Norepinephrine Ignites Local Hotspots of Neuronal Excitation: How Arousal Amplifies Selectivity in Perception and Memory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Norepinephrine Ignites Local Hotspots of Neuronal Excitation: How Arousal Amplifies Selectivity in Perception and Memory BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2016), Page 1 of 75 doi:10.1017/S0140525X15000667, e200 Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory Mara Mather Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-0191 [email protected] David Clewett Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-2520 [email protected] Michiko Sakaki School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading RGX 7BE, United Kingdom; and Kochi University of Technology, Kami, 782-0003, Japan [email protected] Carolyn W. Harley Professor Emeritus, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, NL A1C 5S7, Canada [email protected] Abstract: Emotional arousal enhances perception and memory of high-priority information but impairs processing of other information. Here, we propose that, under arousal, local glutamate levels signal the current strength of a representation and interact with norepinephrine (NE) to enhance high priority representations and out-compete or suppress lower priority representations. In our "glutamate amplifies noradrenergic effects" (GANE) model, high glutamate at the site of prioritized representations increases local NE release from the locus coeruleus (LC) to generate “NE hotspots.” At these NE hotspots, local glutamate and NE release are mutually enhancing and amplify activation of prioritized representations. In contrast, arousal-induced LC activity inhibits less active representations via two mechanisms: 1) Where there are hotspots, lateral inhibition is amplified; 2) Where no hotspots emerge, NE levels are only high enough to activate low-threshold inhibitory adrenoreceptors. Thus, LC activation promotes a few hotspots of excitation in the context of widespread suppression, enhancing high priority representations while suppressing the rest. Hotspots also help synchronize oscillations across neural ensembles transmitting high-priority information. Furthermore, brain structures that detect stimulus priority interact with phasic NE release to preferentially route such information through large-scale functional brain networks. A surge of NE before, during, or after encoding enhances synaptic plasticity at NE hotspots, triggering local protein synthesis processes that enhance selective memory consolidation. Together, these noradrenergic mechanisms promote selective attention and memory under arousal. GANE not only reconciles apparently contradictory findings in the emotion-cognition literature but also extends previous influential theories of LC neuromodulation by proposing specific mechanisms for how LC-NE activity increases neural gain. Keywords: arousal; attention; emotion; locus coeruleus; long-term consolidation; memory; norepinephrine; perception 1. Introduction information and less on peripheral information (Easterbrook 1959). Since his seminal paper, researchers have accumulated When jolted by a rough skydiving landing, psychologist James many more observations that arousal evoked by emotional Easterbrook observed that his sense of space and time shrank events enhances some aspects of perception and memory and slowly re-expanded (Easterbrook 1982). This sparked his but impairs others (for reviews, see Mather & Sutherland curiosity about how arousal influences attention. Later he 2011; Reisberg & Heuer 2004). For example, victims of a published a review article in which he argued that under crime tend to remember the weapon vividly but forget the arousal, people rely more on central or immediately relevant perpetrator’s face (Steblay 1992). People also pay attention © Cambridge University Press 2016 0140-525X/16 Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Graduate Institute, on 30 May 2018 at 11:35:20, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms1 . https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15001879 Mather et al.: Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation to emotional information at the expense of neutral informa- Bottom-up processes are largely automatic, determined tion (Dolcos & McCarthy 2006; Knight et al. 2007). These by the perceptual properties of a stimulus, and do not examples fit with Easterbrook’s formulation that arousal depend on top-down attention or task demands. For impairs attention to peripheral information. But arousing example, stimuli that contrast with their surroundings, stimuli can sometimes enhance memory of peripheral such as a bright light in a dark room, engage attention auto- neutral information (Kensinger et al. 2007; Knight & matically even if they are currently goal irrelevant (Itti & Mather 2009). Thus, although it is clear that arousal Koch 2000; Parkhurst et al. 2002; Reynolds & Desimone shapes attention and memory, knowing that something is 2003). Top-down goals can also bias competition in favor neutral or spatially peripheral is not enough to predict of particular stimuli that otherwise would not stand out. how it will fare under emotional conditions. Although not included in the original biased competition So, then, how does arousal influence the brain’s selection models, past history with particular stimuli is also a of features to highlight versus suppress? An initial answer to source of selection bias (Awh et al. 2012; Hutchinson & this puzzle was provided by the arousal-biased competi- Turk-Browne 2012). For example, one’s name or a novel tion (ABC) model, which