Current Biology 070819.Pdf

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Current Biology 070819.Pdf Current Biology Magazine Correspondence interested in his diversity of movements One important difference between to music. This was because his owner Snowball’s dancing and human dancing Spontaneity (author I.S.) observed that Snowball is that Snowball danced in short was using new movements to music episodes rather than continuously and diversity of not present in our original study, and (mean duration = 3.69 seconds, std = appeared to be in a period of ‘movement 2.72 seconds; N = 141), consistent with movement to music exploration’. Importantly, this was not earlier reports [2]. Informal observations are not uniquely due to modeling by I.S., who does by author I.S. suggest that Snowball not make a wide range of movements moves more continuously when a human when dancing with Snowball and human dances with him, which we plan tends only to engage in head bobbing to examine in future work. and hand waving. Also, Snowball was To determine if certain moments in R. Joanne Jao Keehn1, John R. Iversen2, never explicitly trained to make specifi c each song triggered specifi c dance Irena Schulz3, and Aniruddh D. Patel4,5,6,* movements to music (for example, movements, time segments during via operant conditioning with food which Snowball danced in all three Spontaneous movement to music rewards, as in [7]). During this period of trials were fi rst identifi ed (Figure S1B). occurs in every human culture and is a exploration, Snowball seemed to favor We then examined each such segment foundation of dance [1]. This response movement diversity over synchronization to see if he used the same dance to music is absent in most species accuracy: his rhythmic movements often movement in all three trials. In both (including monkeys), yet it occurs in seemed not highly synchronized to the songs, there was not a single segment parrots, perhaps because they (like beat, possibly because he was primarily in which all three trials contained the humans, and unlike monkeys) are vocal exploring new movements rather than same dance move. Thus, in addition to learners whose brains contain strong exploiting old ones. the diversity of his movement to music, auditory–motor connections, conferring To quantify Snowball’s movement another sign of Snowball’s fl exibility in sophisticated audiomotor processing diversity we fi lmed him moving to two moving to music is that his movements abilities [2,3]. Previous research has pop songs: “Another One Bites the are not constrained by certain audio shown that parrots can bob their Dust” and “Girls Just Wanna Have features of the music. We also found heads or lift their feet in synchrony Fun”, each presented three times (23 that Snowball used different proportions with a musical beat [2,3], but humans minutes of music in total). Filming took of dance moves in the two songs (Figure move to music using a wide variety of place in September 2008. Snowball S2), though more data would be needed movements and body parts. Is this also was 12 years old and had not danced to determine whether this is a stable true of parrots? If so, it would constrain to these songs with anyone other than difference. theories of how movement to music is his owner. During fi lming, the owner was Snowball is not unique: other controlled by parrot brains. Specifi cally, in the room and gave occasional verbal examples of diversity in parrot as head bobbing is part of parrot encouragement (such as “good boy!”), movement to music can be found on courtship displays [4] and foot lifting but did not dance or move rhythmically. the internet (see links in Supplemental is part of locomotion, these may be Movement coding was performed using Information). A key question, however, is innate movements controlled by central frame-by-frame analysis with the audio how such moves are acquired. Parrots pattern generators which become muted and was conducted by author can imitate movements [9], and if their entrained by auditory rhythms, without R.J.J.K., a cognitive neuroscientist and movements to music are due to imitation the involvement of complex motor classically and contemporarily trained (for example, from seeing humans planning. This would be unlike humans, dancer. The temporal onset and offset dance), it would suggest that parrots where movement to music engages of each movement or sequence of can solve the ‘correspondence problem’ cortical networks including frontal and repeated movements were recorded, [1] in a remarkably sophisticated way parietal areas [5]. Rich diversity in parrot focusing on ‘dance movements’, (watching an individual with a very movement to music would suggest a defi ned (following [8]) as movements different body morphology perform strong contribution of forebrain regions that are clearly intentional but which are a motor pattern, then mapping