Emery (2004) the Mentality of Crows

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Emery (2004) the Mentality of Crows REVIEW The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes Nathan J. Emery1* and Nicola S. Clayton2 tools. Hook tools are crafted from twigs by Discussions of the evolution of intelligence have focused on monkeys and apes because trimming and sculpting until a functional of their close evolutionary relationship to humans. Other large-brained social animals, hook has been fashioned (13) and are used to such as corvids, also understand their physical and social worlds. Here we review recent poke out insect larvae from holes in trees studies of tool manufacture, mental time travel, and social cognition in corvids, and using slow deliberate movements (14). suggest that complex cognition depends on a ‘‘tool kit’’ consisting of causal reasoning, The crows also manufacture stepped-cut flexibility, imagination, and prospection. Because corvids and apes share these cognitive Pandanus leaves (14), which are used to tools, we argue that complex cognitive abilities evolved multiple times in distantly probe for prey under leaf detritus, using a related species with vastly different brain structures in order to solve similar series of rapid back-and-forth movements or socioecological problems. slow deliberate movements that spear the prey onto the sharpened end or the barbs n Aesop_s fable, a thirsty crow spied a Do Corvids Have the Brains for of the leaf, if the prey is located in a hole. pitcher containing a small amount of Complex Cognition? These tools are consistently made to a stan- I water. Unfortunately, the water was out The crow has a brain significantly larger dardized pattern and are carried around on of reach of the crow_s bill, but next to the than would be predicted for its body size (7), foraging expeditions (15). The manufacture pitcher was a pile of stones. The crow began and it is relatively the same size as the of stepped tools appears to be lateralized at placing the stones in the pitcher, thereby chimpanzee brain. The relative size of the the population level (16) and tool use at the raising the water until it could drink. Did the forebrain in corvids is significantly larger individual level (17, 18). crow understand that its actions would than in other birds (with the exception of Observations of the crows’ tool use in the increase the water level? some parrots) (2), particularly those areas wild suggest complex cognition. For exam- Throughout folklore, the corvids (crows, thought to be analogous to the mammalian ple, there is potential cumulative evolution in jays, ravens, and jackdaws) (Fig. 1) have prefrontal cortex: the nidopallium and meso- the complexity of stepped tools (increasing been credited with intelligence. Recent pallium (Fig. 2B) (8, 9). This enlargement of the number of steps required to make a more experiments investigating the cognitive abil- the ‘‘avian prefrontal cortex’’ may reflect an complex tool), which are analogous to minor ities of corvids have begun to reveal that this increase in primate-like intelligence in cor- technological innovations in humans (19). reputation has a factual basis. These studies vids (10, 11). There are also population differences in the have found that some corvids are not only types of tools manufactured (19), seemingly superior in intelligence to birds of other avian An Overview of Corvid Cognitive independent of ecological variability, which species (perhaps with the exception of some Psychology has been suggested as a form of culture in parrots), but also rival many nonhuman To fully appreciate how corvid and ape chimpanzees (20). primates. Traditionally, studies of complex psychology are similar, it is important to de- Laboratory experiments have confirmed cognition have focused on monkeys and apes scribe how corvids may represent their phys- the sophisticated cognitive abilities of these (1). However, there is no reason to assume ical and social worlds, and how these forms crows. One of them, Betty, appears to be that complex cognition is restricted only to of mental representation may be similar or capable of reasoning by analogy with her the primates (2). Indeed, the social intelli- dissimilar to those used by apes in solving previous experience with hooks, by modify- gence hypothesis (3) states that intelligence similar problems. We use the term ‘‘under- ing nonfunctional novel material (metal evolved not to solve physical problems, but standing’’ to convey the idea that corvids wire) into hook-like shapes to retrieve food to process and use social information, such as and apes reason about a domain (physical or in a bucket inside a vertical tube (21). Fur- who is allied with whom and who is related social) in a way that transcends basic asso- thermore, she chooses the correct length or to whom, and to use this information for ciative and reinforcement processes. diameter of tool out of a ‘‘tool box’’ con- deception (4). There is evidence that some Tool use and