<<

\,

Journal ol Experimental : General 1976, Vol. 105, No. 1, 3-46

Learned Helplessness: Theory and Evidence

Steven F. Maier Martin E. P. Seligman University of Colorado University of Pennsylvania

SUMMARY In 1967, Overmier and Seligman found that dogs exposed to inescapable and unavoidable electric shocks in one situation later failed to learn to escape shock in a different situation where escape was possible. Shortly thereafter Seligman and Maier (1967) demonstrated that this effect was caused by the uncontrollability of the original shocks. In this article we review the effects of exposing organisms to aversive events which they cannot control, and we review the explanations which have been offered. There seem to be motivational, cognitive, and emotional effects of uncontrol- lability. (a) Motivation. Dogs that have been exposed to inescapable shocks do not subsequently initiate escape response in the presence of shock. We review parallel phenomena in cats, fish, rats, and man. Of particular interest is the discussion of learned helplessness in rats and man. Rats are of interest because learned helplessness has been difficult to demonstrate in rats. However, we show that inescapably shocked rats do fail to learn to escape if the escape task is rea- sonably difficult. With regard to man, we review a variety of studies using in- escapable noise and unsolvable problems as agents which produce learned helpless- ness effects on both instrumental and cognitive tasks, (b) Cognition. We argue that exposure to uncontrollable events interferes with the organism's tendency to perceive contingent relationships between its behavior and outcomes. Here we review a variety of studies showing such a cognitive set. (c) Emotion. We re- view a variety of experiments which show that uncontrollable aversive events pro- duce greater emotional disruption than do controllable aversive events. We have proposed an explanation for these effects, which we call the learned helplessness hypothesis. It argues that when events are uncontrollable the organism learns that its behavior and outcomes are independent, and that this learning pro- duces the motivational, cognitive, and emotional effects of uncontrollability. We describe the learned helplessness hypothesis and research which supports it. Finally, we describe and discuss in detail alternative hypotheses which have been offered as accounts of the learned helplessness effect. One set of hypotheses argues that organisms learn motor responses during exposure to uncontrollable shock that compete with the response required in the test task. Another explanation holds that uncontrollable shock is a severe stressor and depletes a neurochemical necessary for the mediation of movement. We examine the logical structure of these explanations and present a variety of evidence which bears on them directly. STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

What follows are three instances of the fails to solve the second one also. The third phenomenon to be explained: anagram, a relatively easy one, he solves, after about 60 sec. He fails to solve the 1. When placed in a shuttle box an ex- next eight, appearing to give up after about perimentally naive dog, at the onset of the 60 sec with, each one. He then solves six first electric shock, runs frantically about, in a row, but very slowly, and finally sees the until it accidentally scrambles over the bar- pattern. He now solves the final three im- rier and escapes the shock. On the next mediately. trial, the dog, running frantically, crosses the 3. A naive rat is placed in a shuttle box barrier more quickly than on the preceding and trained to escape from shock. Shock trial. Within a few trials the animal be- terminates immediately upon his running to comes very efficient at escaping and soon the opposite side; he learns readily, and learns to avoid shock altogether. After about escapes efficiently. A second rat who had 50 trials the dog becomes nonchalant and received inescapable shock earlier in another stands in front of the barrier. At the onset apparatus learns just as well as the first rat of the signal for shock, the dog leaps grace- to escape in the shuttle box. Now, how- fully across and rarely gets shocked again. ever, the contingency between crossing the But dogs first given inescapable shock in a shuttle box and shock termination is ob- Pavlovian hammock show a strikingly dif- fuscated ; shock does not terminate im- ferent pattern. Such a dog's first reactions mediately upon crossing, but only after 3 sec to shock in the shuttle box are much the elapse. The first rat continues to escape same as those of a naive dog. He runs readily, learning to bridge a 3-sec delay of around frantically for about 30 sec, but then . The second rat, however, stops moving, lies down, and quietly whines. fails to respond; on other trials, he runs After 1 min. of this, shock terminates auto- across during shock, but overall he shows no matically. The dog fails to cross the barrier learning curve. and escape from shock. On the next trial, We believe these three phenomena are all the dog again fails to escape. At first he instances of "learned helplessness," instances struggles a bit and then, after a few seconds, in which an organism has learned that out- seems to give up and passively accept the comes are uncontrollable; by his responses shock. On all succeeding trials, the dog and is seriously debilitated by this knowl- continues to fail to escape. edge. This article explores the evidence for 2. A college student is confronted with a the phenomenon of learned helplessness, and series of 25 letter anagrams, each with the discusses a variety of theoretical interpreta- same pattern, 34251. He has a little trouble tions. Since the phenomenon results from with the first one, taking about 45 sec to experience with uncontrollable outcomes, we solve it. He solves each of the next three begin by defining uncontrollability. in about 30 sec, and now he sees the pattern. Each of the last 16 anagrams is solved im- TJJNCONTROLLABILITY AND THE INSTRU- mediately. In striking contrast is the college ! MENTAL TRAINING SPACE student who has first faced a series of dis- llearning theorists have usually viewed crimination problems which are unsolvable the I relations between instrumental respond- or a series of loud tones which are inescap- ing! and outcomes to which organisms are able. He works hard on the first anagram, sensitive in terms of the _ conditional prob- trying many rearrangements of letters, but ability of an outcome or reinforcer following fails to solve it in the 100 sec allowed. He a response />(RF/R), which can have values ranging from 0 to 1.0. At 1.0, every re- The preparation of this manuscript was supported sponse produces a reinforcer or outcome by Grant MH26827-01 to Steven F. Maier and (continuous reinforcement). At- 0, a re- Grant MH19604-01 to Martin E. P. Seligman. sponse never produces a reinforcer (extinc- Requests for reprints should be sent to Steven F. Maier, Department of Psychology, University tion). Intermediate values represent vari- of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado 80302. ous degrees of partial reinforcement. LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE

