Nonlinear Dynamics of the Perceived Pitch of Complex Sounds

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Nonlinear Dynamics of the Perceived Pitch of Complex Sounds Nonlinear Dynamics of the Perceived Pitch of Complex Sounds Julyan H. E. Cartwright1,∗, Diego L. Gonz´alez2,†, and Oreste Piro3,‡ 1Instituto Andaluz de Ciencias de la Tierra, IACT (CSIC-UGR), E-18071 Granada, Spain. 2Istituto Lamel, CNR, I-40129 Bologna, Italy 3Institut Mediterrani d’Estudis Avanc¸ats, IMEDEA (CSIC–UIB), E-07071 Palma de Mallorca, Spain (Physical Review Letters, 82, 5389–5392, 1999) We apply results from nonlinear dynamics to an old problem in acoustical physics: the mechanism of the perception of the pitch of sounds, especially the sounds known as complex tones that are important for music and speech intelligibility. PACS numbers: 05.45.-a, 43.66.+y, 87.19.La The pitch of a sound is where we perceive it to lie on a (Fig. 1b), the difference combination tone ωC = ω2 − ω1 musical scale. Like all sensations, pitch is a subjective quan- (i.e., p = 1, q = −1) between two successive partials has tity related to physical attributes of the stimulus, in this case the frequency of the missing fundamental ω0, that is ωC = mainly its component frequencies. For a pure tone with a sin- (k + 1)ω0 − kω0 = ω0. However, a crucial experiment seri- gle frequency component, the relation is monotonic and en- ously challenged nonlinear theories of the residue. Schouten ables us to adopt an operational definition of pitch in terms et al. [4] demonstrated that the behavior of the residue cannot of frequency: the pitch of an arbitrary sound is given by the be described by a difference combination tone: if we shift all frequency of a pure tone of the same pitch. The scientific in- the partials by the same amount ∆ω (Fig. 1c), the difference vestigation of pitch has a long history dating back to Pythago- combination tone remains unchanged, and the same should ras, but the origin of the pitch of complex tones with several thus be true of the residue. Instead it is found that the per- frequency components is still not well understood. The first ceived pitch also shifts, showing a linear dependence on ∆ω perceptual theories considered pitch to arise at a peripheral (Fig. 1d). This phenomenon is known as the first pitch shift level in the auditory system [1–5]; more recently it has been effect, and has been accurately measured in many experiments thought that central nervous system processing is necessary (psychoacoustic experiments on pitch can attain an accuracy [6–8]. However, the experimental evidence is that this pro- of 0.2% [13]). A first attempt to model qualitatively the be- cessing is carried out before the primary auditory cortex [9]. havior of the pitch shift shows that the slopes of the lines in The latest models integrate neural and peripheral processing Fig. 1d depend roughly on the inverse of the harmonic num- [10,11]. Here we develop a nonlinear theory of pitch percep- ber k +1 of the central partial of a three-component complex tion for complex tones that describes experimental results on tone. However, for small k at least, the change in slope is the pitch perception of complex sounds at least as well as do slightly but consistently larger than this, but smaller if we re- current models, and that removes the need for extensive pro- place k +1 by k. This behavior is known as the second pitch cessing at higher levels of the auditory system. shift effect. Also, an enlargement of the spacing between par- A key phenomenon in pitch perception is known as the tials while maintaining fixed the central frequency produces problem of the missing fundamental, virtual pitch, or residue a decrease in the residue pitch. As this anomalous behavior perception [12], and consists of the perception of a pitch that seems to be correlated with the second pitch shift effect, it cannot be mapped to any frequency component of the stimu- is usually included within it [4]. Pitch-shift experiments, and lus. Suppose that a periodic tone such as that shown in Fig. 1a others that demonstrated that the residue is elicited dichoti- arXiv:chao-dyn/9907002v1 26 Jun 1999 is presented to the ear. Its pitch is perceived to be that of a cally (part of the stimulus exciting one ear and the rest of the pure tone at the frequency of the fundamental. The number stimulus the other, controlateral, ear) and not just monotically of higher harmonics and their relative amplitudes give tim- and diotically (all of the stimulus exciting one or both ears) bral characteristics to the sound, which allow one to distin- led to the abandonment of peripheral (periodicity [2,4] and guish a trumpet from a violin playing the same musical note. place [1,3]) theories and to the development of theories that Now suppose that