Avian and Human Movement to Music: Two Further Parallels

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Avian and Human Movement to Music: Two Further Parallels Communicative & Integrative Biology ISSN: (Print) 1942-0889 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/kcib20 Avian and human movement to music: Two further parallels Aniruddh D. Patel, John R. Iversen, Micah R. Bregman & Irena Schulz To cite this article: Aniruddh D. Patel, John R. Iversen, Micah R. Bregman & Irena Schulz (2009) Avian and human movement to music: Two further parallels, Communicative & Integrative Biology, 2:6, 485-488, DOI: 10.4161/cib.2.6.9373 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.4161/cib.2.6.9373 Copyright © 2009 Landes Bioscience Published online: 01 Nov 2009. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 42 View related articles Citing articles: 2 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=kcib20 Download by: [Tufts University] Date: 01 July 2016, At: 11:17 ARTICLE ADDENDUM ARTICLE ADDENDUM Communicative & Integrative Biology 2:6, 485-488; November/December 2009; © 2009 Landes Bioscience Avian and human movement to music Two further parallels Aniruddh D. Patel,1,* John R. Iversen,1 Micah R. Bregman1,2 and Irena Schulz3 1The Neurosciences Institute; 2University of California San Diego; San Digeo, CA USA 3Bird Lovers Only Rescue Service, Inc.; Dyer, IN USA t has recently been demonstrated that Ia nonhuman animal (the medium sul- phur-crested cockatoo Cacatua galerita eleonora) can entrain its rhythmic move- ments to the beat of human music across a wide range of tempi. Entrainment occurrs in “synchronized bouts”, occa- sional stretches of synchrony embed- ded in longer sequences of rhythmic movement to music. Here we examine non-synchronized rhythmic movements made while dancing to music, and find strong evidence for a preferred tempo around 126 beats per minute (bpm). The animal shows best synchroniza- tion to music when the musical tempo is near this preferred tempo. The tendency to dance to music at a preferred tempo, and to synchronize best when the music is near this tempo, parallels how young humans move to music. These findings support the idea that avian and human synchronization to music have similar Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 11:17 01 July 2016 neurobiological foundations. Key words: rhythm, music, All over the world, people move rhythmi- synchronization, entrainment, beat, cally in response to music with a regular tempo, birds, evolution, comparative beat. One notable feature of this response cognition is the tendency to synchronize movements Snowball, a cockatoo that dances to music. Submitted: 06/26/09 to the timing of perceived beats.1 This abil- Accepted: 06/29/09 ity involves the conjunction of two under- lying abilities: musical beat perception human, reflecting an evolutionary adapta- Previously published online: (the ability to infer a regular pulse from a tion for music cognition.4 However, recent www.landesbioscience.com/journals/cib/ complex rhythmic/melodic pattern), and empirical research has documented BPS article/9373 rhythmic motor production synchronized abilities in a number of vocal-learning spe- *Correspondence to: to the inferred beat. Musical beat percep- cies,5,6 supporting the hypothesis that BPS Aniruddh D. Patel; Email: [email protected] tion and synchronization (BPS) is a com- is a consequence of the brain circuitry for plex form of biological entrainment that complex vocal learning.7 Addendum to: Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I. Experimental evidence for differs in several ways from other examples At least one species, the medium synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhu- of synchrony in nature, such as the chorus- sulphur-crested cockatoo Cacatua gal- man animal. Curr Biol 2009; 19:827–30; PMID: ing of certain frogs or insects.2,3 Some the- erita eleonora, can entrain its rhythmic 19409790; DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038. orists have speculated that BPS is uniquely movements to the beat of human music www.landesbioscience.com Communicative & Integrative Biology 485 tended to dance at a tempo of about 155 bpm. Older children (3–4 years) tended to dance a bit more slowly, around 145 bpm, and showed a slight (but statistically nonsignificant) tendency to adjust move- ment tempo according to musical tempo. Hence when young children dance rhyth- mically to music, they tend to move at a preferred tempo which is not very sensi- tive to the musical tempo. Does Snowball, like young children, also have a preferred tempo when he is moving rhythmically to music but not synchronized to the beat? Evidence for Preferred Tempo To address this question, we examined all head bobs in our study which