One conditional probability, however, is an inadequate description of the relations be- tween response and outcomes about which an organism may learn. Important events can sometimes occur when no specific re- sponse has been made, and it would be a woefully maladaptive organism that was in- sensitive to such a contingency. Rather p (RF/R) than representing environmental contingen- .40 - cies as occurring along a single dimension, we think instrumental training can be better described using a two-dimensional space, as .20 - shown in Figure 1. The .ar-axis />(RF/R) represents the traditional dimension, the conditional probability of an outcome follow- (.00 ing a response. Orthogonal to the condi- tional probability of an outcome, given a FIGURE 1. The response-reinforced contingency response, is the conditional probability of an space. p( RF/R) = conditional probability of an outcome occurring in the absence of that re- outcome following a response, />(RF/R) = condi- tional probability of an outcome occurring in the sponse />(RF/R). This dimension is repre- absence of that response. sented along the y-axis. We assume that organisms are sensitive to variations along is reinforced 50% of the times that it makes both dimensions conjointly, and the empiri- R, but even if it fails to make R, it is rein- cal meaning of this assumption is that sys- forced 20% of the time. tematic changes in behavior should occur The traditional training procedures ar- with systematic changes along both dimen- rayed along the #-axis have been thoroughly sions. There is considerable convergence of explored by many experimenters (e.g., Fer- opinion and evidence among learning theo- ster & Skinner, 1957; Honig, 1966). The rists today that organisms can indeed learn points in the training space which do not about the contingencies within this instru- fall along the #-axis have not, however, been mental training space, including the crucial systematically investigated. Consider the 45° line (e.g., Catania, 1971; Church, 1969;; points that lie along the 45° line (x, y, Gibbon, Berryman, & Thompson, 1974; where x = y). Whether or not the subject Maier, Seligman, & Solomon, 1969; Res- responds, the density of reinforcement is the corla, 1967, 1968; Seligman, Maier, & Solo- same. The conditional probability of an mon, 1971; Wagner, 1969; Weiss, 1968). outcome, given a specific response, does not Thus an organism may learn the extent to differ from the conditional probability of re- which food occurs when it does not make a inforcement in the absence of that response. specific response along with learning the The outcome is independent of responding. extent to which food occurs when it does The concepts of controllability and uncon- make a specific response. trollability are defined within this instru- Consider a few examples. In Figure 1, mental training space. Any time there is Point a (1.0,0) is a case of continuous re- something the subject can do or refrain inforcement : The subject is always rein- from doing that changes what it gets, it has forced for response R, and is never rein- control. Specifically, a response R stands in forced if it fails to make R. Point b (0,1.0) a relation of control to a reinforcer RF if is a case in which the subject is never rein- and only if forced for making the designated R, and is /» (RF/R) ^# (RF/R). (1) always reinforced for refraining from R (dif- ferential reinforcement of other behavior). Furthermore, when a response will not Consider Point c (.5,.2): Here the subject change what the subject gets, the response STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN, and reinforcer are independent. Specifically, a response R stands in relation of independ- ence to a reinforcer RF if and only if />(RF/R) = (2) When this is true of all emitted responses (as in Pavlovian conditioning) the subject cannot control the reinforcer, and the rein- forcer is defined as uncontrollable. How can we tell that the phenomena we will discuss result from experiencing uncon- 4 56 trollable outcomes as opposed to merely ex- TRIALS periencing the outcome itself? To put it FIGURE 2. Median response latency in a shuttle another way, how can we tell whether help- box for dogs given escapable, yoked inescapable, lessness is a psychological phenomenon as or no shock in a harness. (The yoked group did not learn to escape.) (From "Failure to Escape opposed to merely being the result of physi- Traumatic Shock" by Martin E. P. Seligman and cal stimulation? Steven F. Maier, Journal of Experimental Psy- There is a simple and elegant experimental chology, 1967, 74, 1-9, Copyright 1967 by the design which isolates the effects of con- American Psychological Association. Reprinted trollability from the effects of the outcome by permission.) being controlled. In this "triadic" design, trol which it had over shdck; while pressing three groups are used: One group receives the panel did not affect the programmed as its pretreatment an outcome that it can shocks in the yoked group, panel pressing control by some response. A second group terminated shock in the escape group. A is "yoked"—it receives exactly the same naive control group received no shock in the physical outcome as its counterpart in the hammock. :- first group, but there is no response the Twenty-four hours after the hammock yoked subject can make which modifies these treatment, all three groups received escape/ outcomes. A third group receives no pre- avoidance training in a shuttle box. Figure treatment. Later, all groups are tested on a 2 shows the results of this experiment. The new task. escape group and the naive control group Helplessness does not result from trauma performed well in the shuttle box. They per se: In the studies we discuss, deficits jumped the barrier readily. In contrast, the do not occur in the groups that control yoked group was significantly slower to re- shock, but only in the yoked group (Hiroto spond than the escape group and the naive & Seligman, 1975; Maier, 1970; Maier, control group. Six of the eight subjects in Anderson, & Lieberman, 1972; Seligman & the yoked group failed completely to escape Beagley, 1975; Seligman & Maier, 1967).1 shock. So it was not the shock itself, but The triadic design is a direct test of the the inability to control the shock, that pro- hypothesis that learning that shock is uncon- duced failure to respond.1 trollable, and not shock per se, causes help- lessness. Here is an example of how the EFFECTS OF UNCONTEOLLABILITY triadic design is used: Seligman and Maier Having defined the objective conditions (1967) used three groups of eight dogs. An under which uncontrollability occurs and escape group was trained in a hammock to turn off shock by pressing a panel with its lit should be mentioned: that Church (1963) nose. A yoked group received shocks identi- has argued against the use of the yoked group cal in number, duration, and pattern to the as a control group for instrumental learning. This shocks delivered to the escape group. The argument is not relevant to helplessness experi- ments in which the yoked ;group is the experi- yoked group differed from the escape group mental group, and the other groups are each con- only with respect to the instrumental con- trol groups. LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE delineated the kind of experimental design of the signal and the shock is 10 sec. If the which isolates the effects of uncontrollability dog jumps the shoulder-high barrier during from the effects of stimulation per se, we now this interval, the signal terminates and shock review the deficits produced by uncontrol- is prevented. Failure to jump during the lable outcomes. In general, when an organ- signal-shock interval leads to a 4.5-mA ism experiences uncontrollable events, three shock which remains until .the dog jumps the deficits often ensue: motivational, cognitive, barrier. If the dog fails to jump the barrier and emotional. within 60 sec after signal onset, the trial a) The motivation to respond in the face automatically ends. of later aversive events seems to wane, b) Between the years 1965 and 1969 the be- Moreover, even if the subject does respond havior of about 150 dogs that received prior and the response succeeds in producing re- inescapable shock was studied. Of these,- lief, the subject often has difficulty learning two thirds (about 100) did not learn to that the response worked, c) Finally, emo- escape and went through the striking se- tional balance may be distributed; quence of behaviors that we described. The and anxiety, measured in a variety of ways, other one third seemed completely normal. may predominate. The motivational deficits Like naive dogs, they escaped efficiently. produced by helplessness are in many ways There was no intermediate outcome. In- the most striking, so we turn to them first terestingly enough, of the several hundred for review and analysis. naive dogs who had been given shuttle box training, about 95% have been efficient re- Motivational Deficits sponders. The other 5% failed to learn Dogs. These sets of experiments form the even with no prior inescapable shock. We base of the pyramid on which we construct believe that the prior history of these dogs a theory of learned helplessness, so these before they arrived at the laboratory may studies will be examined thoroughly. The determine whether a naive dog will show a behavior of dogs exposed to inescapable learned helplessness effect and whether a shock seems to typify what many species do dog given inescapable shock will be immune when they are faced with uncontrollability. to this effect. When we discuss the theory Here is the typical procedure that pro- and prevention of the learned helplessness duces learned helplessness in the dog (Over- effect below, we will be more explicit about mier, 1968; Overmier & Seligman, 1967; how to immunize against failure to escape. Seligman & Groves, 1970; Seligman & Since dogs exposed to inescapable shock Maier, 1967; Seligman, Maier, & Geer, seem to be physically capable of jumping the 1968). On the first day, the subject is barrier, the behavioral deficit must have a strapped into a hammock and given 64 in- psychological base. Occasionally, they jump escapable electric shocks, each 5.0 sec long the barrier between trials. Further, if a dog and of 6.0 mA (moderately painful) in- has been sitting and taking shock after shock tensity. The shocks are not predicted by any on the left side of the box, and the door on signal and they occur randomly in time. the right side is opened at the end of the Twenty-four hours later, the subject is given session, he will often come bounding across 10 trials of signalized escape/avoidance train- to escape from the box altogether ing in a two-way shuttle box. The dog The learned helplessness effect in the dog must jump over the barrier from one com- occurs in a variety of situations and is easily partment into the other to escape or avoid produced. Within limits, it does not de- shock. Shocks can occur in either com- pend on the use of any particular shock partment, so there is no place that is always parameters. We have varied the frequency, safe, but the response of shuttling or jump- intensity, duration, and temporal pattern of ing always leads to shock termination. shocks, and still produced the effect. Further- The onset of a signal (light dimming) be- more, it does not matter if the inescapable gins each trial, and the signal stays on until shock is or is not preceded by a signal. the trial ends. The interval between the start Finally, it does not matter what apparatus STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN the inescapable shocks are given in or where goldfish and then tested them in an aquatic the escape/avoidance training takes place. shuttle box. These fish were slower to The shuttle box and hammock are inter- avoid than naive controls. (See Behrend changeable. If the dog is given inescapable & Bitterman, 1963; Bintz, 1971; Frumkin shock in the shuttle box and then required & Brookshire, 1969; Padilla, 1973, for re- to press panels with its head to escape in lated goldfish data.) the hammock, he is still helpless. Further, Rats. Until recently, it has proven dif- after uncontrollable shock, dogs are not only ficult to produce a learned helplessness effect debilitated at fleeing from the shock itself in rats. A substantial number of experi- (escape), but they also seem to be unable ments were performed involving inescapable to prevent or "avoid" shock. Overmier shock; by and large, however, these showed (1968) gave dogs inescapable shock in the rather small, if any, effects on later response hammock and then tested them in a shuttle initiation. (Maier et al., 1969, and Selig- box. If the dog jumped after the signal man et al., 1971, reviewed the complicated went on, but before the shock came on, he literature, and the interested reader is re- could avoid the shock. But no escape was ferred there for details. See also Anderson, allowed, for if the dog failed to jump in the Cole, & McVaugh, 1968; de Toledo & Black, signal-shock interval, the barrier was closed 1967; Dinsmoor & Campbell, 1965a, 1965b; and inescapable shock occurred. The help- Looney & Cohen, 1972; Mullin & Mogen- less dogs failed to avoid, just as they had son, 1963; Weiss, Krieckhaus, & Conte, failed to escape. So, dogs previously ex- 1968, for representative experimental posed to inescapable shocks failed to cope studies.) Unlike dogs, a rat given prior adaptively with signals for shock as well as inescapable shock was typically only a bit with shock. slower to escape shock on the first few trials, Debilitation of response initiation as a con- or slower to acquire avoidance—it did not sequence of uncontrollable outcomes has fail to learn. been reported in cats, rats, mice, birds, pri- After intensive experimentation, however, mates, fish, and man, as well as in dogs. several investigators have :now independently The learned helplessness effect seems rather produced substantial learned helplessness ef- general among species that learn. fects in rats. (Maier, Albin, & Testa, 1973; Cats. Thomas and Baiter (in press) re- Maier & Testa, 1975; Seligman & Beagley, ported an effect in cats which seems identical 1975; Seligman, Rosellini, & Kozak, 1975.)« to learned helplessness in dogs. (See also In doing so, one crucial factor emerged—the Masserman, 1943, 1971; Seward & Hum- response used in the test for learned help- phrey, 1967; Zielinski & Soltysik, 1964, for lessness must be difficult, and not something other reports of debilitation in cats caused by the rat does very readily. So, for example, inescapable shock). They designed a ham- if rats are first exposed to inescapable shock mock for cats and gave them inescapable and then tested on a simple escape response shock. When later placed in a cat shuttle like pressing a bar once (FR-1), or fleeing box, those cats failed to escape. Like dogs, to the other side of a shuttle box, no deficits they sat and took the shock. Thomas and are found. If, however, the response re- Baiter also reported that blocking activity of quirement is increased—so the bar must be the septal area of the brain breaks up help- pressed three times in order for shock to lessness, and directly stimulating the septum end (FR-3 lever pressing) or the rat has to electrically produces a learned helplessness- run across the shuttle box and back (FR-2 like effect. When we discuss the theory and shuttling)—then the rat that has experienced therapy for helplessness, we will examine inescapable shock fails to escape. In con- these physiological data in more detail. Fish. Following inescapable shock, fish 2 In passing it should be mentioned that mice also show poor escape and avoidance re- have been reported to show response deficits fol- sponding. Padilla, Padilla, Ketterer, and lowing inescapable shock (Braud, Wepman, & Giacolone (1970) gave inescapable shock to Russo, 1969). LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 9

