Cortical Responses to the 2F1-F2 Combination Tone Measured Indirectly Using Magnetoencephalography

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cortical Responses to the 2F1-F2 Combination Tone Measured Indirectly Using Magnetoencephalography Cortical responses to the 2f1-f2 combination tone measured indirectly using magnetoencephalography ͒ ͒ David W. Purcell,a Bernhard Ross ͑ß͒, Terence W. Picton, and Christo Pantevb Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest, 3560 Bathurst Street, Toronto, Ontario, M6A 2E1, Canada ͑Received 1 March 2007; revised 26 May 2007; accepted 29 May 2007͒ The simultaneous presentation of two tones with frequencies f1 and f2 causes the perception of several combination tones in addition to the original tones. The most prominent of these are at frequencies f2-f1 and 2f1-f2. This study measured human physiological responses to the 2f1-f2 combination tone at 500 Hz caused by tones of 750 and 1000 Hz with intensities of 65 and 55 dB SPL, respectively. Responses were measured from the cochlea using the distortion product otoacoustic emission ͑DPOAE͒, and from the auditory cortex using the 40-Hz steady-state magnetoencephalographic ͑MEG͒ response. The perceptual response was assessed by having the participant adjust a probe tone to cause maximal beating ͑“best-beats”͒ with the perceived combination tone. The cortical response to the combination tone was evaluated in two ways: first by presenting a probe tone with a frequency of 460 Hz at the perceptual best-beats level, resulting in a 40-Hz response because of interaction with the combination tone at 500 Hz, and second by simultaneously presenting two f1 and f2 pairs that caused combination tones that would themselves ͑ ͒ beat at 40 Hz. The 2f1-f2 DPOAE in the external auditory canal had a level of 2.6 s.d. 12.1 dB SPL. The 40-Hz MEG response in the contralateral cortex had a magnitude of 0.39 ͑s.d. 0.1͒ nA m. The perceived level of the combination tone was 44.8 ͑s.d. 11.3͒ dB SPL. There were no significant correlations between these measurements. These results indicate that physiological responses to the 2f1-f2 combination tone occur in the human auditory system all the way from the cochlea to the primary auditory cortex. The perceived magnitude of the combination tone is not determined by the measured physiological response at either the cochlea or the cortex. © 2007 Acoustical Society of America. ͓DOI: 10.1121/1.2751250͔ PACS number͑s͒: 43.64.Ri, 43.64.Jb, 43.64.Qh, 43.66.Ki ͓BLM͔ Pages: 992–1003 ͒ I. INTRODUCTION Goldstein, 1966; Smoorenburg, 1972a, b . The 2f1-f2 nonlin- earity is termed “essential” because it occurs at low stimulus The mammalian cochlea contains nonlinearities that levels ͑Goldstein, 1966͒. This is unlike a mainly linear sys- generate frequencies not present in the stimulus. The sim- tem distorting only at higher levels ͑von Helmholtz, 1877/ ͒ plest acoustic stimulus that can be used to study this phe- 1954; Goldstein, 1966 . The level and phase of the 2f1-f2 nomena is composed of two pure tones with frequencies f1 tone within the cochlea can be estimated by presenting an Ͼ and f2, where f2 f1. In response, the cochlea produces a set additional “cancellation” tone of the same frequency of combination tones, or distortion products, of which the ͑Zwicker, 1955; Goldstein, 1966, 1969͒. Perception of the cubic 2f1-f2 and the quadratic f2-f1 are the most prominent combination tone can be eliminated by carefully adjusting and most studied. the magnitude and phase of the cancellation tone until noth- With favorable stimulus conditions, combination tones ing is heard. Goldstein ͑1966͒ facilitated the subject’s ability are audible. The Italian violinist Tartini knew of their exis- to hear the