Publ 107346 Issue CH05 Page

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Publ 107346 Issue CH05 Page Squibs and Discussion SYMMETRIC RECIPROCAL 1 Introduction SEMANTICS AS A PREDICTOR Wilkinson (1971) and Lawler (1972) originally observed the phenome- OF PARTIAL CONTROL non of partial control (PC). Descriptively, PC refers to situations in J.-Marc Authier which the reference of PRO must include that of an overt argument The Pennsylvania State in the matrix clause, but is not exhaustively determined by that argu- University ment. The effects of PC are best observed in sentences like (1a), which Lisa Reed contain an infinitival whose predicate is unambiguously collective (i.e., The Pennsylvania State one that requires, rather than just allows, its subject to denote a plural University entity; cf. (1b)). to kiss םto meet at 6:00/PROj םa. Clairej wanted [PROj (1) in the kitchen]. b. The lovers/*Claire met/kissed in the kitchen. Most of the research on PC has focused on the (semantic) properties of those matrix predicates that license the phenomenon (see, e.g., Landau 2000, White and Grano 2013 for a survey and experimental data, as well as Pearson 2016). One notable exception to this trend is provided by Sheehan (2012, 2014), who observes that PC in European Portu- guese, Italian, Spanish, and French displays a selective availability based on what kind of collective predicate appears in the embedded infinitival containing PRO.1 Specifically, she points out that the de- scriptive generalization in (2) seems to hold. We gratefully acknowledge the helpful comments and suggestions made by Benjamin Bruening, Idan Landau, Tal Siloni, Dominique Sportiche, the LI reviewers, the Squibs and Discussion editors, and the audiences at the 41st Penn Linguistics Colloquium and the 47th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages hosted by the University of Delaware. We also wish to thank our informants: Nigel Duffield and Ian Roberts for (British) English and Simon Cottart, Morgane Haesen, Johann LeGuelte, Marie Paillard, and Timothe´e Va- lentin for French. All remaining errors are, naturally enough, our sole responsi- bility. 1 Landau (2000:86) was the first to notice this selective availability in French but erroneously assumed that while PC was generally available in French, it was impossible with any of the collective se-predicates. He also noted that his theory of control did not offer a satisfactory explanation for the latter fact. Linguistic Inquiry, Volume 49, Number 2, Spring 2018 379–393 ᭧ 2018 by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology doi: 10.1162/ling_a_00276 379 Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/LING_a_00276 by guest on 23 September 2021 380 SQUIBS AND DISCUSSION (2) PC readings arise in Romance only with those embedded collective predicates that can take an overt comitative argu- ment. Thus, French se re´unir ‘meet/gather’ can take an overt comitative argument but s’embrasser ‘kiss/hug’ cannot, and, as a result, only the former can occur in a PC infinitival.2 (3) a. Eric s’est re´uni avec ses amis. Eric SE-is met with his friends ‘Eric met with his friends.’ .[se re´unir dans la cuisine םb. Ericj voulait [PROj Eric wanted SE meet in the kitchen ‘Eric wanted to meet in the kitchen.’ (4) a. *Eric s’est embrasse´ avec Nadine. Eric SE-is kissed with Nadine ‘*Eric kissed with Nadine.’ .[s’embrasser dans la cuisine םb. *Ericj voulait [PROj Eric wanted SE-kiss in the kitchen ‘Eric wanted to kiss in the kitchen.’ Since the generalization in (2) does not seem to apply to English (see the English glosses in (4)), Sheehan calls examples like (3b) instances of ‘‘fake PC’’ and argues that this distinct phenomenon arises indi- rectly from a silent comitative phrase present in the infinitival comple- ment (as Hornstein (2003) and others have proposed is the case in all languages). Landau (2016a) convincingly shows, however, that Sheehan’s analysis of PC in Romance is untenable by pointing out that elements syntactically and/or semantically associated with overt comitatives are systematically unavailable with PC complements. For example, while an adverb like se´pare´ment ‘separately’ can modify an overt comitative (5a), it fails to occur in those PC complements alleged to have a null comitative structure (5b). (5) a. Le Pre´sident a dit a` ses homologues des E-U the president has said to his counterparts of.the US et de la Russie qu’il pre´fe´rait se re´unir avec and of the Russia that-he preferred SE meet with eux se´pare´ment. them separately ‘The president told his US and Russian counterparts that he preferred to meet with them separately.’ 2 Note that (4b) is grammatical on an exhaustive obligatory control read- ing, with se taking on a reflexive meaning paraphrasable in English as ‘Eric wanted to kiss himself in the kitchen’. Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/LING_a_00276 by guest on 23 September 2021 SQUIBS AND DISCUSSION 381 b. Le Pre´sident a dit a` ses homologues des E-U the president has said to his counterparts of.the US et de la Russie qu’il pre´fe´rait se re´unir and of the Russia that-he preferred SE meet (*se´pare´ment) avant Noe¨l. (separately) before Christmas ‘The president told his US and Russian counterparts that he preferred to meet (*separately) before Christmas.’ We add to Landau’s arguments one of our own, one that is based on the observation, due to Dimitriadis (2004), that the semantics of simple reciprocals enriched with a comitative phrase (also called discontin- uous reciprocals) is more specific (or expressive) than that of their corresponding simple reciprocals. Consider in this respect the para- digm in (6). (6) a. Eric, Nadine, et quelqu’un d’autre se sont dispute´s. Eric Nadine and someone else SE are argued ‘Eric, Nadine, and someone else argued.’ b. Eric et Nadine se sont dispute´s avec quelqu’un Eric and Nadine SE are argued with someone d’autre. else ‘Eric and Nadine argued with someone else.’ As Dimitriadis points out, a sentence like (6a) describes a quarreling event involving Eric, Nadine, and someone else with no specification as to who was in conflict with whom. The interpretation of the discon- tinuous reciprocal construction in (6b), on the other hand, is more specific in that it expresses a reciprocal relation between pairs consist- ing of one participant (possibly plural) taken from the denotation of the subject and another participant taken from the denotation of the comitative phrase. Thus, (6b) is either about a disagreement involving Eric and Nadine versus someone else, or about two separate conflicts, one involving Eric versus someone else and another involving Nadine versus that someone else. Consider next the sentence in (7) on the PC reading symbolized by the indices. s’eˆtre םEric et Nadine]j se rappellent [PROj] (7) Eric and Nadine SE remember SE-to.be dispute´s]. argued ‘Eric and Nadine remember arguing.’ On the assumption that (7) contains a null comitative phrase, we expect the interpretation of that sentence to be akin to that of (6b) in that it should involve pairs whose first member corresponds to the denotation of the controller (i.e., Eric and Nadine) and whose second member corresponds to the denotation of the null comitative. This expectation is not fulfilled, however. Indeed, the PC reading associated with (7) can only be paraphrased as follows: Eric and Nadine remember that there was a quarreling event involving Eric, Nadine, and some other Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/LING_a_00276 by guest on 23 September 2021 382 SQUIBS AND DISCUSSION unspecified individual(s) with no specification as to who was in conflict with whom. The fact that (7) cannot have the more specific reading tied to the presence of an overt comitative phrase in (6b) thus provides an additional argument against the presence of a null comitative in PC contexts. Landau’s (2016a,b) analysis of PC, which does not posit a null comitative phrase but takes PRO in PC to be a group-denoting, syntac- tically singular but semantically plural noun, is not (directly) chal- lenged by the facts in (5) and (7). However, given the untenable charac- ter of Sheehan’s analysis of Romance ‘‘fake PC’’ demonstrated above, neither Landau’s account of PC nor its alternatives can explain why the selective availability of PC exhibited by Romance embedded predi- cates is not also present in English (see, e.g., (4b) vs. (1a)). In this squib, we will claim that (a) there is no such thing as ‘‘fake PC’’; that is, there is only one kind of PC, and this phenomenon is subject to the same conditions in English as it is in Romance; (b) one such condition is that the embedded collective predicate have (irreducibly) symmetric reciprocal semantics in the sense of Siloni (2002, 2012) and Dimitriadis (2004, 2008); and (c) the difference between English and Romance boils down to the facts that only recip- rocals formed in the lexicon introduce symmetric semantics and that the set of reciprocals formed in the lexicon in English and that formed in the lexicon in Romance are not identical. The assumption that recip- rocal verbs can be formed in different components of the gram- mar—namely, the lexicon and the syntax—is based on a coherent cluster of distinctive properties (such as degree of productivity) first uncovered by Siloni (2001) and further substantiated by Reinhart and Siloni (2005), who capture the split via a Lex(icon)-Syn(tax) parame- ter. This parameter states that Universal Grammar allows valence- changing operations to apply either in the lexicon or in the syntax. Following Siloni (2002), we will argue that only lexical reciprocals are symmetric and that French, being a language with a syntactic setting of the Lex-Syn parameter, only has isolated instances of lexical reciprocal verbs.
