grammar. This is further explored in Ryan (1961). This is the definition of hypercorrection which shall be studied in the following experiments. For the purposes of this study, we will observe the hypercorrection of the English-Language accusative case into the pronoun form which is gener- ally present in the nominative case. Take, for example, the sentences: 1. Lily loves James and me. 2. Lily loves James and I. Example 1 Sentence 2 in Example 1 demonstrates the supplantation of the ac- cusative form the English first person \me" in a group with one which does not track with traditional grammatical structures, specifically, \I." This rule of \I" being perceived as more prestigious may very well be due to the un- derstanding of \me" in the nominative case is traditionally used in media to portray those who have an incomplete handle on language and/or are unin- telligent. As such, the rule of saying \I" instead of the accusative alternative is established as being prestigious. I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 7 Given that the occurrence of this instance of hypercorrection is gen- erally agreed upon in the linguistic community, the objectives of this paper will be A) to establish its frequency of use among members of the target population and B) to establish the extent to which L2 English learners may need to know about this change. 2.3 Identity in L1 English Hypercorrection This phenomenon of rule overgeneralization extends beyond second language learning, however, as students from nonstandard American dialects of English have been shown to demonstrate hypercorrect responses in up to 15% of cases in the L1 English language classroom, specifically in primary school grammar classes (Pfaff, 1976). This phenomenon has been demon- strated as not established solely in the English language classroom. As a matter of fact, there are established cases of this specific rule generalization in native speakers who rank highly in academia, the likes of which exchanged \was" for \were," which is used in order to make the mood subjunctive, but was not standard in the studied dialect at the time (Ryan, 1961; Cazden, 1975). Looking more into the processes behind these changes, previous liter- ature makes apparent the system of linguistic stratification among pronun- ciation and lexical decisions. Take, for example, Labov (1972), wherein the language choices made by speakers of AAE designate them as members of the African American community. Taking this into account with the find- ings of Pfaff (1976), one may draw the conclusion that the hypercorrection undertaken may be in order to form an identity which fits in more with a population showing a distinct lexical variety which may be different from that which the speaker is already a part of, which further adheres to the definition of hypercorrection as established here. This phenomenon of identity construction and deconstruction by way of linguistic choices is established rather thoroughly in the literature. Take, I IS PRESTIGIOUS Page 8 for example, the work which has been done by Podesva (2011) in the Califor- nia and Texas Gay communities, which indicate that individuals' linguistic choices underlie their intended persona within a community of practice. For example, the structures which a speaker takes in order to get across a set of identity markers may indicate that this speaker is a set of personality markers which indicate their intended final personality structure. In the case of Podesva and Chun (2007), multiple factors came together in order to de- termine that a speaker may be a \diva," with this determined by a set of personality traits and phonological choices.
Recommended publications
  • Open Research Online Oro.Open.Ac.Uk
    Open Research Online The Open University’s repository of research publications and other research outputs “You can just give those documents to myself’: Untriggered reflexive pronouns in 21st century spoken British English Book Section How to cite: Paterson, Laura (2018). “You can just give those documents to myself’: Untriggered reflexive pronouns in 21st century spoken British English. In: Brezina, Vaclav; Love, Robbie and Aijmer, Karin eds. Corpus Approaches to Contemporary British Speech. Sociolinguistic Studies of the Spoken (BNC2014). Abingdon: Routledge. For guidance on citations see FAQs. c 2018 Routledge Version: Accepted Manuscript Link(s) to article on publisher’s website: https://www.routledge.com/Corpus-Approaches-to-Contemporary-British-Speech-Sociolinguistic-Studies/Brezina-Love-Aijmer/p/book/9781138287273 Copyright and Moral Rights for the articles on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. For more information on Open Research Online’s data policy on reuse of materials please consult the policies page. oro.open.ac.uk You can just give those documents to myself’: Untriggered reflexive pronouns in 21st century spoken British English Laura L. Paterson, The Open University, UK 1. Introduction Reflexive pronouns, such as myself, herself, yourselves, share a real-world referent with other components of the clause (or local domain) in which they occur. As such, they require a coreferent noun phrase (NP) to fulfil their syntactic criteria. In the sentence ‘The cat washes herself’, the NP the cat and the reflexive pronoun herself correspond to the same entity and share a syntactic bond. However, despite formal syntactic constraints, reflexive pronouns are used without coreferent NPs in (some varieties of) English (Parker, Riley and Meyer 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • LEXICAL and SOCIOLINGUISTIC VARIATION in QATARI ARABIC by Darwish Ghuloom Hussein Yousef Al-Amadidhi Thesis Submitted for the De