60 were then taken to a hand shuttle box; in FR-2 FR-3 order to escape noise the individual had only 50 to move his hand from one side to the other. Preshocked Both the no-noise and escape group learned 40 readily to shuttle with their hands. Like other species, however, the human inescap- 30 able group failed to escape and avoid; rather, most sat passively and took the aver- 20 sive noise. (For other learned helplessness experiments in man, see Fosco & Geer, 10 1971; Glass & Singer, 1972; Hiroto & Selig- man, 1975; Klein, Fencil-Morse, & Selig- 1234 1234 1234 man, in press, Krantz, Glass, & Snyder, Blocks of 10 Trials 1974; Miller & Seligman, 1975a, Note 1; FIGURE 3. Mean latency to press a lever to Racinskas, 1971; Rodin, 1975, Roth, 1973; escape shock in an FR-1, FR-2, and FR-3 sched- ule, for rats given inescapable or no shocks. (The Roth & Bootzin, 1974; Roth & Kubal 1975; yoked subjects escaped very poorly only on the Thornton & Jacobs, 1971.) FR-3.) (From "Learned Helplessness in the Rat" Hiroto's design was actually more com- by Martin E. P. Seligman and Gwyneth Beagley, plicated and had two other important factors. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- chology, 1975, 88, 534-541. Copyright 1975 by the Half the subjects in each of the three groups American Psychological Association. Reprinted were told that how they did in the shuttle by permission.) box was a test of skill; the other half were told that how they did was governed by trast, rats that have had prior escapable chance. Those who received chance instruc- shock or no shock learn even more difficult tions tended to respond poorly in all groups. responses without difficulty (see Figure 3). Finally, the personality dimension of "ex- Hannum, Rosellini, & Seligman (in press) ternal vs. internal of rein- recently extended these findings to develop- forcement" was also varied in Hiroto's de- ment. Three groups of rats received four sign (see Lefcourt, 1966; Rotter, 1966; sessions of escapable, inescapable, or no James, Note 2), with half of all the students shock shortly after they were weaned. At in each group being "externals" and half 90 days of age they were tested on an FR-3 lever press escape task. Rats that had re- 60 r ceived inescapable shock at weaning failed to escape shock. Rats that had received - SO escapable shock or no shock escaped well vu Yoked (see Figure 4). 40 Man. What are the laboratory effects of inescapable trauma in man? Like the dog, 30 cat, rat, and fish, when a human being is Control faced with noxious events that it cannot control, its motivation to respond seems to 20 be reduced. 10 Hiroto's study (1974) is representative Escapable and it replicated in college students the find- ings on dogs. In a triadic design, subjects 1234 in his escape group received loud noise which Blocks of 10 Trials they learned to turn off by button pushing. The subjects in the inescapable group re- FIGURE 4. Mean escape response latency for ceived the same noise, but the noise was rats given either escapable, yoked inescapable, or no shocks as infants (Hannum, Rosellini, & Selig- independent of their responding. A third man, in press). The rats given inescapable shocks group received no noise. All the groups as infants did not learn to escape.) 10 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

"internals." An external is a person who believes, as shown by his answers on a per- sonality inventory, that occur in his life by chance or luck and are beyond his control. An internal believes that he controls his own reinforcers and that skill will out. Hiroto found that externals be- came helpless in his experiment more easily than did internals (see also Dweck, 1975). So three independent factors, the laboratory procedure of uncontrollability, the cognitive set induced by chance instructions, and ex- 1357 13 IS 16 8 2O 22 2« 26 28 30 ternality, each produced a learned helpless- Trials ness effect. Given this convergence, Hiroto FIGURE S. Mean speed to escape a nonrewarded concluded that these three factors all erode goalbox. (The group designated by E previously motivation to escape by contributing to the received escapable shocks, the group designated by expectation that responding and relief are I received inescapable shocks, and the groups independent. designated by C received no shocks. The groups designated by IS received rewarded runway ac- This concludes the survey of motivational quisition training, whereas the group designated by deficits produced by exposure to uncontrol- 0 did not. The rats .given inescapable shocks lable aversive events in different species. It [15-1] showed no tendency to escape the frustrat- seems to be generally true that uncontrol- ing goalbox.) (From "Frustration and Learned Helplessness" by Robert A. Rosellini and Martin lability produces deterioration in the readi- E. P. Seligman, Journal of Experimental Psy- ness of dogs, cats, rats, fish, monkeys, and chology: Animal Behavior Processes, 197S, 1, men to respond actively to trauma. 149-157. Copyright 1975 by the American Psy- Generality. Does uncontrollability pro- chological Association. Reprinted by permission.) duce a habit limited to situations very like the ones in which uncontrollability is experi- in the rat, also for the earliest apparent enced, or does it produce a more general ef- learned helplessness study in the literature.) fect ? The question we are really asking here Rosellini and Seligman (1975) performed is "Is helplessness just an isolated set of an experiment in which shock-induced habits or does it involve a more basic learned helplessness may have transferred change?" We believe that what is learned to frustration. Three groups of rats received when the environment is uncontrollable can escapable shock, inescapable shock, or no have consequences for a wide range of be- shock. After this pretreatment, they learned havior. to run down an alleyway in order to get At the lowest level of generality, helpless- food in a goal box. Food was present on ness transfers from some types of apparatus every trial in the goal box. Once they had to others, as long as shock occurs in both learned, food was no longer placed in the .situations; dogs given inescapable shock in goal box. So during this extinction pro- a hammock fail to escape later in a shuttle cedure, the rats ran down the alleyway into box. But does what is learned transfer to the goal box where they expected food, but aversive situations not involving shock? found none. This has been shown to be a Braud et al. (1969) used a triadic design "frustrating" and aversive experience for a with mice. One group could escape shock rat (Amsel, Rashotte, & MacKinnon, 1966). by climbing up a pole, a second group was The rats were then given an opportunity to yoked, and a third group received no shock. jump out of the goal box and escape this All groups were then placed in an alley frustration. Rats that had experienced flooded with water and had to swim out in escapable shock or no shock escaped the goal order to escape. The yoked group was box readily. Rats that had experienced in- poorest at escaping from water, (See Mc- escapable shock did not escape from the goal Culloch & Bruner, 1939, for similar findings box. (See Figure 5.) The effect of ex- LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 11