cancellation effect by adding an additional probe tence as early as 1714 ͑Jones, 1935͒. As Jones ͑1935͒ re- tone slightly mistuned from the cancellation tone ͑e.g., ports, by 1801 it was known that the tone produced by a 4Hz͒, of sufficient level to beat with the combination tone. 32-ft pipe in a cathedral organ ͑about 16 Hz͒ could be gen- The cancellation tone was then adjusted to eliminate the erated in the ears of listeners by simultaneously activating a beating sensation by canceling the combination tone. Using 16-ft pipe ͑about 32 Hz͒ and an even shorter pipe producing the cancellation technique, the cochlear 2f1-f2 combination ͑ ͒ its fifth about 48 Hz through the 2f1-f2 combination tone, tone has an apparent level of an externally supplied tone that thus dispensing with the need for 32-ft pipes. is 20–40 dB below that of equal level primaries in the ex- ͑ The 2f1-f2 combination tone has been extensively stud- ternal ear canal where tone pair stimuli f2 and f1 have equal ied psychoacoustically ͑e.g., Zwicker, 1955; Plomp, 1965; levels L2 =L1; Goldstein, 1966; Smoorenburg, 1972b; Hall, 1975͒. Cancellation methods may overestimate the level of the combination tone for small f2 / f1 due to suppression ef- a͒ ͑ Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; Electronic mail: fects of the stimulus f1 Smoorenburg, 1972b; Shannon and [email protected]. Currently affiliated with the National Centre for Au- Houtgast, 1980; Zwicker, 1981͒. diology at the University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, N6G 1H1, The psychoacoustically estimated levels of the combina- Canada. b͒ ͑ Currently affiliated with the Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosig- tion tones with respect to the stimuli i.e., 20–40 dB below nalanalysis at the University of Münster, Münster, Germany. the primaries͒ are similar to the levels of distortion products 992 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 122 ͑2͒, August 20070001-4966/2007/122͑2͒/992/12/$23.00 © 2007 Acoustical Society of America measured in the mechanical motion of the basilar membrane is impossible to use time-domain measures such as the ͑Robles et al., 1997͒, the cochlear microphonic ͑Gibian and evoked N1 or sustained response. In contrast, steady-state Kim, 1982͒, receptor potentials of IHCs ͑Nuttall and Dolan, frequency-domain measurements of auditory evoked poten- 1990͒, and single fibers of the auditory nerve ͑Goldstein and tials and magnetic fields can separate the responses to the Kiang, 1968; Buunen and Rhode, 1978; Kim et al., 1980͒. stimuli and to the combination tones because they are at The response can be eliminated from recordings of nerve different frequencies. Unfortunately, steady-state responses fibers using cancellation techniques ͑Goldstein and Kiang, above 100 Hz derive mainly from brainstem sources ͑Herd- 1968; Goldstein et al., 1978͒. These results indicated that the man et al., 2002; Purcell et al., 2004͒, and combination tones 2f1-f2 combination tone is present in the cochlea, and sug- are most relevant at frequencies higher than 100 Hz. Steady- gested that it was processed at the same characteristic place state measurements cannot therefore directly measure the re- as an externally supplied tone of equal frequency. The 2f1-f2 sponse to the 2f1-f2 combination tone in cortex. To over- combination tone was later observed directly in the mechani- come these constraints, an approach based on beating used in cal motion of the basilar membrane at its characteristic place psychoacoustics was adopted ͑Goldstein, 1966; Furst et al., ͑Robles et al., 1991, 1997͒. 