were not part of synchronized bouts. Figure 1A shows Snowball’s mean inter-bob-interval (mean Snowball ibi) for all non-entrained head bobs in each of the 22 trials with bouts. Mean Snowball ibi is plotted as a function of the musical inter-beat-interval (music Figure 1. The rate of Snowball’s inter-bob-intervals (SB ibi) for head bobs not entrained to the ibi, i.e., the temporal interval between musical beat. (A) Mean SB ibi of non-entrained head bobs in the 22 trials with bouts, as a function of musical inter-beat-intervals (Musical ibi). The best fitting regression line is shown (thick solid, musical beats as computed from the musi- Y = 0.286 + 0.366 * X; if outlier point is excluded, Y = 0.262 + 0.427 * X), as well as the identity line cal tempo: see Figure 1 caption for the (thin dashed). (B) Histogram of all non-entrained SB ibi’s in trials with bouts (n = 1,529; mean value equation relating tempo and music ibi). As shown by red line). (C) SB ibi as a function of Music ibi in the 16 trials without bouts. The best fitting evident from the figure, there is a tendency regression line is shown (thick solid, Y = 0.384 + 0.149 * X; if outlier point is excluded, Y = 0.387 + for these non-entrained head bobs to slow 0.153 * X), as well as the identity line (thin dashed). (D) Histogram of SB ibi in trials without bouts (n = 1,693; mean value shown by red line). In the scatterplots, tempo and musical inter-beat-interval down as the music slows down in tempo are related by the following equation: Musical tempo [bpm] = 60/Musical ibi (s). In the histograms, (i.e., as music ibi increases). However, bin width is 33 ms (equal to 1 video frame at 30 frames per second), and values >=1 s have been the regression relating Snowball ibi to excluded, as these indicate pauses in dancing. Music ibi is only marginally significant (p = 0.08, r2 = 0.14). If the outlying data across a wide range of tempi.5 This tem- experiment, bouts occurred in a subset of point with Snowball ibi <0.4 s is excluded, poral flexibility provides an important trials (58%), and in these trials, accounted the regression becomes significant Downloaded by [Tufts University] at 11:17 01 July 2016 parallel to human BPS, and raises the for about 25% of the head bobs on aver- (p < 0.01), but still explains only a modest question of whether other parallels exist age. In fact, across all head bobs measured amount of variance (r2 = 0.35, see Figure 1 between avian and human movement to in the study (n = 3,999), only 14% were caption for regression equations). In other music. One might expect further paral- part of bouts. words, the rate of non-entrained head bobs lels if similar brain mechanisms subserve The current paper focuses on the is only moderately sensitive to musical movement to music in birds and humans. remaining 86% of head bobs, and asks if tempo. Figure 1B suggests this could be Here we provide evidence for two further these show any systematic timing patterns. due to a tendency for the timing of these parallels, based on data gathered during In particular, do they show any evidence of head bobs to be dominated by a preferred our experimental study of a Cacatua gal- a preferred tempo, a rate of rhythmic move- tempo. This figure presents a histogram of erita eleonora named “Snowball”.5 In that ment toward which Snowball naturally all non-entrained Snowball ibi durations study, Snowball entrained his head bobs to gravitates while dancing? This question is across the 22 trials with bouts. The dis- the beat of music at nine different tempi motivated by research on young children.8 tribution has a single clear peak, near the spanning 98–130 beats per minute [bpm]. Specifically, a study of 2–4 year old chil- mean value of 0.480 s, corresponding to a Entrainment occurred in “synchronized dren dancing rhythmically to music found tempo of 125 bpm. bouts” (henceforth, “bouts”), episodes of strong evidence for a preferred movement Figure 1C and D extend these analyses synchrony embedded in longer stretches tempo. In that study, the tempo of a musi- to the 16 trials without bouts. Figure 1C of rhythmic movement to music. Notably, cal excerpt was varied across four rates, shows mean Snowball ibi values as a func- bouts accounted for a minority of head spanning 113 to 169 bpm. Despite this tion of Music ibi values during these trials. bobs made to music. Across the entire variation, younger children (2–3 years) Once again, the regression line relating 486 Communicative & Integrative Biology Volume 2 Issue 6 Snowball ibi to Music ibi is not significant (p = 0.20, r2 = 0.11). If the outlying data point with Snowball ibi <0.4 s is excluded, the regression becomes significant (p < 0.04), but explains only a modest amount of variance (r2 = 0.29, see Figure 1 caption for regression equations).