gressive as well as defensive responses. (See 40-1 Brookshire, Littman, & Stewart, 1961, Ex- periment 6; and Anderson & Paden, 1966, for other evidence on intra-aversive trans- fer.) X O Does experiencing uncontrollable aversive 30H events have effects on nonaversively moti- vated behavior ? Recently, Hiroto and Selig- man (1975) and Miller and Seligman (1975) systematically explored the transfer CONFINED^-* of helplessness between instrumental tasks u 20" ESCAPED = and cognitive tasks. Three groups of col- YOKED* • lege students received escapable, inescap- able, or no loud noise. They were then switched to a nonaversive anagrams test, 12345 and time to solve a series of 20 anagrams like IATOP was recorded. Students who SESSIONS had received inescapable loud noise failed FIGURE 6. Shock-induced fighting frequency, for to find the solutions more often than the each of five test sessions, for rats that had received escape group and the no-noise group. So escapable, yoked inescapable, or no shocks. (In- exposure to uncontrollable aversive events escapable shocks depressed fighting.) (From "In- fluence of Control of Shock on Subsequent Shock- retards solution of cognitive problems in a Elicited Aggression" by Steven F. Maier, Christine nonaversive situation. Anderson, and David A. Lieberman, Journal of Are the debilitating effects of uncontrol- Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1972, lability only produced by uncontrollable SI, 94-100. Copyright 1972 by the American Psy- chological Association. Reprinted by permission.) trauma? What happens to response initia- tion when preceded by a history of uncon- posure to one kind of uncontrollable event trollable outcomes which are not traumatic? generalized to another—frustration. Hiroto and Seligman (1975) tried to pro- Another possible instance of the transfer duce a learned helplessness effect using un- of a learned helplessness effect is related to solvable discrimination problems. Just as a "shock-elicited aggression.'' If two rats are solvable discrimination problem is control- placed in a small enclosure and repeatedly lable in the same sense that an escapable shocked, the shocks frequently come to shock is controllable, an unsolvable discrimi- elicit attack and aggressive posturing. Maier nation problem is uncontrollable in the same et al., (1972) gave rats escapable shock, sense that an inescapable shock is uncon- inescapable shock, or no shock, and then trollable. The conditional probability of placed these rats in a shock-elicited aggres- success given any response (e.g., to the left) sion situation. As shown in Figure 6, the is the same as the probability of success if amount of shock-elicited aggression was the alternate response (e.g., to the right) is lower in the rats previously exposed to in- made. escapable shock than it was in the rats given With the formal similarity of unsolv- escapable or no shock (also see Payne, ability and inescapability in mind, Hiroto Anderson, & Murcurio, 1970; Powell & and Seligman (1975) and Klein et al. (in Creer, 1969). In a related study (unpub- press) gave three groups of college students lished data) we found that dogs that had four sets of solvable, unsolvable, or no- received inescapable shock as puppies lost discrimination problems. Then all groups out in competition for food (only one nose were given the hand shuttle box with loud fits into a coffee cup full of Alpo) with dogs noise to be escaped. Individuals who had that received no shock or escapable shock. solvable discrimination problems or no prior So it seems that exposure to uncontrollable problems escaped noise readily. The un- aversive events retards the initiation of ag- solvable group failed to escape the noise. So 12 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN we believe that response initiation to con- berg et al. removed the treadle during the trol noxious events may be impaired by ex- autoshaping test, and so it could be argued perience with uncontrollable reward. that the stimulus controlling the competing Equally intriguing is the possibility that behavior was removed for the treadle group. delivery of appetitive events independent of The stimulus controlling any competing be- behavior may produce a failure to learn re- havior was not removed for the noncontin- sponses to procure such events, an effect gent food group, and this difference could analogous to learned helplessness. Engberg, account for the results. In response to Hansen, Welker, and Thomas (1973) these criticisms Welker (1974) conducted trained one group of pigeons to press a a further investigation of the phenomenon. treadle to obtain food. A second group of One group of pigeons was trained to peck pigeons received food delivered indepen- a key for food, a second was given non- dently of behavior, and a third group re- contingent food, and a third was given only ceived only a small amount of magazine a small amount of magazine training. All training. Following this pretreatment, all pigeons were then given a test in which pigeons were tested with a key peck auto- treadle pressing produced food on a con- shaping procedure. In autoshaping (Brown tinuous reinforcement schedule. The key & Jenkins, 1968) the pigeon key is periodi- was present during the treadle test, so it cally illuminated for a brief period of time, could not be argued that the stimulus con- and food is presented at the end of each key trolling the competing behavior was re- illumination period whether or not a peck moved for the contingent food group. The has occurred. Pigeons normally acquire results were the same as in Engberg et al.; key pecking under such an arrangement. noncontingent food presentation retarded ac- The result was that the group given previous quisition. Taking a hint from the helpless- treadle press training was fastest to acquire ness experiments in rats (see pp. 8-9), Wel- key pecking and the group given noncon- ker repeated his experiment with an FR-3 tingent food was the slowest to acquire key treadle response as the test task. That is, pecking. Engberg et al. interpreted their the treadle had to be pressed three times in results as constituting an appetitive analogue order to produce food. This magnified the to learned helplessness; food was delivered size of the observed effect. independently of behavior, and this retarded A potentially related finding was recently the acquisition of a response to procure food. reported by Bainbridge (1973). At SO days However, the Engberg et al. experiment of age, rats were given a solvable black- has been criticized on a number of grounds white discrimination problem, an unsolvable (Gamzu, Williams, & Schwartz, 1973). problem, or no problem. Twenty days later First, Gamzu et al. argued that autoshaping they were tested with the same tasks but is not a reasonable task to use in attempting different stimuli, or in a Hebb-Williams to establish an appetitive analogue to learned maze based on spatial cues. The rats given helplessness because autoshaped key pecks an initial unsolvable problem performed are largely controlled by Pavlovian con- very poorly in both the same and the differ- tingencies (Moore, 1973) rather than instru- ent apparatus. Thus exposure to appetitive mental contingencies; learned helplessness is events delivered independently of behavior a debilitation of instrumental responding. may interfere with the acquisition of ap- Second, Gamzu et al. noted that it is easy petitively motivated responses, just as ex- to account for the Engberg et al. experiment posure to noncontingent shock interferes in terms of competing motor responses. with the acquisition of shock-motivated re- Both the group given treadle training and sponses. the group given noncontingent food would So we believe that the psychological state have been expected to learn a motor re- produced by uncontrollability may under- sponse incompatible with key pecking mine response initiation quite generally. (treadle responding and standing by the Dogs, rats, cats, fish, and people make fewer grain hopper, respectively). However, Eng- responses to escape shock after receiving LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 13 uncontrollable shock. Furthermore, these It appears as if one success is just not motivational deficits may not be limited to enough to make a dog that has experienced shock or even noxious events. Escape from inescapable shock learn that his responding frustration, aggressive behavior, and even now produces shock termination. the propensity to solve anagrams might be Miller and Seligman (1975) and Klein undermined by inescapable aversive events. et al. (in press) found that such a negative Conversely, uncontrollable rewards disrupt cognitive set results from uncontrollability escape from loud noise, learning to procure in man. Three groups of students received food, and competitiveness. However, it escapable, inescapable, or no loud noise. should be stressed that many of the experi- Then they confronted two new tasks, a task ments cited here are subject to alternative of skill and a task of chance. On each of 10 interpretation, and much more research will trials in the skill task, they sorted 15 cards be required before the true extent of these into 10 categories of shape, attempting to effects will be known. Clearly, there must complete it within IS sec. Unknown to be a limit to the degree of generalization of them, the experimenter arranged to have learned helplessness effects, and these limits them succeed or fail on any given trial by must be explored and delineated. We can- saying that time was up before they had not yet say what factors will prove import- finished or after. So they went through a ant. All that can be said now is that there prearranged run of successes and failures. appears to be some degree of transfer. At the end of each trial, the subjects rated. (on a 0-10 scale) what they thought their Cognitive Deficits chances of succeeding on the next trial We have argued that a major consequence would be. Subjects who were previously ex- of experience with uncontrollable events is posed to inescapable noise showed very little motivational; uncontrollable events under- change in their expectancy for successes mine the motivation to initiate voluntary re- after each new success and failure. They sponses to control future events. Experience had difficulty perceiving that their response with uncontrollable events may also have a would affect succeeding or failing. Control second consequence that is cognitive; ex- subjects and subjects who had escaped noise perience with uncontrollability may produce showed large expectancy changes following a difficulty in learning that responses have each .success and failure. This showed that succeeded, even when responding is actually they believed outcomes to be dependent on successful. Uncontrollability may retard the their actions. The three groups did not perception of control. differ in expectancy changes following suc- This phenomenon seems to appear in cess and failure in a "chance" task that they helpless dogs, rats, and men. Occasionally, perceived as a guessing game. So inescap- a naive dog sits and takes shock on the first able loud noise produced a cognitive set in three or four trials in the shuttle box, and which people believed that success and fail- then on the next trial jumps the barrier and ure is independent of their own skilled; escapes shock successfully for the first time. actions within the experimental situation, Once a naive dog makes one response that and they therefore had difficulty perceiving produces shock termination, it learns rapidly. that skilled responses were effective. On all further trials, the subject responds Hiroto and Seligman (1975), Miller and vigorously and goes on to learn to avoid Seligman (Note 1) and Klein et al. (in shock altogether. But dogs that first re- press) also reported this negative cognitive ceived inescapable shock were different in set in another form. After escapable, in- this respect also. About one third of them escapable, or no noise, as the reader will go through a similar pattern—sitting through recall, students had to solve anagrams (see shock on the first three or four trials and p. 11). Two kinds of deficits emerged. then escaping successfully on the next. These Inescapable noise interfered with their dogs, however, then revert to taking the ability to solve any given anagram. In shock, and they fail to escape on future trials. addition, there was a pattern to the 20 ana- 14 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