that to this behavior, perhaps including not an effi cient means of achieving any pattern across modalities onto one’s motor learning regions abutting the plausible external goal, such as basic own motor system, without direct complex vocal-learning ‘shell’ regions locomotion. Our analyses revealed that reinforcement). Another possibility is that that are unique to parrots among vocal Snowball had a diverse repertoire of 14 some moves may refl ect creativity. This learning birds [6]. Here we report that dance movements and two composite would also be remarkable, as creativity a sulphur-crested cockatoo (Cacatua movements (Table 1). An excerpt in nonhuman animals has typically been galerita eleonora) responds to music from one trial annotated with dance documented in behaviors aimed at with remarkably diverse spontaneous movements is provided in Supplemental obtaining an immediate physical benefi t, movements employing a variety of body Video S1 and Figure S1A. A compilation such as access to food or mating parts, and suggest why parrots share of the 14 dance movements is shown opportunities. Snowball does not dance this response with humans. in Supplemental Video S2 (all six videos for food or in order to mate; instead, his Soon after our original study [2] of from the study, annotated by R.J.J.K., dancing appears to be a social behavior synchronization to a musical beat in this are available via a link in Supplemental used to interact with human caregivers parrot (named ‘Snowball’), we became Information.) (his surrogate fl ock). Current Biology 29, R603–R622, July 8, 2019 © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. R621 Current Biology Magazine Table 1. Dance movements and descriptions. Movementa Description ‘Girls’ ‘Another’ Body Roll (B) Wave passes through head, then body x x Counter-Clockwise Circle (CCW) Head moves in a circular trajectory in counter-clockwise direction x Downward (D) Head bobs up and down xx Head shakes while bobbing down, 3 frames of shaking at lowest Down-Shake (DS) x dip Foot-Lift (F) Foot lifts, body remains stationary x x Foot-Lift Down Swing (FL) Foot lifts while head swings diagonally downward xx Headbang (H) Head is thrown forward and backward, sometimes in pattern-8 x Head-Foot Sync (HF) Head moves in sync with foot xx Headbang w/ Lifted Foot (HL) Foot lifts while head bangs xx Pose (P) Body poses/holds a stationary position x x Side-to-Side (S) Foot lifts while head moves from side-to-side with rebound in neck x x Both feet remain close to or in contact w/ surface; head follows a Semi-Circle Low (SCL) x semi-circle (lower half) trajectory Both feet remain close to or in contact w/ surface; head follows Semi-Circle High (SCH) xx a semi-circle (upper half) trajectory Vogue (V) Head moves from one side of lifted foot to the other xx Head bobs up and down interspersed with head moving in sync with Downward/Head-Foot Sync (D/HF) x foot Headbang/Semi-Circle Low Inter- Head is thrown forward and backward interspersed with head x changed (H/SCL) following a semi-circle (lower half) trajectory aPredominant movements are in bold text. These are movements with durations comprising at least 10% of the overall dance movement duration for any given trial. In right two columns, x = occurred during that song. Building on ideas articulated in [1], The Neurosciences Institute (San Diego, CA). entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non We thank Irene Pepperberg and Erich Jarvis vocal mimic. J. Comp. Psychol. 127, 412–427. we suggest that spontaneous and 8. Schachner, A., and Carey, S. (2013). Reasoning diverse movement to music arises when for their input on issues of parrot behavior and about ‘irrational’ actions: When intentional fi ve traits converge: A) complex vocal neurobiology. movements cannot be explained, the movements themselves are seen as the goal. Cognition 129, learning, B) the capacity for nonverbal 309–327. movement imitation, C) a tendency to AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS 9. Heyes, C., and Saggerson, A. (2002). Testing for imitative and nonimitative social learning in the form long-term social bonds, D) the budgerigar using a two-object/two-action test. ability to learn complex sequences J.R.I. and A.D.P. designed the experiments, I.S. Anim. Behav. 64, 851–859. of actions, and E) attentiveness to conducted the experiments, R.J.J.K. analyzed 10. Auersperg, A.M., Kacelnik, A., and von Bayern, the data, R.J.J.K, J.R.I. and A.D.P. wrote the A.M. (2013). Explorative learning and functional communicative movements. Parrots are inferences on a fi ve-step means-means-end unusual in sharing all of these traits with paper. problem in Goffi n’s cockatoos (Cacatua goffi ni). humans [4,9,10], which could explain PLoS One 8, e68979. why (to date) only humans and parrots REFERENCES show spontaneous and diverse dancing 1Brain Development Imaging Labs, Department to music.