    LEXICAL AND SOCIOLINGUISTICVARIATION IN QATARI ARABIC by Darwish Ghuloom Hussein Yousef Al-amadidhi Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy University of Edinburgh 1985 Dedication To my parents and my wife with love and gratitude Abstract This thesis embodies the result of an investigation into two linguistic variables: the (d3) and the (Q) in QD. The basic issue tackled is this: are variations observed in these variables rule governed? If so, are they linguistic or non-linguistic? A close examination of the data has shown that the variables are governed to a great extent by the class of lexical item containing the variaoles. Moreover they have demonstrated co-variation with paralinguistic factors such as social group membership, age, level of education and style. The social motivation for change and variation are highlighted. Such processes occur as a result of status-ranking of local social dialects and as a result of the tendency of the younger people to modify their speech in the direction of the superimposed variety, which is learnt at school. The impact of the process of modernization on linguistic change is also examined. Acknowledgement I would like to thank the University of Qatar for providing me with the full scholarship which made it possible for me to continue my post graduate studies and complete this work. I must also- express my deep appreciation and thanks to the Cultural Attache's Office (Embassy of Qatar) in London for the help they offered during my stay in Britain-. This thesis would perhaps not have been completed without the interest, encouragement, patience and perspicacious criticism of my supervisor, Professor James R.
    [Show full text]
  • Usage, Rules, and Style
    USAGE, RULES, AND STYLE BY JACK LYNCH, ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR OF ENGLISH RUTGERS UNIVERSITY, NEWARK , NEW JERSEY Web: http://andromeda.rutgers.edu/~jlynch/Writing/links.html Email: <[email protected]> The Above, The Following. Lists are common in some sorts of writing, introduced by the following and referred to by the above. But you can often make a sentence clearer and punchier with simple pronouns: instead of the above topics, try these topics — the context makes your subject clear. Action Verbs. Action verbs, as the name reveals, express actions; contrast them with verbs of being. Think of the difference between "I study" (action verb, even if it's not the most exciting action) and "I am a student" (verb of being). It's often wise to cut down on verbs of being, replacing them (whenever possible) with action verbs; that'll make your writing punchier. Whatever you do, though, don't confuse action verbs with the active voice, which is the opposite of the passive voice. Sentences with verbs of being (such as am, is, are, were) aren't necessarily passive sentences, even if they're often weak ones. Affect versus Effect. Affect with an a is usually a verb; effect with an e is (usually) a noun. When you affect something, you have an effect on it. The usual adjective is effective. If the usuals leave you curious, here's the rest of the story: affective as an adjective means "relating to or arousing an emotional reaction"; effect as a verb means "to bring about" or "to accomplish," as in "to effect a change." All of.
    [Show full text]
  • MIXED CODES, BILINGUALISM, and LANGUAGE MAINTENANCE DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requi
    BILINGUAL NAVAJO: MIXED CODES, BILINGUALISM, AND LANGUAGE MAINTENANCE DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Charlotte C. Schaengold, M.A. ***** The Ohio State University 2004 Dissertation Committee: Approved by Professor Brian Joseph, Advisor Professor Donald Winford ________________________ Professor Keith Johnson Advisor Linguistics Graduate Program ABSTRACT Many American Indian Languages today are spoken by fewer than one hundred people, yet Navajo is still spoken by over 100,000 people and has maintained regional as well as formal and informal dialects. However, the language is changing. While the Navajo population is gradually shifting from Navajo toward English, the “tip” in the shift has not yet occurred, and enormous efforts are being made in Navajoland to slow the language’s decline. One symptom in this process of shift is the fact that many young people on the Reservation now speak a non-standard variety of Navajo called “Bilingual Navajo.” This non-standard variety of Navajo is the linguistic result of the contact between speakers of English and speakers of Navajo. Similar to Michif, as described by Bakker and Papen (1988, 1994, 1997) and Media Lengua, as described by Muysken (1994, 1997, 2000), Bilingual Navajo has the structure of an American Indian language with parts of its lexicon from a European language. “Bilingual mixed languages” are defined by Winford (2003) as languages created in a bilingual speech community with the grammar of one language and the lexicon of another. My intention is to place Bilingual Navajo into the historical and theoretical framework of the bilingual mixed language, and to explain how ii this language can be used in the Navajo speech community to help maintain the Navajo language.