the fundamental and some of the first few considered the pitch of complex tones to be a result of central higher harmonics are removed (Fig. 1b). Although the timbre nervous system processing [6–8]; thence to integrated neural changes, the pitch of the tone remains unchanged and equal to and peripheral models [10,11]. the missing fundamental; this is residue perception. We demonstrate below that the crucial pitch-shift experi- The first physical theory for the residue is due to von ment of Schouten et al. can be accurately described in terms Helmholtz [3], who attributed it to the generation of differ- of generic attractors of nonlinear dynamical systems, such that ence combination tones in the nonlinearities of the ear. A a theory of pitch perception for complex tones can be con- passive nonlinearity fed by two sources with frequencies ω1 structed without the need to resort to extensive central pro- and ω2 generates combination tones of frequency ωC , which cessing. We model the auditory system as a generic nonlinear are nontrivial solutions of the equation pω1 + qω2 + ωC =0, forced oscillator, and identify experimental data with struc- where p and q are integers. For a harmonic complex tone turally stable behavior of this class of dynamical system. We 1 emphasize that since we are interested in universal behavior, Since complex tones can be decomposed as a series of the results we obtain are not dependent on the construction partials — a sum of purely sinusoidal components — and of a particular model, but rather represent the behavior of any residue perception is elicited with at least two of these, we dynamical system of this type. search for suitable attractors in the class of two-frequency quasiperiodically forced oscillators. Quasiperiodically forced oscillators show a great variety of qualitative behavior that falls into the three categories of periodic attractors, quasiperi- odic attractors, and strange attractors (both chaotic and non- Temporal Fourier Pitch Waveform Spectrum chaotic). We propose that the residue behavior we seek to 1 explain is a resonant response to forcing the auditory system a 2 quasiperiodically. From stability arguments [14–17] we sin- 3 4 5 P=ω gle out a particulartype of two-frequencyquasiperiodicattrac- 6 7 0 8 tor, which we term a three-frequencyresonance, as the natural ω0 candidate for modelling the residue. Three-frequency reso- 4 b 5 nances are given by the nontrivial solutions of the equation 6 7 pω1+qω2+rω =0, where p, q, and r are integers, ω1 and ω2 8 P=ω R 0 are the forcing frequencies, and ωR is the resonant response, and can be written compactly in the form (p,q,r). Notice that ω 0 combination tones are three-frequency resonances of the re- c stricted class (p, q, 1). This is the only type of response possi- P=ω +∆P ble from a passive nonlinearity, whereas a dynamical system 0 such as a forced oscillator is an active nonlinearity with at ω least one intrinsic frequency, and can exhibit the full panoply 0 ∆ω of three-frequency resonances, which include subharmonics d k=6 k=7 k=8 P of combination tones. The investigation of three-frequency systems is a young area of research, and there is not yet any ∆ ∆P ∝∆ω P consistency in the nomenclatureof these resonancesin the sci- ω0 ∆ω entific literature: as well as three-frequency resonances, they are also called weak resonances or partial mode lockings; see ω ω ω f 70 8 0 9 0 Baesens et al. [18] and references therein. It is known that FIG. 1. Fourier spectra and pitch of complex tones. Whereas three-frequency resonances form an extensive web in the pa- pure tones have a sinusoidal waveform corresponding to a single fre- rameter space of a dynamical system. In particular, between quency, almost all musical sounds are complex tones that consist of any two parent resonances (p1, q1, r1) and (p2, q2, r2) lies the a lowest frequency component, or fundamental, together with higher daughter resonance (p1 + p2, q1 + q2, r1 + r2) on the straight frequency overtones. The fundamental plus overtones are together line in parameter space connecting the parents. collectively called partials. a A harmonic complex tone. The over- For pitch-shift experiments, the vicinity of the external fre- tones are successive integer multiples k = 2, 3, 4 . of the funda- quencies to successive multiples of some missing fundamen- mental ω0 that determines the pitch. The partials of a harmonic com- tal ensures that (k + 1)/k is a good rational approximation to plex tone are termed harmonics. b Another harmonic complex tone. their frequency ratio. Hence we concentrate on a small inter- The fundamental and the first few higher harmonics have been re- val around the missing fundamental between the frequencies moved. The pitch remains the same and equal to the missing fun- damental.