posits that arousal does not have stimulus tends to engage attention (Moray 1959; Reicher fixed rules about which types of stimuli to enhance or et al. 1976). In addition, faces, text, and emotionally suppress. Instead, arousal amplifies the stakes of ongoing salient stimuli all grab attention (e.g., Cerf et al. 2009; selection processes, leading to “winner-take-more” and Knight et al. 2007; MacKay et al. 2004; Niu et al. 2012). “loser-take-less” effects in perception and memory (Mather A core aspect of most current theories of visual attention is & Sutherland 2011). The ABC model builds on biased com- that these different signals are integrated into maps of the petition models proposing that stimuli must compete for environment that indicate the priority or salience of stimuli limited mental resources (Beck & Kastner 2009; Desimone across different locations (Itti & Koch 2000; Soltani & & Duncan 1995; Duncan 2006). As conceptualized by Desi- Koch 2010;Treisman1998). Regions in frontoparietal mone and Duncan (1995), both bottom-up and top-down cortex integrating sensory and top-down signals help repre- neural mechanisms help resolve competition. sent such priority maps (Ptak 2012). Moreover, having both feedforward and feedback connections between sensory regions and cortical priority maps enables distributed repre- sentations of prioritized information to modulate their own DAVID CLEWETT, who received his Ph.D. in Neurosci- processing (e.g., lower-level visual features) even further ence, has been the recipient of two Endowed Fellow- (Klink et al. 2014;Ptak2012; Serences & Yantis 2007; ships from the University of Southern California. His Soltani & Koch 2010). Thus, priority signals are self-biasing research focuses on the role of the noradrenergic fi system in mediating the influence of arousal on to enhance ef cient information processing in the brain. memory and healthy cognitive aging. In 2014, he was In the ABC model, arousal further biases mental process- recognized as the program’s Neuroscience Student of ing to favor high- over low-priority representations, regard- the Year. less of whether initial priority is determined by bottom-up salience, emotional salience, or top-down goals. Thus, CAROLYN HARLEY, Professor Emeritus in Psychology because spatially peripheral information is usually lower pri- and Neuroscience at Memorial University of New- ority than central information, arousal usually impairs foundland, is the author of more than 95 publications memory for it (Steblay 1992; Waring & Kensinger 2011). in learning and memory and brain plasticity with a Yet, when peripheral information is perceptually salient special focus on norepinephrine and the locus coeru- or goal relevant, arousal instead enhances memory for it leus. She has served as President of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behavior and Cognitive Science. (e.g., Kensinger et al. 2007, Experiment 4). But the ABC Her ongoing research program is concerned with model does not tackle how this works in the brain. Previous behavioral functions of noradrenergic locus coeruleus brain-based models of emotion and cognition also do not activation and is funded by the Natural Sciences and account for the dual role of arousal. Most models posit Engineering Research Council of Canada. that the amygdala enhances perception and memory consolidation of emotionally salient stimuli, but fail to MARA MATHER, Professor of Gerontology and Psychol- address how arousal sometimes enhances and sometimes ogy at the University of Southern California, conducts – impairs information processing. research focusing on emotion cognition interactions In this article we propose the glutamate amplifies nor- with a particular focus on how emotional arousal adrenergic effects (GANE) model, in which arousal shapes perception and memory. She has received the fi Distinguished Scientific Award for Early Career Contri- ampli es the activation difference between high- and low-priority representations via local synaptic self-regulation bution to Psychology from the
Recommended publications
  • The Brain Is Contained Within the Cranium, and Constitutes the Upper, Greatly Expanded Part of the Central Nervous System
    The brain is contained within the cranium, and constitutes the upper, greatly expanded part of the central nervous system. Henry Gray (1918) Looking through the gray outer layer of the cortex, you can see a mass of white matter. At the center is a cluster of large nuclei, including the basal ganglia, the hippocampi, the amygdalae, and two egg-shaped structures at the very center, barely visible in this figure, the thalami. The thalami rest on the lower brainstem (dark and light blue). You can also see the pituitary gland in front (beige), and the cerebellum at the rear of the brain (pink). In this chapter we will take these structures apart and re-build them from the bottom up. 09_P375070_Ch05.indd 126 1/29/2010 4:08:25 AM CHAPTER 5 The brain OUTLINE 1.0 Introduction 127 3.2 Output and input: the front-back division 143 1.1 The nervous system 128 3.3 The major lobes: visible and hidden 145 1.2 The geography of the brain 129 3.4 The massive interconnectivity of the cortex and thalamus 149 2.0 Growing a brain from the bottom up 133 3.5 The satellites of the subcortex 151 2.1 Evolution and personal history are expressed in the brain 133 4.0 Summary 153 2.2 Building a brain from bottom to top 134 5.0 Chapter review 153 3.0 From ‘ where ’ to ‘ what ’ : the functional 5.1 Study questions 153 roles of brain regions 136 5.2 Drawing exercises 153 3.1 The cerebral hemispheres: the left-right division 136 1.0 INTRODUCTION found.