manufacture. Tool use is taining tools of different lengths and widths other large-brained social animals, such as defined as ‘‘the use of an external object as a to reach normally inaccessible food (22, 23). cetaceans, demonstrate similar levels of in- functional extension of mouth, beak, hand, Traveling mentally in time and space. telligence as primates (5). Corvids also or claw, in the attainment of an immediate Many corvids cache food for future consump- appear to meet many of the criteria for the goal’’ (12). Although many birds, primates, tion; either a large amount of seeds cached use of social knowledge in their interactions and other animals use tools, it is not clear over a wide area, which are stored seasonal- with conspecifics (6). whether any of these species appreciate how ly, or a smaller amount of higher-quality, tools work and the forces underlying their perishable material, which is recovered hours 1Sub-Department of Animal Behaviour, University of 2 function. Perhaps the most convincing can- or days later. These differences may require Cambridge, CB3 8AA, UK. Department of Experi- mental Psychology, University of Cambridge, CB2 didates are New Caledonian crows, who different cognitive abilities for successful 3EB, UK. display extraordinary skills in making and retrieval. Clark’s nutcrackers living at high *To whom correspondence should be addressed. using tools to acquire otherwise unobtainable elevations cache up to 30,000 pine seeds E-mail: [email protected] foods. In the wild, they make two types of over a wide area and can recover them up to www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 306 10 DECEMBER 2004 1903 R EVIEW 6 months later (24). By contrast, western scrub are cached, to learn their relative decay rates, and selectively searched for the nonperish- jays living in more temperate environments in order to recover food when it is still fresh able caches (27, 28). Furthermore, they could cache fewer of a wider variety of food items and edible. Laboratory studies have capital- recall when they cached different foods in the that differ in their level of perishability and ized on the fact that western scrub jays readily same location and could even distinguish are recovered after much shorter periods (25). cache perishable foods but will not consume between two caches of the same food type Cache recovery may require more than these items when they have degraded. that had been cached at different times (29). simply remembering where their caches are When jays were allowed to cache perish- These results suggest that scrub jays remem- hidden, for species that cache many types of able and nonperishable foods, they were able ber the ‘‘what, where, and when’’ of specific food. These species may need to process to remember not only which foods they caching events (episodic-like memory). This information about the location of the cache cached where, but also how long ago they representation of the time since caching is site, the type and perishability of the cached had cached them. If a short time had elapsed essential for the efficient recovery of perish- item, and the social context of caching (26). between caching and recovery, then they able food items (25, 26). When caching perishable food, it is prudent recovered the perishable food. But if they Social cognition of cache protection and to learn something about the decay rates of had cached a long time ago, they did not pilfering. Food storers should also be sensi- the food, and if two or more perishable foods attempt to recover the rotten degraded food tive to the social context of caching, because caches are susceptible to pilfering (30). For pilferers, the ability to quickly and efficient- ly locate caches made by others may be the difference between successful pilfering and attack by the storer. A number of corvids ob- serve conspecifics caching and demonstrate excellent observational spatial memory for the location of another bird’s caches (25, 31, 32). The use of observational spatial memory as a pilfering strategy may differ between species depending on level of sociality, and as such may be an adaptive specialization (33). Do social pinyon and Mexican jays, and territorial Clark’s nutcrackers, remember where another bird has cached (31, 34)? Pinyon jays remembered the specific location of others’ caches after 1 and 2 days, as did Mexican jays, whereas Clark’s nutcrackers were only as accurate as chance after 1 day. After 2 days, the Clark’s nutcrackers were only accurate at recovering their own caches, not those they had observed. (34). This find- ing supports the adaptive specialization of the social learning hypothesis in corvids; however, another study is more ambiguous (35). Furthermore, western scrub jays are semiterritorial, and they have highly accurate observational spatial memories (25). The social context of caching may be viewed as an arms race between storers and pilferers, in which storers use counterstrat- egies to minimize the risk of having their caches pilfered (36).