Recommended publications
  • Comparison of Procedures for Determination of Acoustic Nonlinearity of Some Inhomogeneous Materials
    Downloaded from orbit.dtu.dk on: Dec 17, 2017 Comparison of procedures for determination of acoustic nonlinearity of some inhomogeneous materials Jensen, Leif Bjørnø Published in: Acoustical Society of America. Journal Link to article, DOI: 10.1121/1.2020879 Publication date: 1983 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link back to DTU Orbit Citation (APA): Jensen, L. B. (1983). Comparison of procedures for determination of acoustic nonlinearity of some inhomogeneous materials. Acoustical Society of America. Journal, 74(S1), S27-S27. DOI: 10.1121/1.2020879 General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. PROGRAM OF The 106thMeeting of the AcousticalSociety of America Town and CountryHotel © San Diego, California © 7-11 November1983 TUESDAY MORNING, 8 NOVEMBER 1983 SENATE/COMMITTEEROOMS, 8:30 A.M. TO 12:10P.M. Session A. Underwater Acoustics: Arctic Acoustics I William Mosely,Chairman Naval ResearchLaboratory, Washington, DC 20375 Chairman's Introductions8:30 Invited Papers 8:35 A1.
    [Show full text]
  • Tutorial #5 Solutions
    Name: Other group members: Tutorial #5 Solutions PHYS 1240: Sound and Music Friday, July 19, 2019 Instructions: Work in groups of 3 or 4 to answer the following questions. Write your solu- tions on this copy of the tutorial|each person should have their own copy, but make sure you agree on everything as a group. When you're finished, keep this copy of your tutorial for reference|no need to turn it in (grades are based on participation, not accuracy). 1. When telephones were first being developed by Bell Labs, they realized it would not be feasible to have them detect all frequencies over the human audible range (20 Hz - 20 kHz). Instead, they defined a frequency response so that the highest-pitched sounds telephones could receive is 3400 Hz and lowest-pitched sounds they could detect are at 340 Hz, which saved on costs considerably. a) Assume the temperature in air is 15◦C. How large are the biggest and smallest wavelengths of sound in air that telephones are able to receive? We can find the wavelength from the formula v = λf, but first we'll need the speed: v = 331 + (0:6 × 15◦C) = 340 m/s Now, we find the wavelengths for the two ends of the frequency range given: v 340 m/s λ = = = 0.1 m smallest f 3400 Hz v 340 m/s λ = = = 1 m largest f 340 Hz Therefore, perhaps surprisingly, telephones can pick up sound waves up to a meter long, but they won't detect waves as small as the ear receiver itself.