    [Show full text]
  • Reading Speech from Still and Moving Faces: the Neural Substrates of Visible Speech
    Reading Speech from Still and Moving Faces: The Neural Substrates of Visible Speech Gemma A. Calvert1 and Ruth Campbell2 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/15/1/57/1757731/089892903321107828.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract & Speech is perceived both by ear and by eye. Unlike heard regions including the left inferior frontal (Broca’s) area, left speech, some seen speech gestures can be captured in superior temporal sulcus (STS), and left supramarginal gyrus stilled image sequences. Previous studies have shown that in (the dorsal aspect of Wernicke’s area). Stilled speech hearing people, natural time-varying silent seen speech can sequences also generated activation in the ventral premotor access the auditory cortex (left superior temporal regions). cortex and anterior inferior parietal sulcus bilaterally. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), the Moving faces generated significantly greater cortical activa- present study explored the extent to which this circuitry was tion than stilled face sequences, and in similar regions. activated when seen speech was deprived of its time-varying However, a number of differences between stilled and moving characteristics. speech were also observed. In the visual cortex, stilled faces In the scanner, hearing participants were instructed to generated relatively more activation in primary visual regions look for a prespecified visible speech target sequence (‘‘voo’’ (V1/V2), while visual movement areas (V5/MT+) were or ‘‘ahv’’) among other monosyllables. In one condition, the activated to a greater extent by moving faces. Cortical regions image sequence comprised a series of stilled key frames activated more by naturally moving speaking faces included showing apical gestures (e.g., separate frames for ‘‘v’’ and the auditory cortex (Brodmann’s Areas 41/42; lateral parts of ‘‘oo’’ [from the target] or ‘‘ee’’ and ‘‘m’’ [i.e., from Heschl’s gyrus) and the left STS and inferior frontal gyrus.
    [Show full text]
  • Are the Neural Correlates of Consciousness in the Front Or in the Back of the Cerebral Cortex?
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/118273; this version posted March 19, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Are the neural correlates of consciousness in the front or in the back of the cerebral cortex? Clinical and neuroimaging evidence Melanie Boly1,2*, Marcello Massimini3,4, Naotsugu Tsuchiya5, Bradley R. Postle2,6, Christof Koch7, Giulio Tononi2* 1 Department of Neurology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, 53705 USA 2 Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, 53719 USA 3 Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ‘Luigi Sacco’, University of Milan, Milan, 20157, Italy 4 Instituto Di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico, Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Milan, 20148, Italy 6 Department of Psychology, Monash University, Melbourne, 3168, Australia 6 Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, 53705 USA 7 Allen Institute for Brain Science, Seattle, WA, 98109 USA Abbreviated title: Is consciousness in the front versus the back of corteX? Correspondence to: Melanie Boly: [email protected] or Giulio Tononi: [email protected]. Address: 6001 Research Park Boulevard, Madison WI 53719 Number of pages: 25 Number of figures: 5 Number of words for Abstract: 45; number of words for Main teXt: 3750 Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Acknowledgments: This work was supported by NIH/NINDS 1R03NS096379 (to M.B.), by the Tiny Blue Dot Foundation and the Distinguished Chair in Consciousness Science (University of Wisconsin) (to G.T.), and by the James S.