Recommended publications
  • Intelligence in Corvids and Apes: a Case of Convergent Evolution? Amanda Seed*, Nathan Emery & Nicola Claytonà
    Ethology CURRENT ISSUES – PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWS Intelligence in Corvids and Apes: A Case of Convergent Evolution? Amanda Seed*, Nathan Emery & Nicola Claytonà * Department of Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany School of Biological & Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK à Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK (Invited Review) Correspondence Abstract Nicola Clayton, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Intelligence is suggested to have evolved in primates in response to com- Street, Cambridge CB23EB, UK. plexities in the environment faced by their ancestors. Corvids, a large- E-mail: [email protected] brained group of birds, have been suggested to have undergone a con- vergent evolution of intelligence [Emery & Clayton (2004) Science, Vol. Received: November 13, 2008 306, pp. 1903–1907]. Here we review evidence for the proposal from Initial acceptance: December 26, 2008 both ultimate and proximate perspectives. While we show that many of Final acceptance: February 15, 2009 (M. Taborsky) the proposed hypotheses for the evolutionary origin of great ape intelli- gence also apply to corvids, further study is needed to reveal the selec- doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01644.x tive pressures that resulted in the evolution of intelligent behaviour in both corvids and apes. For comparative proximate analyses we empha- size the need to be explicit about the level of analysis to reveal the type of convergence that has taken place. Although there is evidence that corvids and apes solve social and physical problems with similar speed and flexibility, there is a great deal more to be learned about the repre- sentations and algorithms underpinning these computations in both groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Preen Others? Predictors of Allopreening in Parrots and Corvids and Comparisons to 2 Grooming in Great Apes
    1 Why preen others? Predictors of allopreening in parrots and corvids and comparisons to 2 grooming in great apes 3 Short running title: Allopreening in parrots and corvids 4 Alejandra Morales Picard1,2, Roger Mundry3,4, Alice M. Auersperg3, Emily R. Boeving5, 5 Palmyre H. Boucherie6,7, Thomas Bugnyar8, Valérie Dufour9, Nathan J. Emery10, Ira G. 6 Federspiel8,11, Gyula K. Gajdon3, Jean-Pascal Guéry12, Matjaž Hegedič8, Lisa Horn8, Eithne 7 Kavanagh1, Megan L. Lambert1,3, Jorg J. M. Massen8,13, Michelle A. Rodrigues14, Martina 8 Schiestl15, Raoul Schwing3, Birgit Szabo8,16, Alex H. Taylor15, Jayden O. van Horik17, Auguste 9 M. P. von Bayern18, Amanda Seed19, Katie E. Slocombe1 10 1Department of Psychology, University of York, UK 11 2Montgomery College, Rockville, Maryland, USA 12 3Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Medical University 13 Vienna, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria. 14 4Platform Bioinformatics and Biostatistics, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Austria. 15 5Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA 16 6Institut Pluridisciplinaire Hubert Curien, University of Strasbourg, 23 rue Becquerel 67087 17 Strasbourg, France 18 7Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UMR 7178, 67087 Strasbourg, France 19 8Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria 20 9UMR Physiologie de la Reproduction et des Comportements, INRA-CNRS-Université de Tours 21 -IFCE, 37380 Nouzilly, France 22 10School of Biological & Chemical Sciences, Queen
    [Show full text]
  • At Natural Foraging Sites Corvus Moneduloides Tool Use by Wild New Caledonian Crows
    Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 30, 2014 Tool use by wild New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides at natural foraging sites Lucas A. Bluff, Jolyon Troscianko, Alex A. S. Weir, Alex Kacelnik and Christian Rutz Proc. R. Soc. B 2010 277, doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1953 first published online 6 January 2010 Supplementary data "Data Supplement" http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/suppl/2010/01/05/rspb.2009.1953.DC1.h tml References This article cites 23 articles, 5 of which can be accessed free http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/277/1686/1377.full.html#ref-list-1 Article cited in: http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/277/1686/1377.full.html#related-urls Subject collections Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections behaviour (1094 articles) Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top Email alerting service right-hand corner of the article or click here To subscribe to Proc. R. Soc. B go to: http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions Downloaded from rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org on January 30, 2014 Proc. R. Soc. B (2010) 277, 1377–1385 doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1953 Published online 6 January 2010 Tool use by wild New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides at natural foraging sites Lucas A. Bluff1,*,†,‡, Jolyon Troscianko2, Alex A. S. Weir1, Alex Kacelnik1 and Christian Rutz1,*,‡ 1Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK 2School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides use tools made from sticks or leaf stems to ‘fish’ woodboring beetle larvae from their burrows in decaying wood.