    [Show full text]
  • L Atdment OFFICF QO9ENT ROOM 36
    *;JiQYL~dW~llbk~ieira - - ~-- -, - ., · LAtDMENT OFFICF QO9ENT ROOM 36 ?ESEARC L0ORATORY OF RL C.f:'__ . /j16baV"BLI:1!S INSTITUTE 0i'7Cn' / PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL INTERVALS: EVIDENCE FOR THE CENTRAL ORIGIN OF THE PITCH OF COMPLEX TONES ADRIANUS J. M. HOUTSMA JULIUS L. GOLDSTEIN LO N OPY TECHNICAL REPORT 484 OCTOBER I, 1971 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH LABORATORY OF ELECTRONICS CAMBRIDGE, MASSACHUSETTS 02139 The Research Laboratory of Electronics is an interdepartmental laboratory in which faculty members and graduate students from numerous academic departments conduct research. The research reported in this document was made possible in part by support extended the Massachusetts Institute of Tech- nology, Research Laboratory of Electronics, by the JOINT SER- VICES ELECTRONICS PROGRAMS (U. S. Army, U. S. Navy, and U.S. Air Force) under Contract No. DAAB07-71-C-0300, and by the National Institutes of Health (Grant 5 PO1 GM14940-05). Requestors having DOD contracts or grants should apply for copies of technical reports to the Defense Documentation Center, Cameron Station, Alexandria, Virginia 22314; all others should apply to the Clearinghouse for Federal Scientific and Technical Information, Sills Building, 5285 Port Royal Road, Springfield, Virginia 22151. THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN APPROVED FOR PUBLIC RELEASE AND SALE; ITS DISTRIBUTION IS UNLIMITED. MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH LABORATORY OF ELECTRONICS Technical Report 484 October 1, 1971 PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL INTERVALS: EVIDENCE FOR THE CENTRAL ORIGIN OF COMPLEX TONES Adrianus J. M. Houtsma and Julius L. Goldstein This report is based on a thesis by A. J. M. Houtsma submitted to the Department of Electrical Engineering at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, June 1971, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Contribution of the Conditioning Stage to the Total Harmonic Distortion in the Parametric Array Loudspeaker
    Universidad EAFIT Contribution of the conditioning stage to the Total Harmonic Distortion in the Parametric Array Loudspeaker Andrés Yarce Botero Thesis to apply for the title of Master of Science in Applied Physics Advisor Olga Lucia Quintero. Ph.D. Master of Science in Applied Physics Science school Universidad EAFIT Medellín - Colombia 2017 1 Contents 1 Problem Statement 7 1.1 On sound artistic installations . 8 1.2 Objectives . 12 1.2.1 General Objective . 12 1.2.2 Specific Objectives . 12 1.3 Theoretical background . 13 1.3.1 Physics behind the Parametric Array Loudspeaker . 13 1.3.2 Maths behind of Parametric Array Loudspeakers . 19 1.3.3 About piezoelectric ultrasound transducers . 21 1.3.4 About the health and safety uses of the Parametric Array Loudspeaker Technology . 24 2 Acquisition of Sound from self-demodulation of Ultrasound 26 2.1 Acoustics . 26 2.1.1 Directionality of Sound . 28 2.2 On the non linearity of sound . 30 2.3 On the linearity of sound from ultrasound . 33 3 Signal distortion and modulation schemes 38 3.1 Introduction . 38 3.2 On Total Harmonic Distortion . 40 3.3 Effects on total harmonic distortion: Modulation techniques . 42 3.4 On Pulse Wave Modulation . 46 4 Loudspeaker Modelling by statistical design of experiments. 49 4.1 Characterization Parametric Array Loudspeaker . 51 4.2 Experimental setup . 52 4.2.1 Results of PAL radiation pattern . 53 4.3 Design of experiments . 56 4.3.1 Placket Burmann method . 59 4.3.2 Box Behnken methodology . 62 5 Digital filtering techniques and signal distortion analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Large Scale Sound Installation Design: Psychoacoustic Stimulation