Recommended publications
  • The Ingredients of Reciprocity in Cuzco Quechua
    Accepted for publication in the Journal of Semantics, pre-final version, February 2007. 1 The ingredients of reciprocity in Cuzco Quechua Martina Faller The University of Manchester Abstract In Cuzco Quechua reciprocity is marked by means of two verbal suffixes, one of which is a marker of reflexivity, the other of which is a marker of pluractionality. The paper develops an analysis that composes reciprocity from these more basic notions. Two further ingredients that are needed will be argued to derive from independent principles: universal quantifi- cation over parts of the reciprocal plural agent derives from plural pred- ication, as has been argued by other researchers for English reciprocity; distinctness of the participants in the reciprocal subevents derives from a semantic version of Condition B. This way of composing reciprocity is not universal, other languages have dedicated reciprocal markers or make other reciprocal ingredients overt. The compositional derivation of reciprocity is therefore a clear candidate for cross-linguistic semantic variation. 1 Introduction This paper develops a compositional analysis of reciprocity in Cuzco Quechua (CQ) with the aim of elucidating the question of how languages may differ in the compositional derivation of this complex semantic notion. In a wider sense, it is a contribution to the growing literature on the possible space of cross-linguistic semantic variation. Consider the following Quechua reciprocal sentence and its English translation. (1) Hayt’a-na-ku-n-ku. kick-pa-refl-3-pl ‘They kick each other.’ Both entail that there are at least two kicking events, that the agent and the theme of each kicking event are distinct but drawn from the same group con- sisting of at least two members (namely the group denoted by they, which serves as the antecedent for the pronominal each other), and that each of the members of this group is an agent of at least one of these events and a theme of at least another one.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntax of the Malagasy Reciprocal Construction: an Lfg Account
    THE SYNTAX OF THE MALAGASY RECIPROCAL CONSTRUCTION: AN LFG ACCOUNT Peter Hurst University of Melbourne Proceedings of the LFG06 Conference Universität Konstanz Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King (Editors) 2006 CSLI Publications http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/ ABSTRACT The verbal reciprocal construction in Malagasy is formed by a reciprocal morpheme prefixing on the main verb with a corresponding loss of an overt argument in c-structure. Analyses of similar constructions in Chichewa and Catalan both treat the reciprocalized verb's argument structure as undergoing an alteration whereby one of its thematic roles is either suppressed or two thematic arguments are mapped to one grammatical function. In this paper I propose that the reciprocal morpheme in Malagasy creates a reciprocal pronoun in f-structure - thus maintaining its valency and leaving the argument structure of the verb unchanged, while at the same time losing an argument at the level of c-structure. 1. INTRODUCTION Malagasy is an Austronesian language and is the dominant language of Madagascar. The Malagasy sentences used in the analysis below are from the literature - in particular from a paper by Keenan and Razafimamonjy (2001) titled “Reciprocals in Malagasy” whose examples are based on the official dialect of Malagasy as spoken in and around the capital city Antananarivo. The Malagasy reciprocal construction is formed by the addition of a prefix -if- or -ifamp- to the stem of the verb accompanied by the loss of an overt argument in object position. Compare sentence (1a) below with its reciprocated equivalent (1b): (1) Malagasy a. N-an-daka an-dRabe Rakoto pst-act-kick acc.Rabe Rakoto V O S 'Rakoto kicked Rabe' b.