TABLE 1 Recent investigation has shown that active MEAN NUMBER AND STANDARD DEVIATION or learning does indeed occur when condi- CONSECUTIVE SUCCESSES BEFORE PATTERN SOLUTION tioned stimuli (CSs) and unconditioned stimuli (USs) are independently presented. Noise Mellgren and Ost (1971) reported that a Subject Inescapable Escapable None group for which CSs were presented inde- pendently of food took longer to learn later Depressed M 5.9 4.2 7.1 that the CSs were associated with the food SD 2.53 1.36 1.22 than did naive rats (or even rats for whom Nondepressed the CSs predicted the opposite relationship M 6.9 4.5 3.7 with food). Kemler and Shepp (1971) SD 3.36 1.58 .86 showed that irrelevant stimuli were most slowly learned about when they became the Note. Adapted from Miller and Seligman (Note 1), In- escapable noise increased the number of successes required in relevant stimuli for the solution of a discrimi- nondepressed subjects. nation problem for children. Thomas, Free- man, Svinicki, Burr, and Lyons (1970) grams to be solved; each was arranged showed that pigeons that had two colors with its letters in this order: 53124, for presented independently of food tended not example, ISOEN, DERRO, OURPG, etc. to discriminate later between two line tilts, As can be seen in Table 1, students who had one of which predicted the presence of food received inescapable noise required about and the other its absence. MacKintosh seven consecutive successes before catching (1973) also reported retardation of condi- on to the pattern; students who received tioning by prior CS-US independence. escapable noise or no noise needed about So independence between two stimuli three consecutive successes. Unsolvable dis- may produce active learning, and this learn- crimination problem's, incidentally, produced ing seems to retard the ability of rats, pi- the same disruption of anagram solution as geons, and men to learn later that the stimuli did inescapable noise. depend on one another. This evidence co- The existence of a negative cognitive set heres with the effects of response-outcome produced by independence between respond- independence on cognition and bolsters the ing and outcomes bears on an important hypothesis that such independence retards issue in learning theory; When two events the ability of the organism to perceive that are presented independently of each other, his responding has contingent consequences for example, a tone and a shock presented (also see the discussion of experiments by at random, does the subject learn anything Maier & Testa, 1975, pp. 24-25). at all about the tone or does he merely come to ignore it? A helplessness point of view Emotional Deficits holds that men and animals actively learn that responses and outcomes are independent Our first hint that uncontrollable aversive of each other, and one way the learning is events had emotional as well as motivational manifested is by the difficulty they later and cognitive consequences came when we have learning that the response produces the found that the motivational effects dissi- outcome when it does. Merely substitute pated in time under some circumstances. shock for outcome and tone for response. Overmier and Seligman (1967) found that This suggests that organisms should also after one session of inescapable shock in the actively learn when a tone and shock are hammock, dogs were helpless in the shuttle independent and that organisms show this box 24 hours later. If, however, the dogs by having trouble learning later that the tone were not tested in 24 hours, but at 48, 72, is followed by the shock when it actually is. or 168 hours after inescapable shock, they Rescorla (1967) held the contrary view. were normal. This is the only 'circumstance Independence between a tone and shock is a we know under which helplessness dissipates neutral condition in which nothing is learned. in time. If multiple sessions of inescapable LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 15 shock are given to the dog in the hammock, mals. Moreover, the yoked rats lost more he will fail to escape a week later in the weight, defecated more, and drank less than shuttle box (Selignian & Groves, 1970). the executive. So rats given inescapable Further, if the dog is reared from birth in shock show more stress or emotional effects laboratory cages, having minimal experience when measured by stomach ulcers than rats with control over events, small amounts of who can control shock. inescapable shock produce nontransient help- There is further evidence in rats that un- lessness (Seligman & Groves, 1970). Finally, controllable shock produces more severe no time course has been observed in rats; emotional reactions than does controllable following one session of inescapable shock, shock. Mowrer and Viek (1948) shocked rats fail to escape 5 minutes, 1 hour, 24 two groups of rats while the rats were hours, and 168 hours later. It must be eating. One group could control the shock borne in mind however, that under one cir- by jumping in the air, and the other group cumstance—a single session of inescapable received uncontrollable shock. The rats shock in dogs of unknown past history— getting uncontrollable shock subsequently helplessness dissipates in time, and such a ate less than those controlling shock (see time course hints of a transient emotional Brimer & Kamin, 1963; Desiderate & New- disturbance. man, 1971; Lindner, 1968; and Payne, 1972, Other evidence indicates the emotional for an ongoing controversy surrounding these consequences of uncontrollability. One data). In an analogous human study by widely quoted study (Brady, Porter, Con- Hokanson, DeGood, Forrest, and Brittain rad, & Mason, 1958) is related to uncon- (1971), subjects performed a symbol match- trollability and helplessness, but appears to ing task while being shocked. The schedules show less emotionality with uncontrollability. were individually arranged so that each sub- Two groups of four monkeys were given ject received an average of one shock every shocks; half of them—the "executives"— 45 sec. Subjects in the controllability group had control over the shock and could avoid were allowed to take as many time-outs as them by bar pressing. The other four were they wished whenever they wanted. A yoked yoked, or helpless, since they could not control group received the same number of modify shock. The executives formed stom- time-outs at the same times. Measures of ach ulcers and died, but the helpless mon- blood pressure taken at 30-sec intervals in- keys did not. Unfortunately, this result dicated that yoked subjects showed con- may be an artifact of the way the monkeys sistently higher blood pressures. (See Averill were assigned to the two groups. All eight & Rosenn, 1972; Bandler, Madaras, & Bern, monkeys were placed on the executive sched- 1968; Corah & Boffa, 1970; Elliot, 1969; ule originally, and the first four to start and Stotland & Blumenthal, 1964, for re- the lever pressing became the executives. lated studies in humans using a variety of The last four became the yoked subjects. other measures of emotional arousal. This It is possible that the more emotional a mon- is a complex and inconsistent literature and key is, the more quickly it begins to bar is reviewed by Averill, 1973.) press when it is shocked (Sines, Cleeland, & Hearst (1965) found that the presenta- Adkins, 1963). So the four most emotional tion of uncontrollable shocks resulted in a animals may have become the executives, breakdown of a well-trained appetitive dis- and the four least emotional may have be- crimination in rats. During uncontrollable come the yoked subjects. shock his rats no longer discriminated be- Jay Weiss (1968, 1971a, 1971b, 1971c) tween two stimuli, one of which signaled the has recently repeated the executive monkey presence and the other the absence of food. design with rats. (See also Moot, Ceballa, During controllable shock the appetitive & Crabtree, 1970.) Three groups of rats discrimination was maintained. were randomly assigned to the triadic de- Such breakdown of appetitive discrimina- sign. The executive animals showed fewer tions is reminiscent of the classic work on and less severe ulcers than the yoked ani- "experimental neurosis." The concept of 16 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN experimental neurosis is not a homogeneous poorly groomed; b'ehaviorally they performed un- one; neither is it well denned. Unlike the predictably if at all on the right hand lever prob- lems, pausing often for naps and rest. The be- other studies reviewed, controllability has haviors exhibited by these animals were clearly not been manipulated explicitly to produce nonadaptive in nature; for example, two subjects the "neurosis." Yet from an operational spent hours in catching "imaginary" flying insects, point of view, we can speculate that the one subject masturbated almost continuously, three subjects became almost compulsive hair lack or loss of control may be important in pullers, and all tended to show movement stereo- their etiology. Typically, an animal is re- typy alternating with an almost total lack of in- strained in some type of harness seriously terest in their external environment. (Stroebel, limiting what it can do. Often the procedure 1969, p. 97) is Pavlovian, and by definition the organism It is not clear whether there can be any has no control over the onset or the offset one theory which can account for "experi- of the stimuli presented. In Shenger-Krest- mental neuroses," nor is it clear whether all nikova's (in Pavlov, 1927) classic experi- these phenomena are closely related. But ment, an appetitive discrimination deterio- uncontrollability is prominently present, and rated and signs of 'distress were noted when emotional disruption is the frequent result. the dog could no longer tell the difference In summary, three types of disruption between the rewarded and nonrewarded stim- seem to be caused by uncontrollability of ulus. In the work of Liddell, James, and aversive events in the laboratory: The mo- Anderson (1934), sheep developed a range tivation to respond is reduced, the propensity of maladaptive behaviors following uncon- to perceive success is undermined, and emo- trollable electric shock. Masserman (1943) tionality is modified. These effects hold trained monkeys to feed in response to a across a variety of situations and species. We signal and made them "neurotic" by pre- need a theory to put all this together and we senting a fear-arousing stimulus during will now propose one. feeding. Without "therapy" these monkeys remained disturbed almost indefinitely. Ac- LEARNED HELPLESSNESS THEORY cording to Masserman (1943) : Markedly different, however, was the case of ani- The basic facts about learned helplessness mals that had been taught to manipulate various effects in the laboratory have now been ar- devices that actuated the signals and feeder be- rayed before the reader. - What must an cause in this way they could exert at least partial adequate theory of helplessness accomplish? control over their environment. This stood them in good stead even after they were made neurotic It must account for the three facets of the in as much as when their hunger increased they effects of uncontrollable aversive events: .gradually made hesitant, but spontaneous, attempts motivational, cognitive, and emotional. It to reexplore the operation of the switches, signals must be testable; there should be experi- and food boxes, and were bolder and more suc- ments that can be performed which would cessful as food began to reappear, (p. 82) confirm it if it is true, or disconfirm it if it is In a dramatic primate study, Stroebel (1969) false. Finally, it must be; applicable outside trained a group of rhesus monkeys to air- the laboratory—it must be: useful in explain- condition their overheated chamber by press- ing any helplessness effects found in the real ing a lever, and also to control loud noise, world. This final requirement is beyond the annoying light, and mild shocks by pressing scope of this article, but the interested reader the same lever. He then retracted the lever should see Seligman (1975). so that it could still be seen, but could no The theory to be presented is straight- longer be pressed. No further physical stres- forward, and the groundwork for it has been sors were presented. Initial responding was prepared by the way we laid out the data frantic. Brain temperature became irregular above. It accounts directly for the motiva- and the circadian rhythm was disrupted. tional and cognitive deficits and, with an additional premise, can account for the emo- As rhythm disturbance developed, members of this . . . group of subjects began to show lassitude and tional disturbance. The j theory has been weakness; their fur became knotted, mottled and tested in several ways, and as a bonus it LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 17

predicts ways to eliminate helplessness effects shock, while the dogs who could escape by and ways to prevent them. panel pressing and the dogs who were not When an organism is faced with an out- shocked did not fail to respond later. Clearly come that is independent of his responses, something different happened to the dogs he sometimes learns that the outcome is in- that received shock independently of their dependent of his responses. responses. We believe they learned that This is the cornerstone of our view and responding was futile and therefore expected probably seems obvious to all but the most future responding to shock to be futile. We sophisticated learning theorist. You will re- have gone to pains to give an objective call our discussion of the response contin- definition of uncontrollabtiity and response- gency space (Figure 1). Learning theorists outcome independence. This is because the would much prefer that the kinds of con- theory is basically a three-step affair. tingencies that .can be learned about be as Information about contingency —> simple as possible. First they believed that Cognitive representation of the contingency the most that could be learned about was a (learning, expectation, perception, belief)—* simple pairing of a response and an outcome, Behavior and the pairing of the response with the ab- sence of the outcome. But this had to be An organism must begin with the informa- broadened to include partial reinforcement, tion about the contingency between response with the subject integrating both kinds of and outcome. This information is a prop- pairings. So what could be learned about erty of the world out there, a set of stimuli, was broadened to the probability of an out- and not a property of the receiver. We have come given a response. Then, it was shown carefully defined the kind of information that that organisms could also learn about the can be called information that a response and probability of an outcome given that it did an outcome are independent. not make the indicated response. The added But the middle step in the chain is crucial step which our view makes is that the organ- and is easily overlooked. The information ism can learn about both these probabilities about the contingency must be processed and conjointly, that variation of experience cor- transformed into a cognitive representation responding to the points in the response of the contingency.3 Such a representation contingency space will produce systematic has been variously called "learning that out- particular, we have argued that exposing come and response are independent," "per- changes in behavior and cognition. In ceiving that response and outcome are in- organisms to the 45° line, in which the prob- dependent," or "believing that response and ability of the outcome is the same whether or outcome are independent." We prefer to not the response of interest occurs, produces call the representation "the expectation that learning. Behaviorally, this learning should responding and an outcome are independent." tend to produce lack of response initiation to A person or animal can be exposed to the control the outcome; cognitively it should environmental contingency in which an out- produce a belief in the inefficacy of respond- come response are independent, yet not ing and difficulty in learning that responding form such an expectation. Immunization, succeeds: and emotionally when the outcome as we shall see later in this article ';(refer to is traumatic it might produce emotional p. 25), is an example. Conversely, a per- changes. The basic triadic design employed in all 3 For attempts to spell out in detail the relation- the helplessness studies reviewed above is, of ship between the contingency information and its course, directly relevant to the premise that cognitive representation, the interested reader should men and animals learn about, and form ex- consult, for example, Kelly (1967, 1972), Weiner, pectations concerning, independence between Frieze, Kukla, Reed, Rest, and Rosenbaum (1971) outcome and responses. So, for example, from an theory point of view; Irwin (1971) and Seligman and Johnston (1973) from in the Seligman and Maier (1967) study a cognitive-learning theory point of view; as well only the yoked dogs later failed to escape as Lazarus (1966) and Stotland (1969). 18 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN son can show a helplessness effect without We assume that the incentive to initiate being exposed to the contingency as such; voluntary responses in a traumatic situa- he can merely be told that events are uncon- tion is partly produced by the expectation trollable. that responding produces relief. In the ab- Glass and Singer (1972) have done an sence of this incentive, voluntary responding extensive series of studies on the role of will decrease in likelihood. When a person controllability in reducing stress and have or animal has learned that relief is indepen- found that merely telling a human subject dent of responding, the expectation that re- about controllability duplicates the effects of sponding produces relief is negated, and actual controllability. They attempted to therefore response initiation is reduced. mimic the stress of the urban environment Most generally put, the incentive to initiate by having their subjects (college students) voluntary responses to control any outcome listen to a very loud melange of sound: two (e.g., food, sex, shock termination) comes, people speaking Spanish, one person speak- in part, from the expectation that responding ing Armenian, a mimeograph machine, a produces that outcome. (See Bolles, 1972, calculator, and a typewriter. When sub- for a discussion of expectational mechanisms jects could actually turn off the noise by and incentive motivation.)* In the absence button pushing, they were more presistent of this incentive, voluntary responding will in problem solving, they found the noise decrease in likelihood. When a person or less irritating, and they did better at proof- animal has learned the outcome is indepen- reading than yoked subjects. dent of responding, the expectation that re- Glass and Singer also presented another sponding will produce the outcome, and group of subjects with the same noise, but therefore response initiation, decreases. this time it was uncontrollable. However, The way in which this undermining of this group had a panic button and was told: motivation works has been seen with crystal "You can terminate the noise by pressing clarity in a human helplessness experiment the button. But we'd prefer you not do it." (Thornton & Jacobs, 1971). Following in- None of the subjects in fact tried to turn escapable shock, college students sat and off the noise. All they had was the false took the shock. When asked why they did information that they could control the noise not respond appropriately, 60% of the sub- if they had to. These subjects did just as jects reported they had no control over well as the subjects who actually controlled shocks, so why try. These subjective re- the noise. Actual controllability and actual ports suggest that a belief in uncontrollability uncontrollability may often produce cor- undermines the incentive to initiate re- responding expectations. But this type of sponses. experiment, in which the expectation is in- We also assume that learning that an out- valid, highlights the fact that it is the ex- come is independent of a response makes it pectation and not the objective conditions of more difficult later to learn that responses controllability that is the cornerstone of our produce that outcome. theory. (See also Geer, Davison, & Gatchel, Response^outcome independence is an ac- 1970; Geer & Maisel, 1972; Klein et al., in tive form of learning, and like any other press; Langer, 1974; Pervin, 1963; and active form of learning, it can proactively Stotland & Blumenthal, 1964, for related interfere with contravening forms of learn- effects of perceived control.) ing through an associative interference. At So the first step of the theory is that the organism acquires an expectation of re- sponse-outcome independence, when out- *It should also be mentioned that innately comes are uncontrollable. The second step elicited struggling is another source of responding in the theory is the means by which the ex- in a traumatic situation, but it is the waxing and waning of voluntary (outcome-sensitive) responses pectation of response-outcome independence which is our concern here. This does not deny produces the effects associated with helpless- that innate responses can be transformed into ness. voluntary responses (Schwartz & Williams, 1972). LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 19