    [Show full text]
  • Yiddish and Relation to the German Dialects Bryan Witmore University of South Carolina
    University of South Carolina Scholar Commons Theses and Dissertations 6-30-2016 Yiddish and Relation To The German Dialects Bryan Witmore University of South Carolina Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.sc.edu/etd Part of the German Language and Literature Commons Recommended Citation Witmore, B.(2016). Yiddish and Relation To The German Dialects. (Master's thesis). Retrieved from https://scholarcommons.sc.edu/ etd/3522 This Open Access Thesis is brought to you by Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. YIDDISH AND ITS RELATION TO THE GERMAN DIALECTS by Bryan Witmore Bachelor of Arts University of South Carolina, 2006 Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of Master of Arts in German College of Arts and Sciences University of South Carolina 2016 Accepted by: Kurt Goblirsch, Director of Thesis Lara Ducate, Reader Lacy Ford, Senior Vice Provost and Dean of Graduate Studies © Copyright by Bryan Witmore, 2016 All Rights Reserved. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis project was made possible in large part by the German program at the University of South Carolina. The technical assistance that propelled this project was contributed by the staff at the Ted Mimms Foreign Language Learning Center. My family was decisive in keeping me physically functional and emotionally buoyant through the writing process. Many thanks to you all. iii ABSTRACT In an attempt to balance the complex, multi-component nature of Yiddish with its more homogenous speech community – Ashekenazic Jews –Yiddishists have proposed definitions for the Yiddish language that cannot be considered linguistic in nature.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Variation and Change in Immigrant Speech : a Sociolinguistic Study of a 1967 Arab-Israeli War Immigrant Speech Community in Damascus, Syria
    Durham E-Theses Phonological variation and change in immigrant speech : A sociolinguistic study of a 1967 Arab-Israeli war immigrant speech community in Damascus, Syria. Jassem, Zaidan Ali How to cite: Jassem, Zaidan Ali (1987) Phonological variation and change in immigrant speech : A sociolinguistic study of a 1967 Arab-Israeli war immigrant speech community in Damascus, Syria., Durham theses, Durham University. Available at Durham E-Theses Online: http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/1682/ Use policy The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that: • a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in Durham E-Theses • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders. Please consult the full Durham E-Theses policy for further details. Academic Support Oce, Durham University, University Oce, Old Elvet, Durham DH1 3HP e-mail: [email protected] Tel: +44 0191 334 6107 http://etheses.dur.ac.uk 2 The copyright of this thesis rests with the author. No quotation from it should be published without his prior written consent and information derived from it should be acknowledged. PHONOLOGICAL VARIATION AND CHANGE IN IMMIGRANT SPEECH A SOCIOLINGUISTIC STUDY OF A 1967 ARAB-ISRAELI WAR IMMIGRANT SPEECH COMMUNITY IN DAMASCUS, SYRIA. by Zaidan Ali Jassem (B.