    [Show full text]
  • A Backwards Binding Construction in Zapotec
    Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session Volume 40 Article 1 1996 A backwards binding construction in Zapotec Cheryl A. Black SIL-UND Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers Part of the Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Black, Cheryl A. (1996) "A backwards binding construction in Zapotec," Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session: Vol. 40 , Article 1. DOI: 10.31356/silwp.vol40.01 Available at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers/vol40/iss1/1 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by UND Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session by an authorized editor of UND Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. A Backwards Binding Construction in Zapotec* Cheryl A. Black Many of the Zapotecan languages have a unique way of signalling coreference between the subject and the possessor of the object: the subject is null. Such a construction is upsidedown or backwards from commonly described anaphora con­ structions and its analysis is therefore problematic to current theories. This paper describes the construction and underlines the theoretical problem by arguing against any obvious alternative analyses. An analysis is proposed where it is the tail {rather than the head) of the chain of coreferent elements that is identified, suggesting that this is another place where parameterization is needed. 1. Introduction One part of Binding Theory deals with simple refl.exive constructions, such as (1) (where coindexing indicates coreference).
    [Show full text]
  • Further Remarks on Reciprocal Constructions (To Appear In: Nedjalkov, Vladimir P
    1 Further remarks on reciprocal constructions (to appear in: Nedjalkov, Vladimir P. (ed.) 2007. Reciprocal constructions. Amsterdam: Benjamins.) MARTIN HASPELMATH In view of the breathtaking scope of the comparative research enterprise led by Vladimir P. Nedjalkov whose results are published in these volumes, I have no choice but to select and highlight a few topics that I find particularly interesting and worthy of further comment and further study. I will focus here on conceptual and terminological issues and on some phenomena that have been discussed in the literature but are not so well represented in this work. I will also try to summarize some of the major known generalizations about reciprocals, as discussed in this work and elsewhere, in the form of twenty-six Greenberg-style numbered universals. 1. Reciprocal, mutual, symmetric Let us begin with a terminological discussion of the most basic term, reciprocal. In the present volumes, this term is used both for meanings (e.g. reciprocal situation, reciprocal event) and for forms (e.g. reciprocal construction, reciprocal marker, reciprocal predicate). In most cases, the context will disambiguate, but it seems to be a good idea to have two different terms for meanings and for forms, analogous to similar contrasts such as proposition/sentence, question/interrogative, participant/argument, time/tense, multiple/plural. Since all reciprocals express a situation with a mutual relation, I propose the term mutual for the semantic plane, reserving the term reciprocal for specialized expression patterns that code a mutual situation. A similar terminological distinction is made by König & Kokutani (2006), Evans (2007), Dimitriadis (2007), but these authors propose the term symmetric for meanings, reserving reciprocal for forms.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Reciprocal in Ndebele Langa KHUMALO 1 University of Kwazulu-Natal, South Africa
    Nordic Journal of African Studies 23(3): 140–161 (2014) On the Reciprocal in Ndebele Langa KHUMALO 1 University of KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa ABSTRACT This article presents an analysis of the reciprocal extension in the Ndebele language (S.44, ISO 639-3 nde; not to be confused with South African Ndebele, S.407, ISO 639-3nbl) using the apparatus of the Lexical Functional Grammar’s Lexical Mapping Theory. The reciprocal in Ndebele, like in most Bantu languages, is clearly marked by the verbal suffix an-. Its typical properties are that the subject NP must be plural or alternatively must be a coordinate structure and that it is an argument changing verbal extension. This article will demonstrate that in Ndebele the reciprocal verb can take the direct object. It will further show that the reciprocal in Ndebele can co-occur with the passive and finally the paper will show that the notion of transitivity is not so straightforward both at syntactic and semantic levels when viewed in the context of certain reciprocal constructions. Keywords : reciprocal, argument structure, LMT, dyadic and monadic reciprocal. 1. INTRODUCTION This article discusses the Ndebele reciprocal derivation using the Lexical Mapping Theory (henceforth LMT), which is a sub-theory of Lexical Functional Grammar (henceforth LFG). It is important to note that LFG owes its origins from the dissatisfaction with Chomsky’s early framework of linguistic analysis as espoused in the Principles and Parameter framework and Government and Binding Theory. LFG has been described as the non-transformational successor to the transformational generative forerunners of Government and Binding.