    LARGE SCALE SOUND INSTALLATION DESIGN: PSYCHOACOUSTIC STIMULATION An Interactive Qualifying Project Report submitted to the Faculty of the WORCESTER POLYTECHNIC INSTITUTE in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Science by Taylor H. Andrews, CS 2012 Mark E. Hayden, ECE 2012 Date: 16 December 2010 Professor Frederick W. Bianchi, Advisor Abstract The brain performs a vast amount of processing to translate the raw frequency content of incoming acoustic stimuli into the perceptual equivalent. Psychoacoustic processing can result in pitches and beats being “heard” that do not physically exist in the medium. These psychoac- oustic effects were researched and then applied in a large scale sound design. The constructed installations and acoustic stimuli were designed specifically to combat sensory atrophy by exer- cising and reinforcing the listeners’ perceptual skills. i Table of Contents Abstract ............................................................................................................................................ i Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ ii Table of Figures ............................................................................................................................. iii Table of Tables .............................................................................................................................. iv Chapter 1: Introduction .................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Combination Tones and Other Related Auditory Phenomena
    t he university ot cbtcago ro m a n wyo ur: no c xxuu.“ COMBINATION TONES AND OTHER RELATED AUDITORY PHENOMENA A DI SSERTA TION SUBMITTED TO THE FA CULTY OF TH E GRADUA TE SCHOOL OF A RTS AND LITERATURE IN CANDIDA CY FO R THE DEG REE OF DOCTOR OF PHI LOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF PSY CHOLOGY BY JOSEPH PETERSON u rr N o o r r un Psvc a o wc xcu. Rm " wa Sm u n . Pvumun AS “o uo c 39 , 1 908. PREFA CE . The first part of this mo no graph is devo ted primarily to a critical exposition of the important theories of combination to n and t t nt e n es a s a eme of th facts upo which they rest . This undertaking i nevitably leads to the mention of a considerable number of closely related phenomena whose significance for I e general theo ry is often crucial . n view of th conditions l n in the t t the t it n th t prevai i g li era ure of subjec , has bee ough expedient that this presentation should in the main follow h n n . n c nt nts c ro ological li es The full a alyt ical table of o e , t t t the n int n n oge her wi h divisio o sect io s , will readily e able he x readers who so desire to consult t te t o n special topics . The second part of the monograph report s cert ain experimcnta l t n t observa io s made by the au hor o n summation tones .
    [Show full text]
  • An Infrasonic Missing Fundamental Rises at 18.5Hz Christopher D
    Papers & Publications: Interdisciplinary Journal of Undergraduate Research Volume 2 Article 11 2013 An Infrasonic Missing Fundamental Rises at 18.5Hz Christopher D. Lacomba University of North Georgia Steven A. Lloyd University of North Georgia Ryan A. Shanks University of North Georgia Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.northgeorgia.edu/papersandpubs Part of the Behavior and Behavior Mechanisms Commons, Biological and Chemical Physics Commons, Biological Psychology Commons, Cognition and Perception Commons, Cognitive Neuroscience Commons, Cognitive Psychology Commons, Computational Neuroscience Commons, Dynamic Systems Commons, Investigative Techniques Commons, Mental Disorders Commons, Non-linear Dynamics Commons, Numerical Analysis and Computation Commons, Psychological Phenomena and Processes Commons, Speech and Hearing Science Commons, and the Systems Neuroscience Commons Recommended Citation Lacomba, Christopher D.; Lloyd, Steven A.; and Shanks, Ryan A. (2013) "An Infrasonic Missing Fundamental Rises at 18.5Hz," Papers & Publications: Interdisciplinary Journal of Undergraduate Research: Vol. 2 , Article 11. Available at: http://digitalcommons.northgeorgia.edu/papersandpubs/vol2/iss1/11 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Center for Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities (CURCA) at Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Papers & Publications: Interdisciplinary Journal of Undergraduate Research by an authorized editor of Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository. The Missing Fundamental (MF) phenomenon creates perceptible tones, which do not actually exist in the audible range, from harmonic series. Sounds that follow a harmonic series are perceptually significant since they stand out from random background noise and frequently represent sounds of biological origin and importance. For example, both animal vocalizations and human speech produce harmonic series of sounds.