Recommended publications
  • ORNITHOLOGIST VOLUME 44 - PARTS 1&2 - November - 2019
    SOUTH AUSTRALIAN ORNITHOLOGIST VOLUME 44 - PARTS 1&2 - November - 2019 Journal of The South Australian Ornithological Association Inc. In this issue: Variation in songs of the White-eared Honeyeater Phenotypic diversity in the Copperback Quailthrush and a third subspecies Neonicotinoid insecticides Bird Report, 2011-2015: Part 1, Non-passerines President: John Gitsham The South Australian Vice-Presidents: Ornithological John Hatch, Jeff Groves Association Inc. Secretary: Kate Buckley (Birds SA) Treasurer: John Spiers FOUNDED 1899 Journal Editor: Merilyn Browne Birds SA is the trading name of The South Australian Ornithological Association Inc. Editorial Board: Merilyn Browne, Graham Carpenter, John Hatch The principal aims of the Association are to promote the study and conservation of Australian birds, to disseminate the results Manuscripts to: of research into all aspects of bird life, and [email protected] to encourage bird watching as a leisure activity. SAOA subscriptions (e-publications only): Single member $45 The South Australian Ornithologist is supplied to Family $55 all members and subscribers, and is published Student member twice a year. In addition, a quarterly Newsletter (full time Student) $10 reports on the activities of the Association, Add $20 to each subscription for printed announces its programs and includes items of copies of the Journal and The Birder (Birds SA general interest. newsletter) Journal only: Meetings are held at 7.45 pm on the last Australia $35 Friday of each month (except December when Overseas AU$35 there is no meeting) in the Charles Hawker Conference Centre, Waite Road, Urrbrae (near SAOA Memberships: the Hartley Road roundabout). Meetings SAOA c/o South Australian Museum, feature presentations on topics of ornithological North Terrace, Adelaide interest.
    [Show full text]
  • Cockatiels Free
    FREE COCKATIELS PDF Thomas Haupt,Julie Rach Mancini | 96 pages | 05 Aug 2008 | Barron's Educational Series Inc.,U.S. | 9780764138966 | English | Hauppauge, United States How to Take Care of a Cockatiel (with Pictures) - wikiHow A cockatiel is a popular choice for a pet bird. It is a small parrot with a variety of color patterns and a head crest. They are attractive as well as friendly. They are capable of mimicking speech, although they can be difficult to understand. These birds are good at whistling and you can teach them to sing along to tunes. Life Expectancy: 15 to 20 years with proper care, and sometimes as Cockatiels as 30 years though this is rare. In their native Australia, cockatiels are Cockatiels quarrions or weiros. They primarily live in the Cockatiels, a region of the northern part of the Cockatiels. Discovered inthey are the smallest members of the cockatoo family. They exhibit many of the Cockatiels features and habits as the larger Cockatiels. In the wild, they live in large flocks. Cockatiels became Cockatiels as pets during the s. They are easy to breed in captivity and their docile, friendly personalities make them a natural fit for Cockatiels life. These birds can Cockatiels longer be trapped and exported from Australia. These little birds are gentle, affectionate, and often like to be petted and held. Cockatiels are not necessarily fond of cuddling. They simply want to be near you and will be very happy to see you. Cockatiels are generally friendly; however, an untamed bird might nip. You can prevent bad Cockatiels at an early age Cockatiels ignoring bad behavior as these birds aim to please.