    [Show full text]
  • Individual Repeatability, Species Differences, and The
    Supplementary Materials: Individual repeatability, species differences, and the influence of socio-ecological factors on neophobia in 10 corvid species SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS 2 Figure S1 . Latency to touch familiar food in each round, across all conditions and species. Round 3 differs from round 1 and 2, while round 1 and 2 do not differ from each other. Points represent individuals, lines represent median. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS 3 Figure S2 . Site effect on latency to touch familiar food in azure-winged magpie, carrion crow and pinyon jay. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS 4 Table S1 Pairwise comparisons of latency data between species Estimate Standard error z p-value Blue jay - Azure-winged magpie 0.491 0.209 2.351 0.019 Carrion crow - Azure-winged magpie -0.496 0.177 -2.811 0.005 Clark’s nutcracker - Azure-winged magpie 0.518 0.203 2.558 0.011 Common raven - Azure-winged magpie -0.437 0.183 -2.392 0.017 Eurasian jay - Azure-winged magpie 0.284 0.166 1.710 0.087 ’Alal¯a- Azure-winged magpie 0.416 0.144 2.891 0.004 Large-billed crow - Azure-winged magpie 0.668 0.189 3.540 0.000 New Caledonian crow - Azure-winged magpie -0.316 0.209 -1.513 0.130 Pinyon jay - Azure-winged magpie 0.118 0.170 0.693 0.488 Carrion crow - Blue jay -0.988 0.199 -4.959 0.000 Clark’s nutcracker - Blue jay 0.027 0.223 0.122 0.903 Common raven - Blue jay -0.929 0.205 -4.537 0.000 Eurasian jay - Blue jay -0.207 0.190 -1.091 0.275 ’Alal¯a- Blue jay -0.076 0.171 -0.443 0.658 Large-billed crow - Blue jay 0.177 0.210 0.843 0.399 New Caledonian crow - Blue jay -0.808 0.228 -3.536
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptive Bill Morphology for Enhanced Tool Manipulation in New Caledonian Crows Received: 30 November 2015 Hiroshi Matsui1,*, Gavin R
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Adaptive bill morphology for enhanced tool manipulation in New Caledonian crows Received: 30 November 2015 Hiroshi Matsui1,*, Gavin R. Hunt2,*, Katja Oberhofer3, Naomichi Ogihara4, Kevin J. McGowan5, Accepted: 19 February 2016 Kumar Mithraratne3, Takeshi Yamasaki6, Russell D. Gray2,7 & Ei-Ichi Izawa1 Published: 09 March 2016 Early increased sophistication of human tools is thought to be underpinned by adaptive morphology for efficient tool manipulation. Such adaptive specialisation is unknown in nonhuman primates but may have evolved in the New Caledonian crow, which has sophisticated tool manufacture. The straightness of its bill, for example, may be adaptive for enhanced visually-directed use of tools. Here, we examine in detail the shape and internal structure of the New Caledonian crow’s bill using Principal Components Analysis and Computed Tomography within a comparative framework. We found that the bill has a combination of interrelated shape and structural features unique within Corvus, and possibly birds generally. The upper mandible is relatively deep and short with a straight cutting edge, and the lower mandible is strengthened and upturned. These novel combined attributes would be functional for (i) counteracting the unique loading patterns acting on the bill when manipulating tools, (ii) a strong precision grip to hold tools securely, and (iii) enhanced visually-guided tool use. Our findings indicate that the New Caledonian crow’s innovative bill has been adapted for tool manipulation to at least some degree. Early increased sophistication of tools may require the co-evolution of morphology that provides improved manipulatory skills. Human tool behaviour is underpinned by adaptations associated with enhanced manipulation of tools and tool material.