    [Show full text]
  • Listening to String Sound: a Pedagogical Approach To
    LISTENING TO STRING SOUND: A PEDAGOGICAL APPROACH TO EXPLORING THE COMPLEXITIES OF VIOLA TONE PRODUCTION by MARIA KINDT (Under the Direction of Maggie Snyder) ABSTRACT String tone acoustics is a topic that has been largely overlooked in pedagogical settings. This document aims to illuminate the benefits of a general knowledge of practical acoustic science to inform teaching and performance practice. With an emphasis on viola tone production, the document introduces aspects of current physical science and psychoacoustics, combined with established pedagogy to help students and teachers gain a richer and more comprehensive view into aspects of tone production. The document serves as a guide to demonstrate areas where knowledge of the practical science can improve on playing technique and listening skills. The document is divided into three main sections and is framed in a way that is useful for beginning, intermediate, and advanced string students. The first section introduces basic principles of sound, further delving into complex string tone and the mechanism of the violin and viola. The second section focuses on psychoacoustics and how it relates to the interpretation of string sound. The third section covers some of the pedagogical applications of the practical science in performance practice. A sampling of spectral analysis throughout the document demonstrates visually some of the relevant topics. Exercises for informing intonation practices utilizing combination tones are also included. INDEX WORDS: string tone acoustics, psychoacoustics,
    [Show full text]
  • COMBINATION TONES in VIOLINS Angela Lohri, Sandra Carral And
    Proceedings of the Second Vienna Talk, Sept. 19−21, 2010, University of Music and Performing Arts Vienna, Austria COMBINATION TONES IN VIOLINS Angela Lohri, Sandra Carral and Vasileios Chatziioannou Institute of Musical Acoustics and International Centre of Harmonics University of Music and Performing Arts Vienna, Austria [email protected] ABSTRACT two different instruments, generated a third tone, in Tartini’s In this study we investigate the appearance of combination words “terzo suono”. He observed that, lying lower than the tones in violins. Most modern textbooks emphasise that played interval, the third tone acted like a bass giving the combination tones occur inside the ear exclusively (intra- interval a third dimension, a subtle harmonic context.[1], pp. 13- aural). In this study this assumption will be subjected to 17. Tartini, not only a genius musician but also a genius thinker, scrutiny based on evidence found in an empirical study, in wrote down the detected correlations between mathematics and which combination tones were measured outside the ear the third tones in his book Trattato di musica secondo la vera (extra-aural). scienza dell’armonia (“Treatise on Music according to the True Science of Harmony”). His insights about the principles of the An experiment was performed in which a violinist played third tone had a significant influence on the development of two tones of a particular musical interval simultaneously. music theory. Tartini’s discovery opened up entirely new This was recorded and subsequently analysed using a possibilities of how sound quality can be understood and Fourier Transformation. In addition to the partial tones of realized by practising musicians.
    [Show full text]
  • 11 – Music Temperament and Pitch
    11 – Music temperament and pitch Music sounds Every music instrument, including the voice, uses a more or less well defined sequence of music sounds, the notes, to produce music. The notes have particular frequencies that are called „pitch“ by musicians. The frequencies or pitches of the notes stand in a particular relation to each other. There are and were different ways to build a system of music sounds in different geographical regions during different historical periods. We will consider only the currently dominating system (used in classical and pop music) that was originated in ancient Greece and further developed in Europe. Pitch vs interval Only a small part of the people with normal hearing are able to percieve the absolute frequency of a sound. These people, who are said to have the „absolute pitch“, can tell what key has been hit on a piano without looking at it. The human ear and brain is much more sensitive to the relations between the frequencies of two or more sounds, the music intervals. The intervals carry emotional content that makes music an art. For instance, the major third sounds joyful and affirmative whereas the minor third sounds sad. The system of frequency relations between the sounds used in music production is called music temperament . 1 Music intervals and overtone series Perfect music intervals (that cannot be fully achieved practically, see below) are based on the overtone series studied before in this course. A typical music sound (except of a pure sinusiodal one) consists of a fundamental frequency f1 and its overtones: = = fn nf 1, n 3,2,1 ,..