    [Show full text]
  • In Budgerigars
    帝京科学大学紀要 Vol.12(2016)pp.29-38 Chorus-like synchronized vocalizations(Big Chorus) in budgerigars 1 Hitomi ABE 1 Fujiro SAKURAI 1 Teikyo University of Science, Faculty of Life & Environmental Sciences-Department of Animal Science Abstract A number of recent studies have reported how budgerigars( Melopsittacus undulatus) are capable of mimicking various behaviors, and that individual vocalizations are strongly influenced by those of other conspecific birds in their vicinity. We examined this effect by placing four budgerigars in separate cages adjacent to each other, and then recording and analyzing all of their vocalizations over a two-week period. Time-based comparisons of the amount of time spent performing warble-songs over a 10 min period revealed a strong correlation between the duration of vocalization when all of the animals were males. However, no such correlation was observed when groups consisted of two males and two females. In the male-only group, whenever one male sang, the others would join in and singing would be synchronized. We repeatedly observed synchronized vocalizations where all of the birds sang together in what could be described as a chorus. While we were unable to demonstrate the functional significance of this behavior, since budgerigars are flocking birds, these synchronized vocalizations were not considered to function as territorial calls or for courtship. Keywords:Budgerigars, Vocal, Warble-song, Chorus-like synchronized, Introduction synchronizing their behaviors to rhythmic changes Budgerigars, the smallest parrots in the order in artificial light and auditory stimuli 7).Thus, Psittaciformes, live in large flocks in Australia while these birds can mimic and synchronize and are known to mimic the sound patterns of their behaviors, the manner in which they are conspecific individuals 1, 2).A recent study reported influenced by the vocalizations of their neighbors that when budgerigars were shown a video of has not yet been clarified.
    [Show full text]
  • Problem Sexual Behaviors of Companion Parrots
    21 Problem Sexual Behaviors of Companion Parrots Fern Van Sant INTRODUCTION Reproductive behaviors observed in the wild, Psittacine species have become popular as com- such as pair bonding, courtship regurgitation, panion animals because they demonstrate many cavity seeking, nest building, territorial aggres- kinds of social behavior that humans find famil- sion, and copulation, are often displayed in a iar and enjoyable. Such endearing traits as beauty, home setting, though with human “flocks.” Fe- intelligence, and mimicry have led humans to males of some species lay large numbers of eggs adopt them as members of their family. over extended periods of time to a point of com- Recognizing that some of these traits are innate plete physical collapse and failure. The reproduc- and genetically determined will allow for a new tive drive can also lead to behaviors that render and better understanding of companion psittacine the bird difficult or impossible to live with. These behavior and enable us to better predict responses behaviors often include incessant screaming, sud- of different species to the conditions of pet bird den aggression toward favored (or not favored) care. Developing an appreciation for how and humans, and destructive attempts to excavate why innate behaviors can be triggered by specific nests in closets, couches, and drawers. As many actions or conditions will hopefully lead to better species are represented in the pet trade, there ex- and healthier lives for captive psittacine birds. ists extreme variability as to when behaviors will Understanding innate behaviors of companion start, which will be displayed, and how long they psittacines requires an appreciation for the intri- may continue.[7] cate physiologic and hormonal events that adapt a In the wild, the expression of these largely re- species to an environmental niche.