    [Show full text]
  • Corvidae Species Tree
    Corvidae I Red-billed Chough, Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax Pyrrhocoracinae =Pyrrhocorax Alpine Chough, Pyrrhocorax graculus Ratchet-tailed Treepie, Temnurus temnurus Temnurus Black Magpie, Platysmurus leucopterus Platysmurus Racket-tailed Treepie, Crypsirina temia Crypsirina Hooded Treepie, Crypsirina cucullata Rufous Treepie, Dendrocitta vagabunda Crypsirininae ?Sumatran Treepie, Dendrocitta occipitalis ?Bornean Treepie, Dendrocitta cinerascens Gray Treepie, Dendrocitta formosae Dendrocitta ?White-bellied Treepie, Dendrocitta leucogastra Collared Treepie, Dendrocitta frontalis ?Andaman Treepie, Dendrocitta bayleii ?Common Green-Magpie, Cissa chinensis ?Indochinese Green-Magpie, Cissa hypoleuca Cissa ?Bornean Green-Magpie, Cissa jefferyi ?Javan Green-Magpie, Cissa thalassina Cissinae ?Sri Lanka Blue-Magpie, Urocissa ornata ?White-winged Magpie, Urocissa whiteheadi Urocissa Red-billed Blue-Magpie, Urocissa erythroryncha Yellow-billed Blue-Magpie, Urocissa flavirostris Taiwan Blue-Magpie, Urocissa caerulea Azure-winged Magpie, Cyanopica cyanus Cyanopica Iberian Magpie, Cyanopica cooki Siberian Jay, Perisoreus infaustus Perisoreinae Sichuan Jay, Perisoreus internigrans Perisoreus Gray Jay, Perisoreus canadensis White-throated Jay, Cyanolyca mirabilis Dwarf Jay, Cyanolyca nanus Black-throated Jay, Cyanolyca pumilo Silvery-throated Jay, Cyanolyca argentigula Cyanolyca Azure-hooded Jay, Cyanolyca cucullata Beautiful Jay, Cyanolyca pulchra Black-collared Jay, Cyanolyca armillata Turquoise Jay, Cyanolyca turcosa White-collared Jay, Cyanolyca viridicyanus
    [Show full text]
  • Age-Related Differences in Diet and Foraging Behavior of the Critically Endangered Mariana Crow
    Age-related differences in diet and foraging behavior of the critically endangered Mariana Crow (Corvus kubaryi) Sarah K. Faegre A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science University of Washington 2014 Committee: Renee Robinette Ha Michael Beecher Program Authorized to Offer Degree: Psychology ©Copyright 2014 Sarah K. Faegre University of Washington Abstract Age-related differences in diet and foraging behavior of the critically endangered Mariana Crow (Corvus kubaryi) Sarah K. Faegre Chair of the Supervisory Committee: Renee Robinette Ha Psychology The Mariana Crow (Corvus kubaryi) is a critically endangered, island-endemic corvid whose single remaining population is on the island of Rota, CNMI. Quantitative information on the Mariana Crow’s diet is lacking and the effects of age and season on diet and foraging behavior are unknown. Most corvids are social omnivores that typically learn from adults to master complex foraging behaviors. We hypothesized that age-related differences in the diets and foraging behaviors of Mariana Crows would occur due to both a period of physical maturation and steps in learning. This study identified 619 food items taken by 36 wild crows and determined the corresponding foraging strata and substrates for 469 and 363 items respectively. Fourteen percent of food captures were plant-based and 86% were animal prey; 65% of animal prey were insects or their larvae and eggs. The study also identified 209 food items, and their corresponding forest strata and substrates, taken by two captive-reared crows during the first eleven months post-release. Comparisons of the foraging behaviors and diets of captive-reared and wild crows indicated that the released crows foraged on all important prey types taken by wild crows; however, several differences in food category proportions were noted.