1988, unpublished͒.. Two pure tones that are similar in fre- The distortion product otoacoustic emission ͑DPOAE͒ at quency and magnitude will cause beating at the difference frequency 2f1-f2 is initially generated at the f2 characteristic frequency. Therefore, cortical correlates of the 2f1-f2 combi- place where the two stimulus tones interact most promi- nation tone could be measured indirectly by causing a 40-Hz nently ͑Talmadge et al., 1999; Shera and Guinan, 1999; beat either between a combination tone and an externally Knight and Kemp, 2000͒. Signals at the DPOAE frequency presented probe tone, or between two combination tones. then propagate on the basilar membrane both basally and The sensation of beating between a 2f1-f2 combination tone apically. The apical propagation will reach the characteristic and an externally supplied tone can be perceived relatively place for frequencies equal to 2f1-f2. The DPOAE measured easily by naïve participants with little training. The 40-Hz in the ear canal can be dominated by energy from either the steady-state magnetic fields generated in the auditory corti- ͑ f2 or the 2f1-f2 place depending on the stimulus parameters ces Mäkelä and Hari, 1987; Pantev et al., 1996; Gutschalk ͑Shera and Guinan, 1999; Knight and Kemp, 2000, 2001; et al., 1999; Schoonhoven et al., 2003͒ by these beats can be Dhar et al., 2005͒. Wilson ͑1980͒ found significant discrep- measured using MEG. ancies between the levels of the ear canal DPOAEs and psy- This indirect method of obtaining correlates of the choacoustic cancellation levels in the same subjects. These 2f1-f2 combination tone measures physiological responses to may have been in part due to changes in dominant DPOAE the difference-frequency type combination tone or “beat.” sources across stimulus conditions. However, Furst et al. Cochlear microphonic ͑Gibian and Kim, 1982; Cheatham ͑1988͒ and Zwicker and Harris ͑1990͒ also reported that and Dallos, 1997͒, IHC ͑Cheatham and Dallos, 1997͒, and DPOAE levels in the canal were low compared to those es- single neuron studies ͑Smoorenburg et al., 1976; Kim et al., timated psychoacoustically in the same subjects. Further- 1980͒ have shown that both cubic and quadratic combination more, the finding that psychoacoustic cancellation does not tones travel as waves on the basilar membrane from their eliminate the ear canal DPOAE highlights the fact that dif- initiation place where the stimuli interact at their own char- ferent processes are involved in the generation of the acteristic places. They are also present in nerve fiber record- DPOAE and the perception of the combination tone ͑Furst et ings both near the stimulus interaction site and at their own al., 1988; Zwicker and Harris, 1990͒.
Recommended publications
  • Comparison of Procedures for Determination of Acoustic Nonlinearity of Some Inhomogeneous Materials
    Downloaded from orbit.dtu.dk on: Dec 17, 2017 Comparison of procedures for determination of acoustic nonlinearity of some inhomogeneous materials Jensen, Leif Bjørnø Published in: Acoustical Society of America. Journal Link to article, DOI: 10.1121/1.2020879 Publication date: 1983 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link back to DTU Orbit Citation (APA): Jensen, L. B. (1983). Comparison of procedures for determination of acoustic nonlinearity of some inhomogeneous materials. Acoustical Society of America. Journal, 74(S1), S27-S27. DOI: 10.1121/1.2020879 General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. PROGRAM OF The 106thMeeting of the AcousticalSociety of America Town and CountryHotel © San Diego, California © 7-11 November1983 TUESDAY MORNING, 8 NOVEMBER 1983 SENATE/COMMITTEEROOMS, 8:30 A.M. TO 12:10P.M. Session A. Underwater Acoustics: Arctic Acoustics I William Mosely,Chairman Naval ResearchLaboratory, Washington, DC 20375 Chairman's Introductions8:30 Invited Papers 8:35 A1.