    [Show full text]
  • Strengths and Vulnerabilities of Australian Networks for Conservation of Threatened Birds
    Strengths and vulnerabilities of Australian networks for conservation of threatened birds T IM Q. HOLMES,BRIAN W. HEAD,HUGH P. POSSINGHAM and S TEPHEN T. GARNETT Abstract We analysed the supportive social networks asso- making actors within an organizational framework trying ciated with the conservation of six threatened Australian to solve collective problems. The participants in an action bird taxa, in one of the first network analyses of threatened situation (Ostrom, ) for a threatened species may in- species conservation programmes. Each example showed clude those actively involved in the conservation of the contrasting vulnerabilities. The Alligator Rivers yellow bird or those with an interest in, or concern about, the chat Epthianura crocea tunneyi had the smallest social net- bird or a related issue. They may be individuals, or represen- work and no real action was supported. For the Capricorn tatives of NGOs, community groups, land owners, govern- yellow chat Epthianura crocea macgregori the network was ments or corporate actors, interacting for a range of reasons, centred on one knowledgeable and committed actor. The including obtaining or disseminating information, solving orange-bellied parrot Neophema chrysogaster had a strongly problems, negotiating, making decisions, collaborating connected recovery team but gaps in the overall network and seeking physical resources (Ostrom, ; Jepson could limit communication. The recovery teams for the et al., ). The key components of these action situations swift parrot Lathamus discolor and Baudin’s black-cockatoo include key participants, interorganizational collaborations Calyptorhynchus baudinii had strong links among most and social networks. Here, we focus on the social networks stakeholders but had weak ties to the timber industry and of key participants and seek to determine how such net- orchardists, respectively, limiting their capacity to manage works may influence the effective management of biodiver- threatening processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Conservation Implications of Illegal Bird Trade and Disease
    CONSERVATION IMPLICATIONS OF ILLEGAL BIRD TRADE AND DISEASE RISK IN PERU A Dissertation by ELIZABETH FRANCES DAUT Submitted to the Office of Graduate and Professional Studies of Texas A&M University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Chair of Committee, Donald J. Brightsmith Co-Chair of Committee, Markus J. Peterson Committee Members, Renata Ivanek-Miojevic Christian Brannstrom Head of Department, Roger Smith III May 2015 Major Subject: Veterinary Microbiology Copyright 2015 Elizabeth Frances Daut ABSTRACT Trade in wild-caught animals as pets is a global conservation and animal-welfare concern. Illegal and poorly-regulated legal wildlife trade can threaten biodiversity, spread infectious diseases, and result in considerable animal suffering and mortality. I used illegal wildlife trade in Peru, specifically native bird trade, as a case study to explore important aspects and consequences of the trade for domestic markets. With data collected from a five-year market survey and governmental seizure records, I applied a statistical modeling approach to investigate the influence of Peru’s legal export quota system on the country’s illegal domestic bird trade. I used an infectious-disease mathematical modeling approach to analyze how illegal harvest influenced disease dynamics in a wild parrot population. Finally, I used qualitative research methods to investigate the role of non-governmental organizations (NGOs) and their members’ philosophical perspectives toward wildlife in combating illegal trade. I found that Peru had a thriving illegal trade in native birds (mostly parrots) for domestic consumers; 150 species were recorded in markets and/or seizures with over 35,250 individuals offered for sale (2007–2011).
    [Show full text]
  • Biodiversity and Human Wellbeing Considerations in Managing the Urban Forest of a Global Biodiversity Hotspot
    Biodiversity and human wellbeing considerations in managing the urban forest of a global biodiversity hotspot Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub December 2020 About the Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub The Clean Air and Urban Landscapes (CAUL) Hub is funded by the Australian Government’s National Environmental Science Program. The remit of the CAUL Hub is to undertake “Research to support environmental quality in our urban areas”. This includes research on air quality, urban greening, liveability and biodiversity, with a focus on practical implementation of research findings, public engagement and participation by Indigenous Australians. The CAUL Hub is a consortium of four universities: The University of Melbourne, RMIT University, the University of Western Australia and the