    [Show full text]
  • UK-First-Breeding-Register.Pdf
    INTRODUCTION TO EDITION The original author and compiler of this record, Dave Coles has devoted a great deal of his time in creating a unique reference document which I am privileged to continue working with. By its very nature the document in a living project which will continue to grow and evolve and I can only hope and wish that I have the dedication as Dave has had in ensuring the continuity of the “First Breeding Register”. Initially all my additions and edits will be indicated in italics though this will not be used in the main body of the record (when a new record is added). Simon Matthews (2011) It is over eighty years since Emilius Hopkinson collated his RECORDS OF BIRDS BRED IN CAPTIVITY, which formed the starting point for my attempt at recording first breeding records for the UK. Since the first edition of my records was published in 1986, classification has changed several times and the present edition incorporates this up-dated classification as well as first breeding recorded since then. The research needed to produce the original volume took many years to complete and covered most avicultural literature. Since that time numerous people have kept me abreast of further breedings and pointing out those that have slipped the net. This has hopefully enabled the records to be kept up-to- date. To those I would express my thanks as indeed I do to all that have cast their eyes over the new list and passed comments, especially the ever helpful Reuben Girling who continues to pass on enquiries.
    [Show full text]
  • Do New Caledonian Crows Solve Physical Problems Through Causal Reasoning? A
    Proc. R. Soc. B (2009) 276, 247–254 doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1107 Published online 16 September 2008 Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? A. H. Taylor*, G. R. Hunt, F. S. Medina and R. D. Gray* Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland, New Zealand The extent to which animals other than humans can reason about physical problems is contentious. The benchmark test for this ability has been the trap-tube task. We presented New Caledonian crows with a series of two-trap versions of this problem. Three out of six crows solved the initial trap-tube. These crows continued to avoid the trap when the arbitrary features that had previously been associated with successful performances were removed. However, they did not avoid the trap when a hole and a functional trap were in the tube. In contrast to a recent primate study, the three crows then solved a causally equivalent but visually distinct problem—the trap-table task. The performance of the three crows across the four transfers made explanations based on chance, associative learning, visual and tactile generalization, and previous dispositions unlikely. Our findings suggest that New Caledonian crows can solve complex physical problems by reasoning both causally and analogically about causal relations. Causal and analogical reasoning may form the basis of the New Caledonian crow’s exceptional tool skills. Keywords: New Caledonian crows; causal reasoning; analogical reasoning; trap tube; trap table 1. INTRODUCTION that non-human animals use sophisticated cognition when The eighteenth century philosopher David Hume solving complex physical problems has led to suggestions (1711–1776) famously used the example of one billiard that causal reasoning in humans is fundamentally different ball rolling into another to illustrate his argument that (Penn & Povinelli 2007; Penn et al.