    [Show full text]
  • Tutorial #5 Solutions
    Name: Other group members: Tutorial #5 Solutions PHYS 1240: Sound and Music Friday, July 19, 2019 Instructions: Work in groups of 3 or 4 to answer the following questions. Write your solu- tions on this copy of the tutorial|each person should have their own copy, but make sure you agree on everything as a group. When you're finished, keep this copy of your tutorial for reference|no need to turn it in (grades are based on participation, not accuracy). 1. When telephones were first being developed by Bell Labs, they realized it would not be feasible to have them detect all frequencies over the human audible range (20 Hz - 20 kHz). Instead, they defined a frequency response so that the highest-pitched sounds telephones could receive is 3400 Hz and lowest-pitched sounds they could detect are at 340 Hz, which saved on costs considerably. a) Assume the temperature in air is 15◦C. How large are the biggest and smallest wavelengths of sound in air that telephones are able to receive? We can find the wavelength from the formula v = λf, but first we'll need the speed: v = 331 + (0:6 × 15◦C) = 340 m/s Now, we find the wavelengths for the two ends of the frequency range given: v 340 m/s λ = = = 0.1 m smallest f 3400 Hz v 340 m/s λ = = = 1 m largest f 340 Hz Therefore, perhaps surprisingly, telephones can pick up sound waves up to a meter long, but they won't detect waves as small as the ear receiver itself.
    [Show full text]
  • L Atdment OFFICF QO9ENT ROOM 36
    *;JiQYL~dW~llbk~ieira - - ~-- -, - ., · LAtDMENT OFFICF QO9ENT ROOM 36 ?ESEARC L0ORATORY OF RL C.f:'__ . /j16baV"BLI:1!S INSTITUTE 0i'7Cn' / PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL INTERVALS: EVIDENCE FOR THE CENTRAL ORIGIN OF THE PITCH OF COMPLEX TONES ADRIANUS J. M. HOUTSMA JULIUS L. GOLDSTEIN LO N OPY TECHNICAL REPORT 484 OCTOBER I, 1971 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH LABORATORY OF ELECTRONICS CAMBRIDGE, MASSACHUSETTS 02139 The Research Laboratory of Electronics is an interdepartmental laboratory in which faculty members and graduate students from numerous academic departments conduct research. The research reported in this document was made possible in part by support extended the Massachusetts Institute of Tech- nology, Research Laboratory of Electronics, by the JOINT SER- VICES ELECTRONICS PROGRAMS (U. S. Army, U. S. Navy, and U.S. Air Force) under Contract No. DAAB07-71-C-0300, and by the National Institutes of Health (Grant 5 PO1 GM14940-05). Requestors having DOD contracts or grants should apply for copies of technical reports to the Defense Documentation Center, Cameron Station, Alexandria, Virginia 22314; all others should apply to the Clearinghouse for Federal Scientific and Technical Information, Sills Building, 5285 Port Royal Road, Springfield, Virginia 22151. THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN APPROVED FOR PUBLIC RELEASE AND SALE; ITS DISTRIBUTION IS UNLIMITED. MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH LABORATORY OF ELECTRONICS Technical Report 484 October 1, 1971 PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL INTERVALS: EVIDENCE FOR THE CENTRAL ORIGIN OF COMPLEX TONES Adrianus J. M. Houtsma and Julius L. Goldstein This report is based on a thesis by A. J. M. Houtsma submitted to the Department of Electrical Engineering at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, June 1971, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Large Scale Sound Installation Design: Psychoacoustic Stimulation
    LARGE SCALE SOUND INSTALLATION DESIGN: PSYCHOACOUSTIC STIMULATION An Interactive Qualifying Project Report submitted to the Faculty of the WORCESTER POLYTECHNIC INSTITUTE in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Science by Taylor H. Andrews, CS 2012 Mark E. Hayden, ECE 2012 Date: 16 December 2010 Professor Frederick W. Bianchi, Advisor Abstract The brain performs a vast amount of processing to translate the raw frequency content of incoming acoustic stimuli into the perceptual equivalent. Psychoacoustic processing can result in pitches and beats being “heard” that do not physically exist in the medium. These psychoac- oustic effects were researched and then applied in a large scale sound design. The constructed installations and acoustic stimuli were designed specifically to combat sensory atrophy by exer- cising and reinforcing the listeners’ perceptual skills. i Table of Contents Abstract ............................................................................................................................................ i Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ ii Table of Figures ............................................................................................................................. iii Table of Tables .............................................................................................................................. iv Chapter 1: Introduction .................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Combination Tones and Other Related Auditory Phenomena