first the dog is not a passive recipient of some theoretical positions which have been shock but emits many responses, but each of advanced as alternatives to the helplessness the responses is unrelated to shock termina- hypothesis. It should be noted at the outset tion. The dog might, for example, turn his that these hypotheses have been offered as head and shock might happen to go off on explanations of only the fact that organisms the trial, but on other trials he might turn exposed to inescapable shock later fail to his head and shock would not go off. Shock learn to escape and avoid shock in a shuttle would also terminate when he had not turned box, rather than as explanations of the his head. Now when he is tested in the broad range of phenomena considered above. shuttle box and jumps the barrier, which in Thus we will describe these theoretical posi- reality causes shock termination, the dog has tions in the context of the basic learned trouble learning this. This is because he has helplessness effect. These alternatiye the- had many experiences in which a pairing be- ories can be divided into two groups, motiva- tween a response and shock termination tional accounts and motor accounts. proved spurious and, as for head turning, he still expects that shock will just as likely go Motivational Alternatives off if he fails to jump the barrier. Such a dog may return to taking shock even after Adaptation. The adaptation hypothesis he makes one or two "successful" jumps. maintains that a subject adapts to shock In contrast, a naive dog has no interfering during pretreatment with inescapable shocks expectation that shock termination is inde- and is therefore not sufficiently motivated to pendent of responding, so one experience escape from shock in the shuttle box. The with barrier jumping leading to shock term- hypothesis is inadequate: ination is sufficient for him to learn. For some evidence regarding such associative 1. Adaptation to repeated, intense shock interference see the discussion of Maier and has never been demonstrated (Church Lo- Testa (1975), pp. 24-25. Lordo, Overmier, Solomon, & Turner, This, then, is the theory of helplessness. 1966). The expectation that an outcome is inde- 2. It is unlikely that very much adapta- pendent of responding (a) reduces the mo- tion could persist for as long as 24 hours tivation to control that outcome and (b) and beyond. interferes with learning that responding con- 3. In experiments with dogs, the dogs do trols the outcome. not look as though they are adapted; they Seligman (1975) has recently extended howl, defecate, and urinate to the first this theory to changes in emotionality. He shock presentation in the shuttle box. On argued that when a traumatic event first later trials, the dogs are passive; but they occurs it causes a heightened state of emo- whimper and jerk with the shock. tionality, which has been called "fear." The 4. We have disconfirmed the adaptation fear continues until the subject learns that hypothesis experimentally. Raising the shock he can or cannot control the trauma. If the level in the shuttle box should increase subject learns he can control the trauma, motivation to escape. However, Overmier fear is reduced and may disappear altogether. and Seligman (1967) found that increasing If the subject learns that he cannot control the shock level from 4.5 mA to 6.5 mA does the traumatic event, fear decreases and is not eliminate the interference effect. replaced with depression. For a more de- 5. A series of escapable shocks in the tailed discussion see Seligman (1975). hammock does not produce failure to escape in the shuttle box (Seligman & Maier, ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS 1967), while the same shocks, if inescapable, Now that we have described the learned do produce failure to escape. By this hy- helplessness hypothesis and the manner in pothesis, both conditions should lead to which it is able to account for the phe- equal adaptation to shock and to similar be- nomena under consideration, we will describe havior in the test situation, but they do not. 20 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