University of Wollongong. www.nespurban.edu.au Acknowledgements We acknowledge the Traditional Owners of the lands and waters where this research took place, The Whadjuk people of the Noongar nation, and pay our respects to Elders past, present and future. We acknowledge that all cities in Australia were built on Indigenous land, and that this land was never ceded. Please cite this document as Dickinson D. and Ramalho C.E. (2020) Biodiversity and human wellbeing considerations in managing the urban forest of a global biodiversity hotspot. Report prepared by the Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub. The Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub and Threatened Species Recovery Hub are funded by the Australian Government's National Environmental Science Program Contents Introduction
    [Show full text]
  • Zoos SA Annual Report 2019/2020
    Annual Report 2019-20 PASSION We inspire and influence by bringing pride, care and commitment to our work. Contents INNOVATION We seek creative ways to achieve goals and turn ideas into reality by promoting a culture of learning and improving. President and Chief Executive’s report ...................................... 2 Board members and governance ............................................... 4 RESPECT Zoos SA purpose and aspiration ............................................. 8 We respect individuals’ values and encourage Year in review: Key numbers ...............................................10 a culture of collaboration, diversity, listening and trust. Year in review: Finance snapshot .........................................12 Saving species inside our gates ............................................14 Leading the way in animal care and research ........................18 Conservation projects beyond our gates ...............................20 EFFECTIVENESS Connecting visitors with nature.........................................22 We are accountable, focus on clearly defined goals and work as a team to turn words Connecting children with nature ......................................24 into action. Educating the conservationists of tomorrow ........................26 Visitor experience and events ..........................................28 Connection to country and culture ..................................30 ENGAGEMENT Inspiring visitation and influencing change ......................32 We create experiences and connections
    [Show full text]
  • Experimental Evidence for Synchronization to a Musical Beat in a Nonhuman Animal
    Current Biology 19, 827–830, May 26, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038 Report Experimental Evidence for Synchronization to a Musical Beat in a Nonhuman Animal Aniruddh D. Patel,1,* John R. Iversen,1 Micah R. Bregman,1,2 of tempi, and is crossmodal, with sound eliciting periodic and Irena Schulz3 movement that is not necessarily aimed at sound production 1The Neurosciences Institute [11]. BPS is thus an unusual form of rhythmic entrainment 10640 John Jay Hopkins Drive and is the focus of a growing body of behavioral, neural, and San Diego, CA 92121 computational research [e.g., 12–16]. USA The spontaneous development and apparent species spec- 2Department of Cognitive Science ificity of BPS make it reminiscent of how aspects of language University of California, San Diego develop. Yet BPS does not appear to be a by-product of our 9500 Gilman Drive linguistic abilities: ordinary speech does not employ regularly La Jolla, CA 92093 timed beats or elicit periodic movements from listeners USA [3, 17]. Might BPS be a biological specialization for music, 3Bird Lovers Only Rescue Service reflecting natural selection for musical abilities in our species P.O. Box 552 [18, 19]? Or is it a consequence of brain circuitry that evolved Dyer, IN 46311 for other reasons [20]? According to the ‘‘vocal learning and USA rhythmic synchronization’’ hypothesis [3], BPS relies on the brain circuitry for complex vocal learning because (1) BPS resembles vocal learning in that it involves special links Summary between the auditory and motor systems [4, 14] and (2) the neural substrates of vocal learning and BPS appear to overlap The tendency to move in rhythmic synchrony with a musical in the brain (e.g., in the basal ganglia and supplementary motor beat (e.g., via head bobbing, foot tapping, or dance) is areas) [5, 12, 21].
    [Show full text]
  • Recovery Plan for South Australian Subspecies of the Glossy Black