Recommended publications
  • ORNITHOLOGIST VOLUME 44 - PARTS 1&2 - November - 2019
    SOUTH AUSTRALIAN ORNITHOLOGIST VOLUME 44 - PARTS 1&2 - November - 2019 Journal of The South Australian Ornithological Association Inc. In this issue: Variation in songs of the White-eared Honeyeater Phenotypic diversity in the Copperback Quailthrush and a third subspecies Neonicotinoid insecticides Bird Report, 2011-2015: Part 1, Non-passerines President: John Gitsham The South Australian Vice-Presidents: Ornithological John Hatch, Jeff Groves Association Inc. Secretary: Kate Buckley (Birds SA) Treasurer: John Spiers FOUNDED 1899 Journal Editor: Merilyn Browne Birds SA is the trading name of The South Australian Ornithological Association Inc. Editorial Board: Merilyn Browne, Graham Carpenter, John Hatch The principal aims of the Association are to promote the study and conservation of Australian birds, to disseminate the results Manuscripts to: of research into all aspects of bird life, and [email protected] to encourage bird watching as a leisure activity. SAOA subscriptions (e-publications only): Single member $45 The South Australian Ornithologist is supplied to Family $55 all members and subscribers, and is published Student member twice a year. In addition, a quarterly Newsletter (full time Student) $10 reports on the activities of the Association, Add $20 to each subscription for printed announces its programs and includes items of copies of the Journal and The Birder (Birds SA general interest. newsletter) Journal only: Meetings are held at 7.45 pm on the last Australia $35 Friday of each month (except December when Overseas AU$35 there is no meeting) in the Charles Hawker Conference Centre, Waite Road, Urrbrae (near SAOA Memberships: the Hartley Road roundabout). Meetings SAOA c/o South Australian Museum, feature presentations on topics of ornithological North Terrace, Adelaide interest.
    [Show full text]
  • Cockatiels Free
    FREE COCKATIELS PDF Thomas Haupt,Julie Rach Mancini | 96 pages | 05 Aug 2008 | Barron's Educational Series Inc.,U.S. | 9780764138966 | English | Hauppauge, United States How to Take Care of a Cockatiel (with Pictures) - wikiHow A cockatiel is a popular choice for a pet bird. It is a small parrot with a variety of color patterns and a head crest. They are attractive as well as friendly. They are capable of mimicking speech, although they can be difficult to understand. These birds are good at whistling and you can teach them to sing along to tunes. Life Expectancy: 15 to 20 years with proper care, and sometimes as Cockatiels as 30 years though this is rare. In their native Australia, cockatiels are Cockatiels quarrions or weiros. They primarily live in the Cockatiels, a region of the northern part of the Cockatiels. Discovered inthey are the smallest members of the cockatoo family. They exhibit many of the Cockatiels features and habits as the larger Cockatiels. In the wild, they live in large flocks. Cockatiels became Cockatiels as pets during the s. They are easy to breed in captivity and their docile, friendly personalities make them a natural fit for Cockatiels life. These birds can Cockatiels longer be trapped and exported from Australia. These little birds are gentle, affectionate, and often like to be petted and held. Cockatiels are not necessarily fond of cuddling. They simply want to be near you and will be very happy to see you. Cockatiels are generally friendly; however, an untamed bird might nip. You can prevent bad Cockatiels at an early age Cockatiels ignoring bad behavior as these birds aim to please.