    [Show full text]
  • Even (Malchukov).Pdf
    Even Andrei L. Malchukov LANGUAGES OF TilE WORLD/Malerials \2 1995 LINCOM EUROPA Miinchen - Newcastle Published by LlNCOM EUROPA. Munchen. Newcaslle. 1995. All correspondence concerning LANGUAGES OF THE WORLD/Materials should be addressed to: LlNCOM EUROPA. P.O. Box 1316. D-85703 Unterschleissheim/Munchen. Germany. All rights reserved . including the righlS of translation into any foreign language. No part of this book may be reproduced in any way without the permission of the publisher. Printed in Nurnberg. Germany Edited by U.J. Uiders Scientific Advisory Board of L1NGUAGES OF THE WORLD/Materials (LW/M); W. Bisang, M. Brenzi nger , F. Corricmc, R.M.W. Dixon. W.Foley, J. Goddard. N. Himmelmann, A.E. Kibrik. L. Johanson. A.S. Kaye, M. Mithun, U. Mosel, J. Owens. G. Sommer, H.E. Wolff. Die DeUlsche BibliOlhek - CIP-Einheitsaufnahme Malchukov, Andrej L.: Even I Andrej L. Malchukov. - MOnchen (i.e. ) Unterschleissheim; Newcastle : LlNCOM EUROPA, 1995 . (Languages of the world: Materials; 12) ISBN 3-929075-13-X British Library Cataloguing in PublicaJion Dala A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Printed on ch lorine·free paper CU lltellls O. Foreword. .......................... .... ...................... ................ .. ............. 3 PART I: Essemials of Even grammar ..... ........................... .. ............................. 3 I. Gener.tl data on Even ..... .............. ....................... .................... .............. 3 1. 1. General socio- and geo·linguistic infonnation ...........
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics
    UC Berkeley Dissertations, Department of Linguistics Title Runyambo Verb Extensions and Constructions on Predicate Structure Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1xp5453s Author Rugemalira, Josephat Publication Date 1993 eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Runyambo Verb Extensions and Constraints on Predicate Structure by Josephat Muhozi Rugemalira B .A. (University of Dar es Salaam) 1984 M.A. (University of Lancaster) 1986 M.A. (University of California at Berkeley) 1991 A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the GRADUATE DIVISION of the UNIVERSITY Of CALIFORNIA at BERKELEY Committee in charge: Professor Charles J. Fillmore, Chair Professor Sam A. Mchombo Professor Johanna B. Nichols Professor Larry M. Hyman 1993 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. The dissertation of Josephat Muhozi Rugemalira is approved: /iW ^ tqq \ Date __________ a/st; % /f fj v' ;---- Dat'e s---- L ---- _ \ / ) FvV <Lr.X--°1 KiCv . A- , 199 5 J . Date (A t!M fC^I* lyt^SL IjVOUJLo / U o o ■ / f i V S University of California at Berkeley 1993 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ABSTRACT RUNYAMBO VERB EXTENSIONS AND CONSTRAINTS ON PREDICATE STRUCTURE by Josephat Muhozi Rugemalira Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California at Berkeley Professor Charles J. Fillmore, Chair This study presents a description of the productive verb extensions in Runyambo, a Bantu language of Tanzania. It challenges the common view that the extensions are potentially a resource for increasing the number of a verb's arguments indefinitely, and shows instead that the extensions form part of a set of interrelated mechanisms, within the Bantu languages, which ensure that the arguments of a verb remain distinguishable from each other.