6. Seligman and Maier (1967) gave one spond on his own. Then the barrier was re- group of dogs 10 trjals of escape/avoidance placed, and the subject continued to escape training in a shuttle box before treatment and avoid. The recovery from helplessness with inescapable shock in the harness. Fol- was complete and lasting. This type of ex- lowing exposure to inescapable shocks in the posure to the escape and avoidance contin- harness, the dogs escaped and avoided shock gencies should not affect any adaptation to normally when returned to the shuttle box. shock that might have been present. Selig- Thus prior exposure to controllable shock man et al. (1975) replicated these findings "immunized" the dogs against the interfer- in rats. ing effects of exposure to inescapable shocks. This result follows from the helplessness hy- Sensitization. Perhaps the inescapable pothesis because prior experience with con- shocks received in the harness sensitize the trollable shock should proactively interfere subject to shock so that it is too motivated with the subject's learning that shock is un- to enable it to make organized responses in controllable and should also allow the sub- the shuttle box. This hypothesis is inade- ject to discriminate between the places where quate, (a) Sensitization explains the in- shocks are controllable and uncontrollable. efficiency of responding but not the absence However, this result is not consistent with of responding, (b) Lowering the shock the adaptation hypothesis. Prior exposure level in the shuttle box should permit the to escapable shock in the shuttle box should subject to make organized responses. How- not eliminate any adaptation produced by ever, Overmier and Seligman (unpublished inescapable shocks in the harness. data) found that the interference effect is 7. If the subject is tested and fails to not attenuated when shock in the shuttle box escape 24 hours after inescapable shocks, is reduced to 3.0 mA. (3) Arguments 5, the learned helplessness effect will persist in 6, and 7 in the previous paragraph, which chronic form; the subject will fail to escape invalidate the adaptation hypothesis, also in- on later opportunities. The helplessness validate the sensitization argument. hypothesis suggests a way to eliminate chro- nic failure to escape. By this hypothesis, Motor Activity Alternatives the dog does not try to escape because Within the last 2 years three hypotheses he does not expect that any instru- have been offered as explanations of the mental response will produce shock termina- learned helplessness effect which are more tion. By forcibly exposing the dog to the difficult to dismiss than are sensitization and escape and avoidance contingencies, this ex- adaptation. These explanations have the pectation might be altered. This type of advantage that they are able to account for training by "putting through" has been used the fact that the learned helplessness effect by others with mixed success (Loucks, is determined by the controllability of the 1935; Miller & Konorski, 1928; Woodbury, shock during the initial treatment. We have 1942). Seligman et al. (1968) reasoned classified them as motor activity hypotheses that forcibly dragging the dog from side to because they all maintain that exposure to side in the shuttle box, in such a way that inescapable shock interferes with subsequent the dog's changing compartments terminated shuttle box acquisition because it changes shock, might effectively expose the dog to motor activity. Two of these hypotheses the response-reinforcement contingency. This argue that exposure to inescapable shock was the case. The experimenter pulled three establishes a motor response that is incom- chronically helpless dogs back and forth patible with shuttling and therefore competes across the shuttle box with long leashes. with shuttling. Thus these two hypotheses This was done during CS and shock, while argue that the organism does not learn shut- the barrier was absent. After being pulled tling because it is performing a. motor re- across the center of the shuttle box (and thus sponse that prevents the occurrence of shut- terminating shock and CS) 20, 35, and 50 tling. The remaining hypothesis maintains times, respectively, each dog began to re- that inescapable shock is a powerful stressor LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 21 and depletes a neurochemical necessary for this hypothesis can explain why escapable the occurrence of movement. Thus this view shock does not produce a subsequent decre- holds that inescapably shocked animals fail ment. If shock is escapable, the organism to acquire shuttling because they cannot learns a response that completely eliminates move sufficiently. : shock (the escape response) and so does Incompatible motor response theories. As not have to learn a response that only miti- already noted, two different incompatible gates shock (the putative incompatible re- motor response explanations of the learned sponse). It need only be argued that the helplessness effect have been proposed. The former is not incompatible with shuttling but first to be described was offered by Brace- the latter is. well and Black (1974) and was suggested We will present a large amount of data by three experiments. In their first experi- which we feel to be inconsistent with the ment they examined the effects of restraint Bracewell and Black (1974) hypothesis. and inescapable shock upon simple FR-1 However, these experiments also bear on the shuttlebox escape/avoidance acquisition. second competing motor response theory and They found that restraint in the absence of so they will not be presented until that theory any exposure to inescapable shock produced has been described. Nevertheless, a few a decrement in FR-1 shuttlebox acquisition, comments are appropriate here. First, the but that exposure to inescapable shock in the fact that explicit punishment of movement absence of restraint did not produce a reli- produces a decrement in subsequent escape/ able decrement. In their second experiment avodiance acquisition does not imply that Bracewell and Black found that exposure to other procedures which produced escape/ "high" intensity (1.0 mA) inescapable shock avoidance decrements do so because they produces a small decrement in FR-1 shuttle punish movement. There are undoubtedly box acquisition even when delivered in the many ways to produce poor escape acquisi- unrestrained condition. In their final ex- tion, and it is unlikely that all operate periment Bracewell and Black explicitly through a similar mechanism. Paralyzing punished movement by arranging a positive the animal with a drug would retard escape correlation between movement and shock acquisition, but this does not mean that the intensity. This procedure led to a decre- debilitating effect of inescapable shocks oc- ment in subsequent FR-1 shuttle box ac- curs because inescapable shock induces pa- quisition. ralysis. Thus the retarding effect of move- Bracewell and Black (1974) proposed the ment punishment does not imply that in- following interpretation of their data: escapable shock retards acquisition because One explanation that accounts for these results is it punishes movement. Moreover, the ex- the operant conditioning of responses incompatible plicit punishment of movement in the Brace- with the shuttle box escape response that occurred well and Black experiment led to a much in the third case [where movement was explicitly smaller escape acquisition decrement than punished], also occurred in the first two. That did inescapable shock delivered to restrained is, restrained rats may have been punished for struggling and consequently, learned to hold still, subjects. The punishment procedure in- and unrestrained rats may have learned to hold creased the subsequent mean escape latency still or to perform some other incompatible re- across the 10 test trials from the control sponse to reduce the high intensity shock, (p. 67) level of 2.4 sec to 9.1 sec. The inescapable Thus Bracewell and Black (1974) argued shock procedure resulted in a 21.5-sec mean that because explicit punishment of move- escape latency. So it can be argued that the ment produced a retardation in shuttle box explicit training of an incompatible response acquisition, the other instances of retarded did not duplicate the effects of exposure to acquisition might also be due to punishment inescapable shock in restrained subjects, and of movement during the pretreatment phase. so the argument that the movement punish- They went on to argue that the learned ment effect accounts for the inescapable shock helplessness effect might be explainable in effect seems questionable. It can, of course, the same terms. It should be noted that be argued that a more effective or different 22 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN punishment procedure might yield a larger et al. (1975) reported large escape deficits escape decrement, but that remains to be produced by exposure tO; inescapable shock demonstrated. in unrestrained rats. Again there are many Further, it is not clear that Bracewell and differences between studies, but it is clear Black (1974) employed a test procedure that that restraint does not always produce a is generally sensitive to learned helplessness subsequent escape decrement nor does in- effects* In order for an effect to be called a escapable shock have to be delivered to re- learned helplessness effect, it must be demon- strained subjects for a large effect to occur. strated that the effect is caused by the con- A second incompatible motor response trollability of the events delivered during theory has been proposed by Anisman and pretreatment. There is no evidence that Waller (1973). They argued that exposure decrements in FR-1 shuttle box acquisition to shock induces response repertoire changes produced by prior exposure to shock are in the organism, and if no, coping response is caused by the inescapability of the shocks in available, "freezing" becomes the organism's rats. There is not a single study employing dominant reaction to shock. Freezing would, a triadic design in rats (see p. 6) that has of course, compete with ;the occurrence of found appropriate effects with FR-1 shuttling active responding in a subsequent escape/ as the dependent variable. In fact, there avoidance task. As evidence for their hy- have been both reported (Anderson, Schwen- pothesis, Anisman and Waller cite a variety diman & Payne, Note 3; Maier et al., of studies which show that manipulations 1973; Seligman & Beagley, 1975) and un- designed to increase freezing have a detri- reported (Carder, Leaf, Note 4) failures to mental effect on avoidance behavior, and find any effect of prior exposure to shock on manipulations designed to reduce freezing FR-1 .shuttle box escape acquisition in the facilitate avoidance performance. So, for rat. It is to be noted that both Anderson example, strong shock, which causes more et al. and Maier et al. (1973) delievered in- freezing in rats, produced more interference escapable shock to restrained rats and still with shuttle avoidance than weak shock failed to observe any effects. There are a (Anisman & Waller, 1972) and scopalamine, number of differences between the various a drug that reduces freezing, makes rats FR-1 shuttle box experiments, and it is not better avoiders (Anisman, 1973). known which are responsible for the differ- A number of comments are appropriate. ence in results. Nevertheless, the dependent First, the experiments discussed by Anisman variable used by Bracewell and Black may and Waller are not directly relevant to the not be one that is sensitive to helplessness learned helplessness effect. They are all effects in rats and so the implications of studies of effects on avoidance, not escape. their results for learned helplessness is dif- There is no evidence that the manipulations ficult to assess. described by Anisman and Waller affect A further problem concerns the generality escape acquisition. In fact, escape latencies of the effects found by Bracewell and Black are not even reported in many of the experi- (1974). It will be recalled that they found ments (e.g., Anisman, 1973). The learned restraint in the absence of shock to produce helplessness effect is a failure to escape—to a subsequent escape deficit and inescapable flee from shock itself—a more profound shock in the absence of restraint to produce debilitation than failure to prevent shock. only a small escape decrement. However, Second, even if it could be shown that in- Maier et al. (1973) used an escape/avoid- ducing freezing debilitates escape, this does ance task known to be sensitive to helpless- not mean that inescapable shock interferes ness Affects in rats (see p. 8) and found with escape by inducing freezing. In addi- no effect of restraint alone. Cohen (1970) tion, dogs do not seem to freeze, people who found no effect of restraint on shuttle box receive unsolvable discrimination problems escape/avoidance acquisition in dogs. or inescapable noise do not freeze, and organ- Further, Looney and Cohen (1972), Selig- isms that receive noncontingent food do not man and Beagley (1975), and Seligman freeze, yet these conditions may all produce LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 23

helplessness. Finally, why does inescapable exposed to inescapable shocks did not learn shock, but not escapable shock, produce the FR-2 escape response even though they freezing (if it does) in rats? We have not responded rapidly on the five FR-1 trials, been able to think of an answer that does not while all control rats learned the FR-2 imply that the rat has learned that shock is escape response. It should be noted that we inescapable, and this is the heart of the have replicated this effect several times, as helplessness view. have other laboratories (Seligman & Experimental evidence. We feel that there Beagley, 1975; Weisman, Note 5)i are six lines of evidence that bear on the in- The fact that rats exposed to inescapable compatible motor response theories, and shock will learn FR-1 shuttling but will not each will be described. learn FR-2 shuttling might be explained in a variety of ways. The helplessness hy- 1. It has already been noted that rats ex- pothesis asserts that prior exposure to in- posed to inescapable shock often do not fail escapable shock interferes with the subse- to learn an FR-1 shuttle box response. This quent formation of associations between has been investigated by Maier et al. (1973), responding and shock termination and, in and in agreement with a number of previous addition, reduces the organism's incentive to investigators they found that rats exposed attempt to escape. From the foregoing dis- to inescapable shock under a wide range of cussion it is clear that crossing the shuttle parameters subsequently responded as box once is a high probability initial response rapidly as did controls in a two-way shuttle to shock in the rat, perhaps a species-specific box escape/avoidance acquisition test. How- defense response (Bolles, 1970) and, at ever, they noted that the rats in their experi- least on early trials, may be elicited. Con- ments seemed to acquire this shuttling escape sistent with this view of shuttle box crossing, response in a manner different from that Maier et al. (1973) found that if conditions exhibited by dogs. The rats responded very were arranged such that there was no. con- rapidly, but the response was- as rapid on tingency between crossing the shuttle box the very first acquisition trial as it was after and shock termination, naive rats would still 30 trials—the acquisition curves were flat. cross the shuttle box in response to shock The mean response latency from shock onset with short latencies for about 10 trials. on the very first shuttle box trial was 2.67 It should be noted that this strong tend- sec. The escape responding of dogs in a ency of the rat to cross the shuttle box shuttle box is quite different. Averaged rapidly in response to shock guarantees that across experiments, the naive control dogs in the preshocked rats will be exposed to the the Maier, Overmier, and Seligman experi- FR-1 contingency a relatively large number ments took slightly over 30.0 sec of shock of times even if such a rat is slow to learn. before escaping on the first trial and did In addition, the contingency between crossing not emit short latency escape responses the shuttle box and shock termination is until the fourth trial. This suggested to relatively simple under the FR-1 procedure, Maier et al. (1973) that an interference and so the preshocked rats might learn FR-1 effect in rats might be found if a more escape even though associative interference is slowly acquired escape response were used actually present. If associative interference as the test task. The escape response they were present, some difference in FR-1 ac- used was one in which the rats had to cross quisition between inescapably shocked and the shuttle box twice in order to terminate control rats, at least on early trials, might shock. However, these FR-2 trials were pre- be expected. However, such a difference ceded by five ordinary shuttle box trials (FR- could be masked by the elicited nature of 1), since pilot investigation revealed that shuttling on early trials. Consistent with naive rats sometimes fail to acquire the FR-2 this argument, Testa, Juraska, and Maier shuttling response if such prior training was (1974) found that FR-1 shuttling extin- not provided. The result was that a large guished more rapidly in groups given previ- interference effect appeared—half of the rats ous exposure to inescapable shock than in 24 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN controls, even though these groups did not relatively difficult to learn with the FR-2 differ in the acquisition of FR-1 shuttling. procedure, for the obvious reason that shock The FR-2 shuttling response, on the other does not terminate after each crossing. In hand, is not as probable an initial uncondi- the Maier et al. (1973) FR-2 experiment, tioned response to shock as is FR-1 shut- shock was simultaneously presented to the tling, and presents a more complicated con- grids of both chambers of the shuttle box, tingency between crossing the shuttle box so that there was no consequence for the first and shock termination than does FR-1 shut- response of the FR-2, not even a momentary tling. Thus the associative interference pro- interruption of shock presentation. The duced by learning that shock is inescapable contingency between the shuttle response and might be able to prevent the acquisition of shock termination can be improved by ar- can alsb explain why exposure to inescap- ranging a brief interruption of shock fol- the FR-2 shuttle box response. lowing the first response in the FR-2. It Both competing motor response hypotheses should be noted that such a procedure will able shock has little or no effect on FR-1 not change the effortfulness of the FR-2 shuttling. The competing motor response nor will it have an appreciable effect on the theories could argue that the competing duration of shock exposure produced by response is not strong enough to interfere the FR-2 procedure, if the interruption in with the highly elicited FR-1 but can com- shock is very brief. pete with the initially weaker FR-2. In Maier and Testa (1975) conducted pre- addition the FR-2 procedure exposes the cisely the experiment suggested above. It rat to more shock than does the FR-1 pro- was found that although inescapably shocked cedure, and it could be argued that this in- rats fail to learn ordinary FR-2 shuttling, tensifies the competing response. they learn as well as nonshocked controls Although these hypotheses are not mutu- when there is a 1-sec interruption of shock ally exclusive, they point to different aspects after the first crossing of the FR-2. Since of the FR-2 procedure as the crucial feature the only obvious difference between the pro- in producing an interference effect. The help- cedures which did and did not yield a learned lessness5 hypothesis emphasizes the difficulty helplessness effect was the nature of the of learriing the contingency between crossing contingency between responding and shock the shuttle box and shock termination in- termination, it seems likely that associative herent in the FR-2 procedure, while the factors are strongly involved. competing response explanations emphasize The previous experiment suggests that the physical difficulty of the FR-2 response the inescapably shocked rats learn the FR-1 and/or the large amount of shock to which task but do not learn the FR-2 task because the FR-2 procedure exposes an organism. the FR-1 task contains an obvious con- There are two obvious ways to determine tingency between shuttling and shock term- which of these aspects of the FR-2 pro- ination while the FR-2 task contains a de- cedure is crucial. The first is to simplify graded contingency. If this is the case, then the contingency between crossing the shut- it ought to be possible to produce a learned tle box and shock termination while at the helplessness effect with a single crossing of same time maintaining the physical difficulty the shuttle box if the contingency between and shock exposure characteristics of the the single crossing and shock termination FR-2. The second is to arrange a com- were degraded. plex contingency between shuttling and shock In a second experiment, Maier and Testa termination while at the same time reducing (1975) degraded the contingency between the physical difficulty of the response and the a single crossing of the shuttle box and extent of shock exposure. Maier and Testa shock termination by interposing a time de- (1975) have conducted experiments using lay between crossing of the shuttle box and each of these strategies. shock termination. Thus the required The contingency between crossing the escape response was no more effortful than shuttle box and shock termination should be the ordinary FR-1 task which does not LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 25