    [Show full text]
  • In Budgerigars
    帝京科学大学紀要 Vol.12(2016)pp.29-38 Chorus-like synchronized vocalizations(Big Chorus) in budgerigars 1 Hitomi ABE 1 Fujiro SAKURAI 1 Teikyo University of Science, Faculty of Life & Environmental Sciences-Department of Animal Science Abstract A number of recent studies have reported how budgerigars( Melopsittacus undulatus) are capable of mimicking various behaviors, and that individual vocalizations are strongly influenced by those of other conspecific birds in their vicinity. We examined this effect by placing four budgerigars in separate cages adjacent to each other, and then recording and analyzing all of their vocalizations over a two-week period. Time-based comparisons of the amount of time spent performing warble-songs over a 10 min period revealed a strong correlation between the duration of vocalization when all of the animals were males. However, no such correlation was observed when groups consisted of two males and two females. In the male-only group, whenever one male sang, the others would join in and singing would be synchronized. We repeatedly observed synchronized vocalizations where all of the birds sang together in what could be described as a chorus. While we were unable to demonstrate the functional significance of this behavior, since budgerigars are flocking birds, these synchronized vocalizations were not considered to function as territorial calls or for courtship. Keywords:Budgerigars, Vocal, Warble-song, Chorus-like synchronized, Introduction synchronizing their behaviors to rhythmic changes Budgerigars, the smallest parrots in the order in artificial light and auditory stimuli 7).Thus, Psittaciformes, live in large flocks in Australia while these birds can mimic and synchronize and are known to mimic the sound patterns of their behaviors, the manner in which they are conspecific individuals 1, 2).A recent study reported influenced by the vocalizations of their neighbors that when budgerigars were shown a video of has not yet been clarified.
    [Show full text]
  • Problem Sexual Behaviors of Companion Parrots
    21 Problem Sexual Behaviors of Companion Parrots Fern Van Sant INTRODUCTION Reproductive behaviors observed in the wild, Psittacine species have become popular as com- such as pair bonding, courtship regurgitation, panion animals because they demonstrate many cavity seeking, nest building, territorial aggres- kinds of social behavior that humans find famil- sion, and copulation, are often displayed in a iar and enjoyable. Such endearing traits as beauty, home setting, though with human “flocks.” Fe- intelligence, and mimicry have led humans to males of some species lay large numbers of eggs adopt them as members of their family. over extended periods of time to a point of com- Recognizing that some of these traits are innate plete physical collapse and failure. The reproduc- and genetically determined will allow for a new tive drive can also lead to behaviors that render and better understanding of companion psittacine the bird difficult or impossible to live with. These behavior and enable us to better predict responses behaviors often include incessant screaming, sud- of different species to the conditions of pet bird den aggression toward favored (or not favored) care. Developing an appreciation for how and humans, and destructive attempts to excavate why innate behaviors can be triggered by specific nests in closets, couches, and drawers. As many actions or conditions will hopefully lead to better species are represented in the pet trade, there ex- and healthier lives for captive psittacine birds. ists extreme variability as to when behaviors will Understanding innate behaviors of companion start, which will be displayed, and how long they psittacines requires an appreciation for the intri- may continue.[7] cate physiologic and hormonal events that adapt a In the wild, the expression of these largely re- species to an environmental niche.