    [Show full text]
  • Looking Into Reduplication in Indonesian
    Double Double, Morphology and Trouble: Looking into Reduplication in Indonesian Meladel Mistica, Avery Andrews, I Wayan Arka Timothy Baldwin The Australian National University The University of Melbourne {meladel.mistica,avery.andrews, [email protected] wayan.arka}@anu.edu.au Abstract tronic grammar for Indonesian within the framework of Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG). Our project This paper investigates reduplication in In- forms part of a group of researchers, PARGRAM1 donesian. In particular, we focus on verb redu- whose aim is to also produce wide-coverage gram- plication that has the agentive voice affix meN, exhibiting a homorganic nasal. We outline the mars built on a collaboratively agreed upon set of recent changes we have made to the imple- grammatical features (Butt et al., 1999). In order mentation of our Indonesian grammar, and the to ensure comparability we use the same linguistic motivation for such changes. tools for implementation.2 There are two main issues that we deal with One of the issues we address is how to adequately in our implementation: how we account for account for morphophonemic facts, as schematised the morphophonemic facts relating to sound in Examples (1), (2) and (3): changes in the morpheme; and how we con- ∧ struct word formation (i.e. sublexical) rules in (1) [meN+tarik] 2 creating these derived words exhibiting redu- ↔ meN+tarik+hyphen+meN+tarik plication. ↔ menarik-menarik “pulling (iteratively)” 1 Introduction ∧ (2) meN+[tarik] 2 This study looks at full reduplication in Indone- ↔ meN+tarik+hyphen+tarik sian verbs, which is a morphological operation that ↔ menarik-narik (*menarik-tarik) involves the doubling of a lexical stem.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflexive and Reciprocal Encoding in the Australian Mixed
    languages Article Reflexive and Reciprocal Encoding in the Australian Mixed Language, Light Warlpiri Carmel O’Shannessy 1,* and Connor Brown 2 1 School of Literature, Languages and Linguistics, College of Arts and Social Sciences, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 2600, Australia 2 School of Social Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA 6009, Australia; [email protected] * Correspondence: Carmel.O’[email protected] Abstract: Mixed languages combine significant amounts of grammatical and lexical material from more than one source language in systematic ways. The Australian mixed language, Light Warlpiri, combines nominal morphology from Warlpiri with verbal morphology from Kriol (an English-lexified Creole) and English, with innovations. The source languages of Light Warlpiri differ in how they encode reflexives and reciprocals—Warlpiri uses an auxiliary clitic for both reflexive and reciprocal expression, while English and Kriol both use pronominal forms, and largely have separate forms for reflexives and reciprocals. English distinguishes person and number in reflexives, but not in reciprocals; the other source languages do not distinguish person or number. This study draws on naturalistic and elicited production data to examine how reflexive and reciprocal events are encoded in Light Warlpiri. The study finds that Light Warlpiri combines near-maximal distinctions from the source languages, but in a way that is not a mirror of any. It retains the person and number distinctions of English reflexives and extends them to reciprocals, using the same forms for Citation: O’Shannessy, Carmel, and reflexives and reciprocals (like Warlpiri). Reflexives and reciprocals occur within a verbal structure Connor Brown.
    [Show full text]
  • Encoding Definiteness on Pronominal Objects in Mordvinic
    Mariann Bernhardt University of Turku Encoding definiteness on pronominal objects in Mordvinic This article examines the morphosyntax of pronouns in object function and reveals the syntactic and morphological differences between nominal and pronominal objects in Mordvinic. The variation in case marking and declen- sion type of nominal objects is affected by definiteness. Indefinite objects are in the basic declension nominative, whereas definite ones are in the definite or possessive declension genitive. Furthermore, definite objects may be in- dexed on the verb. In this paper, I analyze the morphosyntax of pronouns, in order to reveal the regularities between semantics and morphological mark- ing and to provide a better understanding of definiteness. For this purpose, the finite forms of perception verbs were collected from the MokshEr corpus, which contains written texts in the literary languages, and native speakers were consulted on the results. Perception verbs were chosen for this study be- cause they agree with the object in person and number more frequently than other semantic classes of verbs, thus providing good material for examining the correlation of definiteness with verbal conjugation. The paper shows how definiteness is displayed within the morphosyntax of pronouns and uncovers how verbal conjugation correlates with different pronominal objects. 1. Introduction 4.2. Demonstrative pronouns 2. Background 4.3. Reflexive pronouns 3. Data 4.3.1. Erzya 4. The pronominal objects of 4.3.2. Moksha perception verbs 4.4. Reciprocal pronouns 4.1. Personal pronouns and other 4.5. Interrogative pronouns person indexes 4.6. Relative pronouns 4.1.1. Indexing object person 4.7. Indefinite pronouns with verbs in the subjective 4.8.