    t he university ot cbtcago ro m a n wyo ur: no c xxuu.“ COMBINATION TONES AND OTHER RELATED AUDITORY PHENOMENA A DI SSERTA TION SUBMITTED TO THE FA CULTY OF TH E GRADUA TE SCHOOL OF A RTS AND LITERATURE IN CANDIDA CY FO R THE DEG REE OF DOCTOR OF PHI LOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF PSY CHOLOGY BY JOSEPH PETERSON u rr N o o r r un Psvc a o wc xcu. Rm " wa Sm u n . Pvumun AS “o uo c 39 , 1 908. PREFA CE . The first part of this mo no graph is devo ted primarily to a critical exposition of the important theories of combination to n and t t nt e n es a s a eme of th facts upo which they rest . This undertaking i nevitably leads to the mention of a considerable number of closely related phenomena whose significance for I e general theo ry is often crucial . n view of th conditions l n in the t t the t it n th t prevai i g li era ure of subjec , has bee ough expedient that this presentation should in the main follow h n n . n c nt nts c ro ological li es The full a alyt ical table of o e , t t t the n int n n oge her wi h divisio o sect io s , will readily e able he x readers who so desire to consult t te t o n special topics . The second part of the monograph report s cert ain experimcnta l t n t observa io s made by the au hor o n summation tones .
    [Show full text]
  • Listening to String Sound: a Pedagogical Approach To
    LISTENING TO STRING SOUND: A PEDAGOGICAL APPROACH TO EXPLORING THE COMPLEXITIES OF VIOLA TONE PRODUCTION by MARIA KINDT (Under the Direction of Maggie Snyder) ABSTRACT String tone acoustics is a topic that has been largely overlooked in pedagogical settings. This document aims to illuminate the benefits of a general knowledge of practical acoustic science to inform teaching and performance practice. With an emphasis on viola tone production, the document introduces aspects of current physical science and psychoacoustics, combined with established pedagogy to help students and teachers gain a richer and more comprehensive view into aspects of tone production. The document serves as a guide to demonstrate areas where knowledge of the practical science can improve on playing technique and listening skills. The document is divided into three main sections and is framed in a way that is useful for beginning, intermediate, and advanced string students. The first section introduces basic principles of sound, further delving into complex string tone and the mechanism of the violin and viola. The second section focuses on psychoacoustics and how it relates to the interpretation of string sound. The third section covers some of the pedagogical applications of the practical science in performance practice. A sampling of spectral analysis throughout the document demonstrates visually some of the relevant topics. Exercises for informing intonation practices utilizing combination tones are also included. INDEX WORDS: string tone acoustics, psychoacoustics,
    [Show full text]
  • COMBINATION TONES in VIOLINS Angela Lohri, Sandra Carral And
    Proceedings of the Second Vienna Talk, Sept. 19−21, 2010, University of Music and Performing Arts Vienna, Austria COMBINATION TONES IN VIOLINS Angela Lohri, Sandra Carral and Vasileios Chatziioannou Institute of Musical Acoustics and International Centre of Harmonics University of Music and Performing Arts Vienna, Austria [email protected] ABSTRACT two different instruments, generated a third tone, in Tartini’s In this study we investigate the appearance of combination words “terzo suono”. He observed that, lying lower than the tones in violins. Most modern textbooks emphasise that played interval, the third tone acted like a bass giving the combination tones occur inside the ear exclusively (intra- interval a third dimension, a subtle harmonic context.[1], pp. 13- aural). In this study this assumption will be subjected to 17. Tartini, not only a genius musician but also a genius thinker, scrutiny based on evidence found in an empirical study, in wrote down the detected correlations between mathematics and which combination tones were measured outside the ear the third tones in his book Trattato di musica secondo la vera (extra-aural). scienza dell’armonia (“Treatise on Music according to the True Science of Harmony”). His insights about the principles of the An experiment was performed in which a violinist played third tone had a significant influence on the development of two tones of a particular musical interval simultaneously. music theory. Tartini’s discovery opened up entirely new This was recorded and subsequently analysed using a possibilities of how sound quality can be understood and Fourier Transformation. In addition to the partial tones of realized by practising musicians.