    [Show full text]
  • Strengths and Vulnerabilities of Australian Networks for Conservation of Threatened Birds
    Strengths and vulnerabilities of Australian networks for conservation of threatened birds T IM Q. HOLMES,BRIAN W. HEAD,HUGH P. POSSINGHAM and S TEPHEN T. GARNETT Abstract We analysed the supportive social networks asso- making actors within an organizational framework trying ciated with the conservation of six threatened Australian to solve collective problems. The participants in an action bird taxa, in one of the first network analyses of threatened situation (Ostrom, ) for a threatened species may in- species conservation programmes. Each example showed clude those actively involved in the conservation of the contrasting vulnerabilities. The Alligator Rivers yellow bird or those with an interest in, or concern about, the chat Epthianura crocea tunneyi had the smallest social net- bird or a related issue. They may be individuals, or represen- work and no real action was supported. For the Capricorn tatives of NGOs, community groups, land owners, govern- yellow chat Epthianura crocea macgregori the network was ments or corporate actors, interacting for a range of reasons, centred on one knowledgeable and committed actor. The including obtaining or disseminating information, solving orange-bellied parrot Neophema chrysogaster had a strongly problems, negotiating, making decisions, collaborating connected recovery team but gaps in the overall network and seeking physical resources (Ostrom, ; Jepson could limit communication. The recovery teams for the et al., ). The key components of these action situations swift parrot Lathamus discolor and Baudin’s black-cockatoo include key participants, interorganizational collaborations Calyptorhynchus baudinii had strong links among most and social networks. Here, we focus on the social networks stakeholders but had weak ties to the timber industry and of key participants and seek to determine how such net- orchardists, respectively, limiting their capacity to manage works may influence the effective management of biodiver- threatening processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Conservation Implications of Illegal Bird Trade and Disease
    CONSERVATION IMPLICATIONS OF ILLEGAL BIRD TRADE AND DISEASE RISK IN PERU A Dissertation by ELIZABETH FRANCES DAUT Submitted to the Office of Graduate and Professional Studies of Texas A&M University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Chair of Committee, Donald J. Brightsmith Co-Chair of Committee, Markus J. Peterson Committee Members, Renata Ivanek-Miojevic Christian Brannstrom Head of Department, Roger Smith III May 2015 Major Subject: Veterinary Microbiology Copyright 2015 Elizabeth Frances Daut ABSTRACT Trade in wild-caught animals as pets is a global conservation and animal-welfare concern. Illegal and poorly-regulated legal wildlife trade can threaten biodiversity, spread infectious diseases, and result in considerable animal suffering and mortality. I used illegal wildlife trade in Peru, specifically native bird trade, as a case study to explore important aspects and consequences of the trade for domestic markets. With data collected from a five-year market survey and governmental seizure records, I applied a statistical modeling approach to investigate the influence of Peru’s legal export quota system on the country’s illegal domestic bird trade. I used an infectious-disease mathematical modeling approach to analyze how illegal harvest influenced disease dynamics in a wild parrot population. Finally, I used qualitative research methods to investigate the role of non-governmental organizations (NGOs) and their members’ philosophical perspectives toward wildlife in combating illegal trade. I found that Peru had a thriving illegal trade in native birds (mostly parrots) for domestic consumers; 150 species were recorded in markets and/or seizures with over 35,250 individuals offered for sale (2007–2011).
    [Show full text]
  • Biodiversity and Human Wellbeing Considerations in Managing the Urban Forest of a Global Biodiversity Hotspot