    [Show full text]
  • And Socio-Ecology of Wild Goffin's Cockatoos (Cacatua Goffiniana)
    Behaviour 156 (2019) 661–690 brill.com/beh Extraction without tooling around — The first comprehensive description of the foraging- and socio-ecology of wild Goffin’s cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana) M. O’Hara a,∗, B. Mioduszewska a,b, T. Haryoko c, D.M. Prawiradilaga c, L. Huber a and A. Auersperg a a Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Medical University Vienna, University of Vienna, Veterinaerplatz 1, 1210 Vienna, Austria b Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Eberhard-Gwinner-Straße, 82319 Seewiesen, Germany c Research Center for Biology, Indonesian Institute of Sciences, Jl. Raya Jakarta - Bogor Km.46 Cibinong 16911 Bogor, Indonesia *Corresponding author’s e-mail address: [email protected] Received 7 June 2018; initial decision 14 August 2018; revised 8 October 2018; accepted 9 October 2018; published online 24 October 2018 Abstract When tested under laboratory conditions, Goffin’s cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana) demonstrate numerous sophisticated cognitive skills. Most importantly, this species has shown the ability to manufacture and use tools. However, little is known about the ecology of these cockatoos, en- demic to the Tanimbar Islands in Indonesia. Here we provide first insights into the feeding- and socio-ecology of the wild Goffin’s cockatoos and propose potential links between their behaviour in natural settings and their advanced problem-solving capacities shown in captivity. Observational data suggests that Goffin’s cockatoos rely on a large variety of partially seasonal resources. Further- more, several food types require different extraction techniques. These ecological and behavioural characteristics fall in line with current hypotheses regarding the evolution of complex cognition and innovativeness.
    [Show full text]
  • Ringing & Migration
    Ringing & Migration VOLUME 24 2008 and 2009 Editor Chris Redfern Managing Editor Jacquie Clark Copy Editor John Marchant Editorial Board 2008 & 2009 Guy Anderson, Franz Bairlein, Jon Coleman, Jennifer Gill, Hugh Insley, Åke Lindström, Stephen Norman, Jane Reid, Roger Riddington and Graham Scott ISSN 0307-8698 Vol 24 Contents Ringing & Migration Contents of Vol 24, 2008 and 2009 Part 1 (June 2008) FULL PAPER Survival rates of hirundines in relation to British and African rainfall. Robert A. Robinson, Dawn E. Balmer and John H. Marchant ....................................................................................................................................................................................1 SHORT REPORTS From fledging to breeding: long-term satellite tracking of the migratory behaviour of a Lesser Black-backed Gull Larus fuscus intermedius. K. Pütz, A.J. Helbig, K.T. Pedersen, C. Rahbek, P. Saurola and R. Juvaste ....................................................7 On reading colour rings. Carl Mitchell and Mark Trinder.......................................................................................................................11 RINGING SCHEME REPORT Bird ringing in Britain and Ireland in 2006. Liz Coiffait, Jacquie A. Clark, Robert A. Robinson, Jeremy R. Blackburn, Bridget M. Griffin, Kate Risely, Mark J. Grantham, John H. Marchant, Trevor Girling and Lee Barber ..........................................15 Part 2 (December 2008) FULL PAPERS Sexual size dimorphism and moult in the Plain Swift Apus unicolor.
    [Show full text]
  • Weir Thesis.Pdf
    Cognitive psychology of tool use in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides ) Alexander Allan Scott Weir Magdalen College and Department of Zoology University of Oxford Thesis submitted for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Trinity Term 2005 Cognitive psychology of tool use in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides ) Alexander Allan Scott Weir, Magdalen College and Department of Zoology Thesis submitted for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy, Trinity Term, 2005 ABSTRACT Most studies into the evolution of humans’ manufacture and use of tools have concentrated on non-human primates. Within the past decade, however, it has been reported that wild New Caledonian crows make and use tools as complex as those of chimpanzees, and that aspects of their behaviour may be culturally transmitted. In this thesis, I present work examining the cognitive basis of New Caledonian crows’ tool-oriented behaviour. I begin by reviewing the hotly-disputed issue of whether non-human animals are capable of ‘reasoning’ in the physical domain, and examining experiments designed to address this issue. It has often been claimed that naturally-occurring tool use and manufacture indicates special cognitive abilities, so I critically analyse this argument and propose a framework that might allow the question to be tested empirically. After reviewing what is known of the ecology of New Caledonian crows, I address cognition directly, presenting results from two studies into their understanding of hooks and tool shape. I report that one subject showed remarkable innovation and flexibility by spontaneously making hooks out of wire when she needed a hooked tool, and by quickly transferring this ability to novel material requiring a different technique.
    [Show full text]