    [Show full text]
  • A. Acoustic Theory and Modeling of the Vocal Tract
    A. Acoustic Theory and Modeling of the Vocal Tract by H.W. Strube, Drittes Physikalisches Institut, Universität Göttingen A.l Introduction This appendix is intended for those readers who want to inform themselves about the mathematical treatment of the vocal-tract acoustics and about its modeling in the time and frequency domains. Apart from providing a funda­ mental understanding, this is required for all applications and investigations concerned with the relationship between geometric and acoustic properties of the vocal tract, such as articulatory synthesis, determination of the tract shape from acoustic quantities, inverse filtering, etc. Historically, the formants of speech were conjectured to be resonances of cavities in the vocal tract. In the case of a narrow constriction at or near the lips, such as for the vowel [uJ, the volume of the tract can be considered a Helmholtz resonator (the glottis is assumed almost closed). However, this can only explain the first formant. Also, the constriction - if any - is usu­ ally situated farther back. Then the tract may be roughly approximated as a cascade of two resonators, accounting for two formants. But all these approx­ imations by discrete cavities proved unrealistic. Thus researchers have have now adopted a more reasonable description of the vocal tract as a nonuni­ form acoustical transmission line. This can explain an infinite number of res­ onances, of which, however, only the first 2 to 4 are of phonetic importance. Depending on the kind of sound, the tube system has different topology: • for vowel-like sounds, pharynx and mouth form one tube; • for nasalized vowels, the tube is branched, with transmission from pharynx through mouth and nose; • for nasal consonants, transmission is through pharynx and nose, with the closed mouth tract as a "shunt" line.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ear Tone Toolbox for Auditory Distortion Product Synthesis
    The Ear Tone Toolbox for Auditory Distortion Product Synthesis Alex Chechile CCRMA, Stanford University [email protected] ABSTRACT nerve. At the same time, outer hair cells receive electric signals from the brainstem and mechanically vibrate at The Ear Tone Toolbox is a collection of open-source unit the frequencies of the sound [4]. This electromotility generators for the production of auditory distortion mechanically increases stimulus-specific vibrations on product synthesis. Auditory distortion products are the basilar membrane, resulting in an increase of hearing sounds generated along the basilar membrane in the sensitivity and frequency selectivity when transmitted to cochlea in response to specific pure-tone frequency the inner hair cells [5, 6]. combinations. The frequencies of the distortion products However, the outer hair cell movement does not occur are separate from the provoking stimulus tones and are exclusively at stimulus frequencies, but is somewhat not present in the acoustic space. Until the release of the irregular, thus making its frequency response nonlinear, Ear Tone Toolbox, music software for the synthesis of extending to an audible range [4]. This nonlinear active auditory distortion products has not been widely process increases basilar membrane movement, which available. This first release is a collection of six externals aids the loss of energy from damping, while an excess of for Max, VST plugins, and patches for the hardware the generated energy causes additional vibrations that OWL synthesizer, all of which produce various travel backwards from the basilar membrane to the combinations of distortion products and acoustic primary middle ear and the ear canal and creates what is known as tones.