    Biodiversity and human wellbeing considerations in managing the urban forest of a global biodiversity hotspot Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub December 2020 About the Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub The Clean Air and Urban Landscapes (CAUL) Hub is funded by the Australian Government’s National Environmental Science Program. The remit of the CAUL Hub is to undertake “Research to support environmental quality in our urban areas”. This includes research on air quality, urban greening, liveability and biodiversity, with a focus on practical implementation of research findings, public engagement and participation by Indigenous Australians. The CAUL Hub is a consortium of four universities: The University of Melbourne, RMIT University, the University of Western Australia and the University of Wollongong. www.nespurban.edu.au Acknowledgements We acknowledge the Traditional Owners of the lands and waters where this research took place, The Whadjuk people of the Noongar nation, and pay our respects to Elders past, present and future. We acknowledge that all cities in Australia were built on Indigenous land, and that this land was never ceded. Please cite this document as Dickinson D. and Ramalho C.E. (2020) Biodiversity and human wellbeing considerations in managing the urban forest of a global biodiversity hotspot. Report prepared by the Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub. The Clean Air and Urban Landscapes Hub and Threatened Species Recovery Hub are funded by the Australian Government's National Environmental Science Program Contents Introduction
    [Show full text]
  • Zoos SA Annual Report 2019/2020
    Annual Report 2019-20 PASSION We inspire and influence by bringing pride, care and commitment to our work. Contents INNOVATION We seek creative ways to achieve goals and turn ideas into reality by promoting a culture of learning and improving. President and Chief Executive’s report ...................................... 2 Board members and governance ............................................... 4 RESPECT Zoos SA purpose and aspiration ............................................. 8 We respect individuals’ values and encourage Year in review: Key numbers ...............................................10 a culture of collaboration, diversity, listening and trust. Year in review: Finance snapshot .........................................12 Saving species inside our gates ............................................14 Leading the way in animal care and research ........................18 Conservation projects beyond our gates ...............................20 EFFECTIVENESS Connecting visitors with nature.........................................22 We are accountable, focus on clearly defined goals and work as a team to turn words Connecting children with nature ......................................24 into action. Educating the conservationists of tomorrow ........................26 Visitor experience and events ..........................................28 Connection to country and culture ..................................30 ENGAGEMENT Inspiring visitation and influencing change ......................32 We create experiences and connections
    [Show full text]
  • Experimental Evidence for Synchronization to a Musical Beat in a Nonhuman Animal
    Current Biology 19, 827–830, May 26, 2009 ª2009 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038 Report Experimental Evidence for Synchronization to a Musical Beat in a Nonhuman Animal Aniruddh D. Patel,1,* John R. Iversen,1 Micah R. Bregman,1,2 of tempi, and is crossmodal, with sound eliciting periodic and Irena Schulz3 movement that is not necessarily aimed at sound production 1The Neurosciences Institute [11]. BPS is thus an unusual form of rhythmic entrainment 10640 John Jay Hopkins Drive and is the focus of a growing body of behavioral, neural, and San Diego, CA 92121 computational research [e.g., 12–16]. USA The spontaneous development and apparent species spec- 2Department of Cognitive Science ificity of BPS make it reminiscent of how aspects of language University of California, San Diego develop. Yet BPS does not appear to be a by-product of our 9500 Gilman Drive linguistic abilities: ordinary speech does not employ regularly La Jolla, CA 92093 timed beats or elicit periodic movements from listeners USA [3, 17]. Might BPS be a biological specialization for music, 3Bird Lovers Only Rescue Service reflecting natural selection for musical abilities in our species P.O. Box 552 [18, 19]? Or is it a consequence of brain circuitry that evolved Dyer, IN 46311 for other reasons [20]? According to the ‘‘vocal learning and USA rhythmic synchronization’’ hypothesis [3], BPS relies on the brain circuitry for complex vocal learning because (1) BPS resembles vocal learning in that it involves special links Summary between the auditory and motor systems [4, 14] and (2) the neural substrates of vocal learning and BPS appear to overlap The tendency to move in rhythmic synchrony with a musical in the brain (e.g., in the basal ganglia and supplementary motor beat (e.g., via head bobbing, foot tapping, or dance) is areas) [5, 12, 21].