    [Show full text]
  • Impairment of the Missing Fundamental Phenomenon in Individuals with Alzheimer’S Disease: a Neuropsychological and Voxel-Based Morphometric Study
    EXTRA Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra 2018;8:23–32 DOI: 10.1159/000486331 © 2018 The Author(s) Received: November 17, 2017 Published by S. Karger AG, Basel Accepted: December 7, 2017 www.karger.com/dee Published online: February 1, 2018 This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Interna- tional License (CC BY-NC-ND) (http://www.karger.com/Services/OpenAccessLicense). Usage and distribu- tion for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. Original Research Article Impairment of the Missing Fundamental Phenomenon in Individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease: A Neuropsychological and Voxel-Based Morphometric Study a a, b a a Makiko Abe Ken-ichi Tabei Masayuki Satoh Mari Fukuda c d a Hironobu Daikuhara Mariko Shiga Hirotaka Kida a, b Hidekazu Tomimoto a Department of Dementia Prevention and Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, b Mie University, Mie, Japan; Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, c Mie University, Mie, Japan; Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan; d Mie Prefectural Dementia-Related Disease Medical Center, Mie, Japan Keywords Alzheimer’s disease · Dementia · Pitch perception · Missing fundamental phenomenon · Magnetic resonance imaging · Voxel-based morphometry Abstract Background/Aims: The missing fundamental phenomenon (MFP) is a universal pitch percep- tion illusion that occurs in animals and humans. In this study, we aimed to determine wheth- er the MFP is impaired in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) using an auditory pitch per- ception experiment. We further examined anatomical correlates of the MFP in patients with AD by measuring gray matter volume (GMV) on magnetic resonance images via voxel-based morphometric analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Locomotion-Induced Sounds and Sonations: Mechanisms, Communication Function, and Relationship with Behavior
    Chapter 4 Locomotion-Induced Sounds and Sonations: Mechanisms, Communication Function, and Relationship with Behavior Christopher James Clark Abstract Motion-induced sound is an intrinsic byproduct of essentially all animal behavior. Locomotion-induced sounds that have evolved specialization for commu- nication are termed sonations. The null hypothesis is that locomotion-induced sounds are noncommunicative (adventitious), produced by nonspecialized mor- phology, and are involuntary. A sound is a sonation if it is produced by specialized morphology, or is produced voluntarily. The production of locomotion-induced sound can be examined at two levels: the animal motions (kinematics) that lead to sound production and the physical acoustic mechanism(s) that generate(s) the sound itself. The physical acoustics of locomotion induced sound are diverse, with both aerodynamic and structural mechanisms, including aeroelastic flutter, percussion, stridulation, and presumably many other undescribed mechanisms. There is a direct sound–motion correspondence between aspects of an animal’s motions and ensuing locomotion-induced sounds, especially in the time domain. This correspondence has two implications. One is experimental: sound recordings are a useful and perhaps underutilized source of data about animal locomotion. The second is behavioral: locomotion-induced sounds intrinsically contain information about an animal’s motions (such as wingbeat frequencies) that may be of interest to other animals. Therefore, locomotion-induced sounds are intrinsically suited to be mechanisms by which animals directly evaluate the locomotor performance of other animals, such as during courtship. The sound–motion correspondence is also a constraint. Sonations seem less acoustically diverse than vocalizations. Because they require discrete behaviors to be produced, animals also have somewhat fewer opportunities to produce sonations strategically, and few sonations are frequency-­ modulated.
    [Show full text]