    [Show full text]
  • Correlation Between the Frequencies of Harmonic Partials Generated from Selected Fundamentals and Pitches of Supplied Tones
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 1962 Correlation between the frequencies of harmonic partials generated from selected fundamentals and pitches of supplied tones Chester Orlan Strom The University of Montana Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Strom, Chester Orlan, "Correlation between the frequencies of harmonic partials generated from selected fundamentals and pitches of supplied tones" (1962). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 1913. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/1913 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. CORRELATION BETWEEN THE FREQUENCIES OF HARMONIC PARTIALS GENERATED FROM SELECTED FUNDAMENTALS AND PITCHES OF SUPPLIED TONES by CHESTER ORLAN STROM BoMo Montana State University, 1960 Presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Music MONTANA STATE UNIVERSITY 1962 Approved by: Chairman, Board of Examine Dean, Graduate School JUL 3 1 1902 Date UMI Number: EP35290 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.
    [Show full text]
  • The Sounds of Music : Science of Musical Scales
    GENERAL ARTICLE The Sounds of Music : Science of Musical Scales I : Human Perception of Sound Sushan Konar Both, human appreciation of music and musical genres, tran- scend time and space. The universality of musical genres and associated musical scales is intimately linked to the physics of sound and the special characteristics of human acoustic sensitivity. In this series of articles, we examine the science underlying the development of the heptatonic scale, one of the most prevalent scales of the modern musical genres, both western and Indian. Sushan Konar works on stellar compact objects. She Introduction also writes popular science articles and maintains a Fossil records indicate that the appreciation of music goes back weekly astrophysics-related to the dawn of human sentience and some of the musical scales in blog called ‘Monday Musings’. use today could also be as ancient. This universality of musical scales likely owes its existence to an amazing circularity (or peri- odicity) inherent in human sensitivity to sound frequencies. Most musical scales are specific to a particular genre of music and there exists quite a number of them. However, the ‘heptatonic’ (having seven base notes) scale happen to have a dominating presence in the world music scene today. It is interesting to see how this has more to do with the physics of sound and the physiology of hu- man auditory perception than history. We shall devote this first article in the series to understand the specialities of human re- sponse to acoustic frequencies. Keywords arXiv:1908.07940v1 [physics.pop-ph] 13 Aug 2019 string vibration, beat frequencies, Human ear is a remarkable organ in many ways.
    [Show full text]
  • Maleti∆ Han®Ek 225-232
    M. MALETI∆ - B. HANÆEK - M. ROGINA: V. DVORˇ ÁK SLIDING TONES, IRASM 37 (2006) 2, 225-232 225 VINKO DVORÁKˇ SLIDING TONES: DO THEY EXIST? MLADEN MALETI∆ UDC: 781.1 Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing Original Scientific Paper University of Zagreb Izvorni znanstveni rad Unska 3a Received: September 14, 2006 Primljeno: 14. rujna 2006. 10000 ZAGREB, Croatia Accepted: September 30, 2006 E-mail: [email protected] PrihvaÊeno: 30. rujna 2006. BRANKO HANÆEK Institute for History and Philosophy of Science Croatian Academy of Sciences and Arts A. KovaËiÊa 5 10000 ZAGREB, Croatia E-mail: [email protected] MLADEN ROGINA Department of Mathematics University of Zagreb BijeniËka cesta 30 10000 ZAGREB, Croatia E-mail: [email protected] Abstract — Résumé We are analysing variations (sliding tones) form with the original signal. There is no am- and combination tones in bird (thrush) song, plitude modulation. Analysis of the records of following the early work of Vinko Dvorˇák. He the bird song does not show any unexpected was a research pioneer in determination of the tone at normal loudness, contrary to early ex- critical bands of hearing, experimenting with the periments where very high intensities were pitch of combination tones with frequency dif- used, causing post experimental TTS and ference between the highest and lowest tones. tinnitus. Instantaneous frequency monitoring was real- Key Words: sliding tones; variation tones; ised after a spectrogram done with SFSWin, and combination tones; thrush; pipe; resonator; the signal from the bird song record was then spectrogram; instantaneous amplitude; in- transformed with the Hilbert transform. An ana- stantaneous phase; instantaneous frequency; lytical signal comes from combining this trans- Hilbert transform; critical band 226 M.
    [Show full text]