    [Show full text]
  • Interaction of Inferior Temporal Cortex with Frontal Cortex and Basal Forebrain: Double Dissociation in Strategy Implementation and Associative Learning
    The Journal of Neuroscience, August 15, 2002, 22(16):7288–7296 Interaction of Inferior Temporal Cortex with Frontal Cortex and Basal Forebrain: Double Dissociation in Strategy Implementation and Associative Learning David Gaffan,1 Alexander Easton,2 and Amanda Parker2 1Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, Oxford OX1 3UD, United Kingdom, and 2School of Psychology, Nottingham University, Nottingham NG7 2RD, United Kingdom Macaque monkeys learned a strategy task in which two groups TSϩAMϩFX cannot be generally attributed to the partial tem- of visual objects needed to be treated differently, one with poral–frontal disconnection that this lesion creates, and there- persistent and one with sporadic object choices, to obtain food fore support the hypothesis that the amnesic effects of this rewards. After preoperative training, they were divided into two lesion are caused primarily by the disconnection of temporal surgical groups of three monkeys each. One group received cortex from ascending inputs from the basal forebrain. The crossed unilateral removals of frontal cortex and inferior tem- results also show that temporal–frontal interaction in strategy poral cortex (IT ϫ FC) and were severely impaired in performing implementation does not require those routes of temporal– the strategy task. The other group received bilateral transection frontal interaction that are interrupted in TSϩAMϩFX, and of anterior temporal stem, amygdala, and fornix (TSϩAMϩFX) therefore support the hypothesis that projections to other pos- and were unimpaired in performing the strategy task. Subse- terior cortical areas allow temporal and frontal cortex to interact quently the same animals were tested in visual object–reward with each other by multisynaptic corticocortical routes in strat- association learning.
    [Show full text]
  • Experience Sampling During Fmri Reveals Default Network and Executive System Contributions to Mind Wandering
    Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering Kalina Christoffa,1, Alan M. Gordonb, Jonathan Smallwoodc, Rachelle Smitha, and Jonathan W. Schoolerc aDepartment of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6Z 2R8; bDepartment of Psychology, Stanford University, CA 94305; and cDepartment of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106 Edited by Michael I. Posner, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, and approved March 27, 2009 (received for review February 9, 2009) Although mind wandering occupies a large proportion of our there were no online measures of mind wandering taken during waking life, its neural basis and relation to ongoing behavior scanning, it is possible that the observed default network recruit- remain controversial. We report an fMRI study that used experi- ment could be due to factors other than mind wandering. In this ence sampling to provide an online measure of mind wandering vein, Gilbert et al. (9) recently argued that instead of mind during a concurrent task. Analyses focused on the interval of time wandering, activations in the medial PFC part of the default immediately preceding experience sampling probes demonstrate network may reflect stimulus-related thought such as enhanced activation of default network regions during mind wandering, a watchfulness toward the external environment that is also likely to finding consistent with theoretical accounts of default network occur during highly practiced tasks. functions. Activation in medial prefrontal default network regions A key objective of the present study was to provide a direct was observed both in association with subjective self-reports of empirical test for the hypothesis that default network recruitment, mind wandering and an independent behavioral measure (perfor- including medial PFC regions, occurs during the precise moments mance errors on the concurrent task).
    [Show full text]
  • Posterior Cortex Epilepsy: Diagnostic Considerations and Surgical Outcome
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector Seizure 18 (2009) 288–292 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Seizure journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yseiz Posterior cortex epilepsy: Diagnostic considerations and surgical outcome Tao Yu, Yuping Wang, Guojun Zhang, Lixin Cai, Wei Du, Yongjie Li * Beijing Institute of Functional Neurosurgery, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, China ARTICLE INFO SUMMARY Article history: Purpose: To assess the role of various pre-surgical evaluations in posterior cortex epilepsy (PCE) and its Received 15 January 2008 surgical outcome. Received in revised form 12 November 2008 Methods: A retrospective analysis of clinical and laboratory data from 43 PCE patients referred for Accepted 20 November 2008 surgery was performed. The diagnosis was established by standard pre-surgical evaluation modalities including semiology, MRI, interictal and ictal scalp video-EEG as well as additional intracranial EEG Keywords: monitoring in selected cases. Posterior cortex Results: The 43 patients included 11 parietal lobe epilepsies, 13 occipital lobe epilepsies, and 19 patients Epilepsy surgery with seizures originating from parieto-occipito-posterior temporal cortex. Thirty-three (76.7%) patients Surgical outcome experienced at least one type of aura. Seventeen patients showed complex focal seizures, which were followed by secondarily generalized tonic–clonic seizures in seven of them; eighteen patients predominantly showed simple motor seizures (clonic seizures in 6, tonic seizures in 7, and versive seizures in 5). Long-term visual field deficits were observed in 8 patients. Other transient neurological deficits occurred in 7 patients. All patients received the follow-up study lasting 1–5 years, and achieved Engel’s I in 26 cases, II in 5, III in 5, and IV–V in 7.