    [Show full text]
  • Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages
    Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages Gerrit J. Dimmendaal University of Cologne John Benjamins Publishing Company Amsterdam / Philadelphia UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs TM The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of American 8 National Standard for Information Sciences — Permanence of Paper for Printed Library Materials, ANSI Z39.48-1984. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Dimmendaal, Gerrit Jan. Historical linguistics and the comparative study of African languages / Gerrit J. Dimmendaal. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. African languages--Grammar, Comparative. 2. Historical linguistics. I. Title. PL8008.D56 2011 496--dc22 2011002759 isbn 978 90 272 1178 1 (Hb; alk. paper) isbn 978 90 272 1179 8 (Pb; alk. paper) isbn 978 90 272 8722 9 (Eb) © 2011 – John Benjamins B.V. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without written permission from the publisher. John Benjamins Publishing Company • P.O. Box 36224 • 1020 me Amsterdam • The Netherlands John Benjamins North America • P.O. Box 27519 • Philadelphia PA 19118-0519 • USA UNCORRECTED PROOFS © JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY 1st proofs Table of contents Preface ix Figures xiii Maps xv Tables
    [Show full text]
  • Contact-Induced Phonological Change in Yiddish
    <LINK "lou-r24"> <TARGET "lou" DOCINFO AUTHOR "Mark L. Louden" TITLE "Contact-induced phonological change in Yiddish" SUBJECT "Diachronica XVII:1" KEYWORDS "" SIZE HEIGHT "220" WIDTH "150" VOFFSET "4"> © Diachronica XVII:1.85–110 (2000). CONTACT-INDUCED PHONOLOGICAL CHANGE IN YIDDISH ANOTHER LOOK AT WEINREICH’S RIDDLES* Mark L. Louden University of Wisconsin-Madison Introduction With the publication in 1953 of his groundbreaking book, Languages in Contact, Uriel Weinreich significantly elevated the status of externally induced change within historical linguistics by rigorously adducing general predictive principles based on solid social, psychological, and linguistic data. Before 1953, contact linguistics had languished, in no small measure due to the priority given to system-internal factors in explanatory accounts of language change by the Young Grammarians and their European and American structuralist successors. As Weinreich and others pointed out, virtually all languages are affected by contact to some degree, meaning that externally induced change is far from exceptional. On the other hand, relative to system-internal change, change resulting from contact typically involves a greater number of variables, e.g. historical, cultural, psychological, a fact which poses a challenge for formulating accurate accounts of how specific changes no longer in progress may have proceeded. Precisely ten years after Languages in Contact, Weinreich took up one particular set of challenges emerging from the history of Yiddish which he collectively termed “four riddles in bilingual dialectology” (U. Weinreich 1963). At the time, Weinreich had begun working on a project dedicated to investigating the extent to which the history of Ashkenazic Jewish contact University of Wisconsin-Madison (wimadisonwi/1) IP: 128.104.46.196 On: Thu, 09 Jan 2020 02:12:43 with coterritorial non-Jewish populations in Eastern Europe might be reconstructed on the basis of (mainly) linguistic evidence.
    [Show full text]
  • English Syntactic Errors by Arabic Speaking Learners: Reviewed. PUB DATE 1996-01-00 NOTE 27P
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 423 660 FL 024 725 AUTHOR Noor, Hashim H. TITLE English Syntactic Errors by Arabic Speaking Learners: Reviewed. PUB DATE 1996-01-00 NOTE 27p. PUB TYPE Information Analyses (070) EDRS PRICE MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Adverbs; *Arabic; Conjunctions; Determiners (Languages); *English (Second Language); *Error Patterns; *Interference (Language); Language Patterns; Prepositions; Second Language Learning; Sentence Structure; *Syntax; Uncommonly Taught Languages; Verbs IDENTIFIERS Relative Clauses ABSTRACT Based on a review of research, the most common syntactic errors made by native Arabic-speaking learners of English as a second language are discussed. Seven categories of error are distinguished and described: verbal errors (use of tense, phase, aspect, voice, verb formation, concord, finite/non-finite verbs); relative clauses (interlingual and intralingual errors, structural misrepresentation, simplification); adverbial clauses (comparison, purpose, redalt, concession, manner); sentence structure; articles (definite and indefinite); prepositions (deletion, substitution, redundancy); and conj%:nctions. It is concluded that most common source of error is the influence of the native language, and that in processing English syntactic structures, Arabic speakers adopt certain strategies similar to those of first-language learners, including simplification and overgeneralization. Contains 52 references. (MSE) ******************************************************************************** Reproductions supplied by EDRS
    [Show full text]
  • Speakin' and Spokin' in Jamaica: Conflict and Consensus in Sociolinguistics
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by University of Essex Research Repository This paper appeared in Proceedings of the 19th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, February 12-15, 1993, eds. Joshua Guenter, Barbara Kaiser, Cheryl Zoll, pp 280-290. Speakin' and Spokin' in Jamaica: Conflict and Consensus in Sociolinguistics by Peter L. Patrick (Georgetown University) and Bonnie McElhinny (Stanford University) 1. Introduction: Language as Commodity Given the material and social rewards attached to speaking standard language, why do some speakers prefer non-standard varieties? One explanation sees the standard as a commodity (Gal 1989). Although the supply of symbolic resources is not limited in the same ways that material ones are-- one can easily imagine all speakers in a community using the standard-- access to standard language is controlled by institutions like schools (Bourdieu 1977), so that some speakers are not permitted to learn the standard even if they want to speak it. This control enhances the value of elite varieties, which become defined as standard. Since speakers may claim to value one variety yet behave as if they value another, responses vary when access to the standard is limited. Speakers may all agree that the standard is valuable and aspire to speak it-- this has come to be called the consensus model of a speech community. Conflict models, on the other hand, have been used to describe an array of cases in which non-elite speakers value their own variety more than the standard (Rickford 1986a, b). The choice between consensus and conflict models of the speech community reflects the analyst's orientation to different models of the social world.