    RECOVERY PLAN FOR THE SOUTH AUSTRALIAN SUBSPECIES OF THE GLOSSY BLACK-COCKATOO (Calyptorhynchus lathami halmaturinus): 2005-2010 P.A. Mooney and L.P. Pedler Glossy Black-Cockatoo Recovery Program Department for Environment and Heritage Kangaroo Island August 2005 Includes information from: Garnett, S.T., Crowley, G.M., Pedler, L. P., Prime, W., Twyford, K.L. and A. Maguire. 2000. ‘Recovery plan for the South Australian sub-species of the Glossy Black-Cockatoo: 1999-2003. i Cite as: Mooney, P.A. and Pedler, L.P. (2005): Recovery Plan for the South Australian subspecies of the Glossy Black-Cockatoo (Calyptorhynchus lathami halmaturinus): 2005-2010. Department for the Environment and Heritage South Australia. Further information and copies of this Recovery Plan are obtainable from: Department for Environment and Heritage Kangaroo Island PO Box 39 Kingscote South Australia 5223 Plan prepared in compliance with the Australian Government’s Environment Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act 1999. ii Acknowledgements This recovery plan is based in part on the previous recovery plan for the South Australian Glossy Black-Cockatoo prepared by Garnett et al. (2000). The assistance of members of the Glossy Black-Cockatoo Recovery Team in the preparation of this Plan is gratefully acknowledged, including, Peter Copley, Dave Dowie, Angela Duffy and Mary- Anne Healy (DEH), Karina Mercer, Vicki-Jo Russell (Threatened Species Network SA), Alison Buck and Jim Puckridge. We would like to express our appreciation to all the volunteers who have assisted us in the field in the last few years. Special thanks to Chris Baxter, Alison Buck, Tim Buck, Bryon Buick, Gail Capon, Sandy Carey, Chris Denman, Terry Dennis, Mark Gellard, Ken and Jenny Grinter, Philippa Kneebone, Nikki Lierich, Peter Malone, Wally Meakins, Anne Morrison, Jim Puckridge, Derek Snowball, Ann and Gordon Warner.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rainbow Bird
    The Rainbow Bird Volume 4 Number 3 August 2015 (Issue 83) THE LIFE OF GANNETS Further to Evelyn Williams' article "Point Danger Gannets" in "During the incubation period, the birds go offshore for up to the last issue of "The Rainbow Bird" The Age newspaper on four days and then when they are rearing the chicks, they June 1 had a report on further research into the Life of are away for between 12 and 15 hours," she said. Gannets. Ms Wells will present the results of the study at the World Bridie Smith, Science Editor writes: Seabird Conference in Cape Town, South Africa, in October. Researchers have attached cameras and tracking devices to the tails of Australasian Gannets to get a bird's-eye view of how they forage for their food in Victorian waters. Results of the fieldwork, carried out during the most recent breeding season in October to February, uncovered a previously undescribed hunting strategy revealing the gannets can adapt their foraging habits to suit their environment. The findings, which also revealed difference in hunting behaviour between the sexes, are important because it shows the seabirds can change their behaviours to suit different environments. "This is significant, as it shows that these birds have a degree of plasticity and that information is important in predicting how they may respond to future changes to their environment," said Deakin University researcher Melanie Wells. She said given the waters of south-east Australia are among the fastest warming waters on the planet, predicting how species will respond was crucial to predicting how a top predator like the gannet would fare as the climate changed.
    [Show full text]
  • Avian Influenza H5N1 and the Wild Bird Trade in Hanoi, Vietnam
    Copyright © 2009 by the author(s). Published here under license by the Resilience Alliance. Brooks-Moizer, F., S. I. Roberton, K. Edmunds, and D. Bell. 2008. Avian influenza H5N1 and the wild bird trade in Hanoi, Vietnam. Ecology and Society 14(1): 28. [online] URL: http://www.ecologyandsociety. org/vol14/iss1/art28/ Research, part of a Special Feature on Risk mapping for avian influenza: a social-ecological problem Avian Influenza H5N1 and the Wild Bird Trade in Hanoi, Vietnam F. Brooks-Moizer 1, Scott I. Roberton 2, Kelly Edmunds 1, and Diana Bell 1 ABSTRACT. Wildlife trade and emerging infectious diseases pose significant threats to human and animal health and global biodiversity. Legal and illegal trade in domestic and wild birds has played a significant role in the global spread of highly pathogenic avian influenza H5N1, which has killed more than 240 people, many millions of poultry, and an unknown number of wild birds and mammals, including endangered species, since 2003. This 2007 study provides evidence for a significant decline in the scale of the wild bird trade in Hanoi since previous surveys in 2000 (39.7% decline) and 2003 (74.1% decline). We attribute this to the enforcement of Vietnam’s Law 169/2005/QD UBND, introduced in 2005, which prohibits the movement and sale of wild and ornamental birds in cities. Nevertheless, 91.3% (21/23) of bird vendors perceived no risk of H5N1 infection from their birds, and the trade continues, albeit at reduced levels, in open market shops. These findings highlight the importance of continued law enforcement to maintain this trade reduction and the associated benefits to human and animal health and biodiversity conservation.
    [Show full text]