    [Show full text]
  • Recovery Plan for South Australian Subspecies of the Glossy Black
    RECOVERY PLAN FOR THE SOUTH AUSTRALIAN SUBSPECIES OF THE GLOSSY BLACK-COCKATOO (Calyptorhynchus lathami halmaturinus): 2005-2010 P.A. Mooney and L.P. Pedler Glossy Black-Cockatoo Recovery Program Department for Environment and Heritage Kangaroo Island August 2005 Includes information from: Garnett, S.T., Crowley, G.M., Pedler, L. P., Prime, W., Twyford, K.L. and A. Maguire. 2000. ‘Recovery plan for the South Australian sub-species of the Glossy Black-Cockatoo: 1999-2003. i Cite as: Mooney, P.A. and Pedler, L.P. (2005): Recovery Plan for the South Australian subspecies of the Glossy Black-Cockatoo (Calyptorhynchus lathami halmaturinus): 2005-2010. Department for the Environment and Heritage South Australia. Further information and copies of this Recovery Plan are obtainable from: Department for Environment and Heritage Kangaroo Island PO Box 39 Kingscote South Australia 5223 Plan prepared in compliance with the Australian Government’s Environment Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act 1999. ii Acknowledgements This recovery plan is based in part on the previous recovery plan for the South Australian Glossy Black-Cockatoo prepared by Garnett et al. (2000). The assistance of members of the Glossy Black-Cockatoo Recovery Team in the preparation of this Plan is gratefully acknowledged, including, Peter Copley, Dave Dowie, Angela Duffy and Mary- Anne Healy (DEH), Karina Mercer, Vicki-Jo Russell (Threatened Species Network SA), Alison Buck and Jim Puckridge. We would like to express our appreciation to all the volunteers who have assisted us in the field in the last few years. Special thanks to Chris Baxter, Alison Buck, Tim Buck, Bryon Buick, Gail Capon, Sandy Carey, Chris Denman, Terry Dennis, Mark Gellard, Ken and Jenny Grinter, Philippa Kneebone, Nikki Lierich, Peter Malone, Wally Meakins, Anne Morrison, Jim Puckridge, Derek Snowball, Ann and Gordon Warner.
    [Show full text]
  • Current Biology 070819.Pdf
    Current Biology Magazine Correspondence interested in his diversity of movements One important difference between to music. This was because his owner Snowball’s dancing and human dancing Spontaneity (author I.S.) observed that Snowball is that Snowball danced in short was using new movements to music episodes rather than continuously and diversity of not present in our original study, and (mean duration = 3.69 seconds, std = appeared to be in a period of ‘movement 2.72 seconds; N = 141), consistent with movement to music exploration’. Importantly, this was not earlier reports [2]. Informal observations are not uniquely due to modeling by I.S., who does by author I.S. suggest that Snowball not make a wide range of movements moves more continuously when a human when dancing with Snowball and human dances with him, which we plan tends only to engage in head bobbing to examine in future work. and hand waving. Also, Snowball was To determine if certain moments in R. Joanne Jao Keehn1, John R. Iversen2, never explicitly trained to make specifi c each song triggered specifi c dance Irena Schulz3, and Aniruddh D. Patel4,5,6,* movements to music (for example, movements, time segments during via operant conditioning with food which Snowball danced in all three Spontaneous movement to music rewards, as in [7]). During this period of trials were fi rst identifi ed (Figure S1B). occurs in every human culture and is a exploration, Snowball seemed to favor We then examined each such segment foundation of dance [1]. This response movement diversity over synchronization to see if he used the same dance to music is absent in most species accuracy: his rhythmic movements often movement in all three trials.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rainbow Bird
    The Rainbow Bird Volume 4 Number 3 August 2015 (Issue 83) THE LIFE OF GANNETS Further to Evelyn Williams' article "Point Danger Gannets" in "During the incubation period, the birds go offshore for up to the last issue of "The Rainbow Bird" The Age newspaper on four days and then when they are rearing the chicks, they June 1 had a report on further research into the Life of are away for between 12 and 15 hours," she said. Gannets. Ms Wells will present the results of the study at the World Bridie Smith, Science Editor writes: Seabird Conference in Cape Town, South Africa, in October. Researchers have attached cameras and tracking devices to the tails of Australasian Gannets to get a bird's-eye view of how they forage for their food in Victorian waters. Results of the fieldwork, carried out during the most recent breeding season in October to February, uncovered a previously undescribed hunting strategy revealing the gannets can adapt their foraging habits to suit their environment. The findings, which also revealed difference in hunting behaviour between the sexes, are important because it shows the seabirds can change their behaviours to suit different environments. "This is significant, as it shows that these birds have a degree of plasticity and that information is important in predicting how they may respond to future changes to their environment," said Deakin University researcher Melanie Wells. She said given the waters of south-east Australia are among the fastest warming waters on the planet, predicting how species will respond was crucial to predicting how a top predator like the gannet would fare as the climate changed.
    [Show full text]