    [Show full text]
  • An Investigation of English Spelling Problems of Arabic-Speaking Students
    Portland State University PDXScholar Dissertations and Theses Dissertations and Theses 1991 An investigation of English spelling problems of Arabic-speaking students Deborah Georgette Keim Portland State University Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds Part of the Applied Linguistics Commons, and the Bilingual, Multilingual, and Multicultural Education Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Keim, Deborah Georgette, "An investigation of English spelling problems of Arabic-speaking students" (1991). Dissertations and Theses. Paper 4172. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.6056 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. Please contact us if we can make this document more accessible: [email protected]. AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF Deborah Georgette Keim for the Master of Arts in Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages presented June 19, 1991. Title: An Investigation of English Spelling Problems of Arabic­ speaking Students. APPROVED BY THE MEMBERS OF THE THE.5IS COMMITTEE: Jeanette DeCarrico, Chair Marjorie Terdal Kimberley BroJn Ray Mariels In this two-part study, English spelling errors of Arabic­ speaking students are investigated. Specifically, an empirical study is done to document and investigate exactly what kinds of English 2 spelling errors Arabic-speaking students actually make. Then these data are analyzed. and spelling error patterns are discovered. Next. a study is done to determine if the presence of spelling errors in written work has a significant negative effect on readers' evaluations of this writing.
    [Show full text]
  • 7.8 Post-Yiddish Ashkenazic Speech This Spurred Conscious and Serious Attempts at Orthographic Reform (Schaechter 303 1999: 4)
    302 Sociolinguistics 7.8 Post-Yiddish Ashkenazic speech This spurred conscious and serious attempts at orthographic reform (Schaechter 303 1999: 4). The major breakthrough came in the lexicographic work of Lifshits their own (such as the newspaper Der Forverts, founded 1897) have recently switched to StY orthography. (1867, 1869, 1876). The impact of Lifshits was significant in setting the di­ rection of future orthographic reform. Among Lifshits' specific innovations. Schaechter (1999: 4) cites: the roje diacritic on IfI !l to distinguish it from Ip/e; 7.7.5 Romanization systems syllabic" I (vs. daytshmerish ~S) -ell, e.g., "O'il himl 'sky'; svarabhakti vowel, e.g.,I!l'JS)rt1s)i~:::l badeif-;}-nis 'need' (cf. StG 8ediirf-~-nis); suffixes lS), 1S)~ (vs. The orthography debates also included calls for romanization _ the writing daytshmerish spelling,., T~; cf. StG -ich, -lich); dropping ofsilent S) in, e.g., lib, of Yiddish in Latin letters (for internal use within the Yiddish speech commu­ fil (vs. daytshmerish :::ls)'~, ~S)'~ cf. StG Lieb, vie/); use of' to indicate palatal­ nity). Serious early calls for romanization came from prominent figures: Nathan ized consonants s)i~'" /I' ad,,1 'any,' Ql)i~'" /I'arnml 'noise'; word-final t:l (tes) Birnbaum (in his early years), the convener of the 1908 TShernovits conference; replacing daytshmerish it thus: t:l~il 'hand; arm,' t:lJ'" 'wind,' t:l~s)J 'money' (cf. Ludwig Zamenhof; Zaretski; Zhitlovski; Yofe; and others (Schaechter 1999: StG Hand, Wind, Geld). 16). The earliest such voice was likely Dr. Philip Mansch (1888-1890). Over­ In general, the history of StY orthography involves an ongoing process to whelmingly, however, there was a strong negative reaction to romanization, free itself from daytshmerish orthography.
    [Show full text]