    [Show full text]
  • The Antisymmetry of Syntax
    Contents Series Foreword xi Preface xiii Acknowledgments xvii Chapter 1 Introduction 3 1.1 Introduction 3 1.2 Proposal 5 Chapter 2 Deriving XBar lhory 7 PART 11 13 Chapter 3 Adjunction 15 3.1 Segments and Categories 15 3.2 Adjunction to a Head 17 3.3 Multiple Adjunctions: Clitics 19 3.4 Multiple Adjunctions: Nonheads 21 3.5 Specifiers 22 ... Vlll Contents Contents 3.6 Verb-Second Effects 27 Chapter 6 3.7 Adjunction of a Head to a Nonhead 30 Coordination 57 6.1 More on Coordination 57 Chapter 4 6.2 Coordination of Heads, Wordorder 33 4.1 The specifier-complement including Clitics 59 Asymmetry 33 6.3 Coordination with With 63 4.2 Specifier-Head-Complement as a Universal Order 35 6.4 Right Node Raising 67 4.3 Time and the Universal Chapter 7 -- Specifier-Head-Complement Order Complementation 69 7.1 Multiple Complements and 36 Adjuncts 69 4.4. Linear Order and Adjunction to 7.2 Heavy NP Shift 71 Heads 38 7.3 Right-Dislocations 78 4.5 Linear Order and Structure below the Word Level 38 Relatives and Posseshes 85 8.1 Postnominal Possessives in 4.6 The Adjunction Site of Clitics English 85 42 8.2 Relative Clauses in English 86 Chapter 5 Fortherconsequences 47 5.1 There Is No Directionality 8.3 N-Final Relative Clauses 92 Parameter 47 8.4 Reduced Relatives and 5.2 The LCA Applies to All Syntactic Representations 48 Adjectives 97 8.5 More on Possessives 101 I 5.3 Agreement in Adpositional Phrases 49 1 8.6 More on French De 105 b 5.4 Head Movement 50 8.7 Nonrestrictive Relatives 1 10 5.5 Final Complementizers and Agglutination 52 ..
    [Show full text]
  • An Investigation of the Syntactic, Semantic and Pragmatic Interfaces
    Reciprocity in Russian: An investigation of the syntactic, semantic and pragmatic interfaces DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Lauren Ressue Graduate Program in Slavic and East European Languages and Cultures The Ohio State University 2015 Dissertation Committee: Andrea Sims, Advisor Judith Tonhauser, Advisor Daniel Collins 1 Copyright By Lauren Ressue 2015 2 Abstract This dissertation explores two reciprocal expressions in Russian, drug druga and reciprocal -sja verbs to determine their distribution, semantics and pragmatics. I argue that while these two expressions are similar in many ways, they also differ in subtle ways not before discussed. While the empirical foundations of this dissertation are data from Russian, my findings have empirical and theoretical consequences for both the formal semantic and the typological literature on reciprocity. In English, the reciprocal expression each other has been studied in detail to explore the relations between participants it is compatible with (Langendoen 1978, Dalrymple et al. 1998b, Beck 2001). For example, the sentence in (i) is compatible with either of the relations depicted in (a) and (b), where the arrows represent a binary painting relation. (i) The four artists painted each other. (a) (b) In this dissertation, I explore the meaning of reciprocal expressions in Russian to discover whether these expressions have the same meaning as each other and whether they contribute any ii meaning other than relations between individuals. My data comes from both a corpus study utilizing the Russian National Corpus and elicitation with native speakers of Russian.
    [Show full text]