    [Show full text]
  • Cortical Connectivity Maps Reveal Anatomically Distinct Areas in the Parietal Cortex of the Rat
    UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works Title Cortical connectivity maps reveal anatomically distinct areas in the parietal cortex of the rat. Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/01d2v835 Journal Frontiers in neural circuits, 8(JAN) ISSN 1662-5110 Authors Wilber, Aaron A Clark, Benjamin J Demecha, Alexis J et al. Publication Date 2014 DOI 10.3389/fncir.2014.00146 License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ 4.0 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 05 January 2015 NEURAL CIRCUITS doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00146 Cortical connectivity maps reveal anatomically distinct areas in the parietal cortex of the rat Aaron A. Wilber 1,2*†, Benjamin J. Clark 1,3*†, Alexis J. Demecha 1, Lilia Mesina 1, Jessica M. Vos 1 and Bruce L. McNaughton 1,2 1 Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, The University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, Canada 2 Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA 3 Department of Psychology, The University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA Edited by: A central feature of theories of spatial navigation involves the representation of spatial Charles F.Stevens, The Salk relationships between objects in complex environments. The parietal cortex has long Institute for Biological Studies, USA been linked to the processing of spatial visual information and recent evidence from Reviewed by: single unit recording in rodents suggests a role for this region in encoding egocentric Richard Hahnloser, ETH University Zurich, Switzerland and world-centered frames. The rat parietal cortex can be subdivided into four distinct Ya-tang Li, California Institute of rostral-caudal and medial-lateral regions, which includes a zone previously characterized as Technology, USA secondary visual cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • REVIEW ARTICLE the Retrosplenial-Parietal Network and Reference Frame Coordination for Spatial Navigation
    REVIEW ARTICLE The Retrosplenial-Parietal Network and Reference Frame Coordination for Spatial Navigation Benjamin J. Clark1, Christine M. Simmons2, Laura E. Berkowitz1, Aaron A. Wilber2 1Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico 2Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida Main text: 35 Pages, 2 figures Key Words: spatial navigation, reference frame, parietal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, retrosplenial cortex To whom correspondence should be addressed: [email protected] [email protected] Acknowledgments: The manuscript was written while the authors were supported by grants from NIAAA P50AA022534 and R21AA024983 to BJC and NIA R00AG049090 to AAW. 2 Abstract The retrosplenial cortex is anatomically positioned to integrate sensory, motor, and visual information and is thought to have an important role in processing spatial information and guiding behavior through complex environments. Anatomical and theoretical work has argued that the retrosplenial cortex participates in spatial behavior in concert with its primary input, the parietal cortex. Although the nature of this interactions is unknown, the central position is that the functional connectivity is hierarchical with egocentric spatial information processed at parietal cortex, and higher-level allocentric mappings generated in the retrosplenial cortex. Here, we review the evidence supporting this proposal. We begin by summarizing the key anatomical features of the retrosplenial-parietal network, and then review studies investigating the neural correlates of these regions during spatial behavior. Our summary of this literature suggests that the retrosplenial-parietal circuitry does not represent a strict hierarchical parcellation of function between the two regions, but instead a heterogeneous mixture of egocentric-allocentric coding and integration across frames of reference.
    [Show full text]
  • Interaction Between Auditory and Motor Systems in Speech Perception
    Neurosci Bull June 1, 2014, 30(3): 490–496. http://www.neurosci.cn 490 DOI: 10.1007/s12264-013-1428-6 ·Review· Interaction between auditory and motor systems in speech perception Zhe-Meng Wu, Ming-Li Chen, Xi-Hong Wu, Liang Li Department of Psychology, Speech and Hearing Research Center, Key Laboratory of Machine Perception (Ministry of Education), PKU-IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Peking University, Beijing 100871, China Corresponding author: Liang Li. E-mail: [email protected] © Shanghai Institutes for Biological Sciences, CAS and Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014 Based on the Motor Theory of speech perception, the interaction between the auditory and motor systems plays an essential role in speech perception. Since the Motor Theory was proposed, it has received remarkable attention in the fi eld. However, each of the three hypotheses of the theory still needs further verifi cation. In this review, we focus on how the auditory-motor anatomical and functional associations play a role in speech perception and discuss why previous studies could not reach an agreement and particularly whether the motor system involvement in speech perception is task-load dependent. Finally, we suggest that the function of the auditory-motor link is particularly useful for speech perception under adverse listening conditions and the further revised Motor Theory is a potential solution to the “cocktail-party” problem. Keywords: auditory-motor interaction; Motor Theory of speech perception; motor cortex; “cocktail-party” problem. Introduction in a reading-machine study. In their experiments, although blind people could recognize independent linguistic How listeners process the acoustic signals of speech is a units, they could not perceive alphabetic sequences and hot question.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Arousal Amplifies the Effects of Biased Competition in the Brain
    Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain Article Accepted Version Lee, T.-H., Sakaki, M., Cheng, R., Velasco, R. and Mather, M. (2014) Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9 (12). pp. 2067-2077. ISSN 1749-5024 doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu015 Available at http://centaur.reading.ac.uk/36912/ It is advisable to refer to the publisher’s version if you intend to cite from the work. See Guidance on citing . Published version at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu015 To link to this article DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu015 Publisher: Oxford University Press All outputs in CentAUR are protected by Intellectual Property Rights law, including copyright law. Copyright and IPR is retained by the creators or other copyright holders. Terms and conditions for use of this material are defined in the End User Agreement . www.reading.ac.uk/centaur CentAUR Central Archive at the University of Reading Reading’s research outputs online 1 Running title: Arousal-Biased Competition in Visual Perception Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain Tae-Ho Lee 1, Michiko Sakaki 2, Ruth Cheng 2, Ricardo Velasco 2 and Mara Mather 1,2,3 1Department of Psychology, University of Southern California 2Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California 3Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Southern California This work was supported by RO1AG025340. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mara Mather, Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, 3715 McClintock Avenue, Los Angeles, CA 90089-0191, U.S.A., email: [email protected] 2 Summary The arousal-biased competition (ABC) model predicts that arousal increases the gain on neural competition between stimuli representations.
    [Show full text]
  • 2019 Frontiers Neural Circuits
    Two Neural Circuits to Point Towards Home Position After Passive Body Displacements Jean Blouin, Anahid Saradjian, Jean-Philippe Pialasse, Gerome Manson, Laurence Mouchnino, Martin Simoneau To cite this version: Jean Blouin, Anahid Saradjian, Jean-Philippe Pialasse, Gerome Manson, Laurence Mouchnino, et al.. Two Neural Circuits to Point Towards Home Position After Passive Body Displacements. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, Frontiers, 2019, 13, 10.3389/fncir.2019.00070. hal-02338493 HAL Id: hal-02338493 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02338493 Submitted on 30 Oct 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 30 October 2019 doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00070 Two Neural Circuits to Point Towards Home Position After Passive Body Displacements Jean Blouin 1*, Anahid H. Saradjian 1, Jean-Philippe Pialasse 2, Gerome A. Manson 1,3, Laurence Mouchnino 1 and Martin Simoneau 2,4 1Aix-Marseille Univ, CNRS, Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, Marseille, France, 2Faculté de Médecine, Département de Kinésiologie, Université Laval, Québec, QC, Canada, 3Centre for Motor Control, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada, 4Centre Interdisciplinaire de Recherche en Réadaptation et Intégration Sociale (CIRRIS), Québec, QC, Canada A challenge in motor control research is to understand the mechanisms underlying the transformation of sensory information into arm motor commands.
    [Show full text]
  • Association of Atrophy of the Medial Temporal Lobe with Reduced Blood
    190 Journal ofNeurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1992;55:190-194 Association of atrophy of the medial temporal lobe J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.190 on 1 March 1992. Downloaded from with reduced blood flow in the posterior parietotemporal cortex in patients with a clinical and pathological diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease K A Jobst, A D Smith, C S Barker, A Wear, E M King, A Smith, P A Anslow, A J Molyneux, B J Shepstone, N Soper, K A Holmes, J R Robinson, R A Hope, C Oppenheimer, K Brockbank, B McDonald Abstract lateral temporal lobes are related to the degree A combination of medial temporal lobe of cognitive decline,"8 but such findings are atrophy, shown by computed tomo- difficult to reconcile with neuropathological graphy, and reduced blood flow in the studies showing that the most severely affected parietotemporal cortex, shown by single parts of the brain in Alzheimer's disease are in photon emission tomography, was found the medial lobe, mainly the amygdala, the in 86% (44151) of patients with a clinical hippocampal formation and adjacent parahip- diagnosis of senile dementia of the Alz- pocampal gyrus.>'3 Atrophy of these areas can heimer type (SDAT). The same combina- be revealed in life by CT studies in which the tion ofchanges was found in four out of 10 scan angle is adjusted to give a clearer view of patients with other clinical types of the medial temporal lobe, and such studies in dementia and in two out of 18 with no patients with a clinical diagnosis ofAlzheimer's evidence of cognitive deficit.
    [Show full text]