TABLE 2 DESIGN OF THE MAIER AND RHOADES EXPERIMENT

Phase Group i 2 3

ES-IS Escapable wheel turn Inescapable tube Escapable shuttle box IS-IS Inescapable wheel turn Inescapable tube Escapable shuttle box NS-IS No-shock wheel turn Inescapable tube Escapable shuttle box NS-NS No-shock wheel turn No-shock tube Escapable shuttle box yield an interference effect, but contained could be incompatible with FR-1 shuttling an obscured contingency between shuttling if there is a short delay in shock termination and shock termination, as does the FR-2 but not incompatible when there is no delay ? task which does yield an interference effect. 2. It will be recalled (p. 20) that Selig- We found that a delay in shock termination man and Maier (1967) found that prior ex- as short as 1 sec severely retarded the ac- posure to controllable shock immunizes the quisition of FR-1 shuttle box escape in rats organism against the deleterious effects of previously exposed to inescapable shocks, exposure to uncontrollable aversive events. but had no effect on subjects not previously This finding might seem inconsistent with the exposed to inescapable shocks. It should incompatible motor response hypotheses since be noted that a procedure designed to con- prior exposure to escapable shock should not trol for the extra amount of shock produced prevent the establishment of the incompatible by the delay revealed that this factor did shock-mitigating response or freezing dur- not account for the results. Thus prior ing exposure to uncontrollable shock. How- exposure to inescapable shock will retard ever, in the Seligman and Maier experiment the acquisition of even low-effort escape re- the subjects' initial experience with con- sponses if the contingency between the trollable shock was given with the same task escape responses and shock termination is that was used as the test task following ex- made less obvious. posure to inescapable shock. That is, the Taken together, the results of these two subjects were first given escape training in a experiments indicate that the nature of the shuttle box, then given inescapable shock in contingency between the escape response and a harness, and then tested in a shuttle box. shock termination is more important than are Thus the immunization effect may not have the effort and shock exposure characteristics been caused by the organisms' initial con- of the escape response in determining trol over shock but rather by the acquisition whether an interference effect will occur. of the specific response later used as a test. Inescapably shocked rats fail to learn FR-2 The confounding of initial control over shuttle box escape because the contingency shock with the acquisition of the test task between crossing the shuttle box and shock response allows the incompatible motor re- termination is here more complex, not be- sponse theories to account for the Seligman cause the FR-2 response is effortful or ex- and Maier immunization experiment. They poses the subject to long shock durations. could argue that the Seligman and Maier It is therefore difficult to account for the rat procedure strengthened shuttling sufficiently interference effect with an explanation that to overcome the putative competing re- does not posit an associative deficit following sponse. inescapable shock. Clearly, what is needed is an experiment The incompatible motor response hy- which separates the effects of initial control potheses cannot handle these data. It is over shock and the prior strengthening of difficult to conceive of a motor response that the specific response later to be tested. An would interfere with FR-2 shuttling when unpublished experiment by Maier and there is no shock interruption, but not when Rhoades meets this requirement. (See also there is. Similarly, what motor response Seligman et al., 1975). The design of tfie 26 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

IS-IS0 to inescapable shock, the immunizing effect 35- ES-IS* was reliable. It is difficult for competing 0 response theories to account for an immuniz- 30- NS-IS° /--»o^' \ ing effect that is caused by the experience s- D u NS-NS* / XX ^^.D ° of control per se rather than by the prior 25- / J^ LU strengthening of the test response. It is H- difficult to argue that learning to turn a small «t f _J 20" v!> \- — wheel with the paws in a wheel-turn box directly strengthens running in a shuttle < LU is- o %S *^ box. This wheel-turn escape training should E a not have an immunizing effect, but it does. le- 3. If inescapable shock interferes with subsequent escape acquisition because it has s' led to the development of a motor response incompatible with the escape response, then i i i i i i the explicit reinforcement of such a response 1 23456 should duplicate the effects of exposure to BLOCKS OF 5 TRIALS inescapable shock. As already noted, Brace- well and Black (1974) found that the explicit FIG. 7. Mean shuttle box FR-2 escape response latency for rats previously given either escapable punishment of movement led to a small shock (ES), yoked inescapable shock (IS), or no decrement in subsequent acquisition in a shock (NS) in a wheel-turn apparatus, and then shuttle box. However, this experiment is given either inescapable shock (IS) or no shock difficult to interpret for the reasons already (NS) in a restraining tube (Maier & Rhoades, un- noted (see pp. 21-22). Another study using published data, 1975). (The rats given escape training in the wheel-turn box and then given in- this strategy was reported by Maier (1970). escapable shocks in the restraining tube [ES-IS] One group of dogs was trained to escape were not helpless in the shuttle box.) shock by holding still, a response directly incompatible with jumping over a hurdle in experiment can be seen in Table 2. The a shuttle box. The dogs were restrained in experiment involved three phases and four a hammock and panels were placed | in. groups of rats. In the first phase one group (.64 cm) above and | in. (.64 cm) to each of rats was trained to escape shock by turn- side of the dog's head. The dogs could not ing a small wheel with their paws, a second prevent the onset of shock, but they could group received yoked inescapable shock in a terminate shock by not touching any of the wheel-turn box, and two groups were placed panels for a specified period of time once the in the wheel-turn box but not shocked. In shock came on. Since shock elicits head the second phase both of the shocked groups movements in restrained dogs, the dogs and one of the control groups received in- were required to actively inhibit movements escapable shock in a restraining tube, and elicited by shock. Because the panels were the remaining group was only restrained. In positioned 1 in. (.64 cm) from the dog's the third phase all groups were given five head, the dogs could refrain from touching trials of FR-1 followed by 25 trials of FR-2 the panels only by remaining almost per- escape/avoidance training in a shuttle box, fectly motionless, by freezing. A second our standard helplessness test task. group of dogs received inescapable shock The results found by Maier and Rhoades yoked to the first group and a third received are seen in Figure 7. Prior experience with no pretreatment. Following this treatment escapable shock in the wheel-turn situation all groups received 12 days of testing in a mitigated the interfering effects of inescap- shuttle box. able shock on shuttle box escape acquisition. If inescapable shock produces a learned Although prior experience with escapable helplessness effect because it induces freez- shock in the wheel-turn situation did not ing or leads to the punishment of movement, completely eliminate the effects of exposure then the group taught to inhibit movement as LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 27

40-1 of the learned helplessness effect. It could, of course, be argued that exposure to in- ^ „-_. — - escapable shock establishes a response that is S 30- CO stronger or even more incompatible with \ YOKED- • shuttling than the one trained in the present £ experiment; the burden of proof, however, E 20- \ """A NAIVE-— would clearly be shifted onto such an argu- ae ment. 4. It will be recalled that Bracewell and £ 10- Black (1974) argued that movement occurs V v during inescapable shock and is punished, 0- presumably because it hurts to struggle. 11 16 21 26 Alternatively, Bracewell and Black sug- BLOCKS OF 5 TRIALS gested that the subject might find some other motor response that reduces the. pain FIGURE 8. Mean latency to respond in a shuttle box for dogs previously given training to escape produced by shock. This position predicts shock by holding still (DRO), given yoked in- that if the subject is not allowed to move escapable shocks, or given no shocks. (The yoked during exposure to inescapable shock, no group did not learn, whereas the group taught to learned helplessness should result. If move- hold still did learn.) (From "Failure to Escape ment does not occur it cannot produce an Traumatic Shock: Incompatible Skeletal Motor Responses or Learned Helplessness?" by Steven increment in pain and so be punished, and if F. Maier, Learning and Motivation, 1970, 1, 157- movement does not occur the subject can- 170. Copyright 1970 by Academic Press. Re- not find a motor response that mitigates printed by permission.) shock. Overmier and Seligman (1967) prevented a means of escaping shock should later movement during exposure to inescapable escape at least as poorly as the group given shock by the administration of curare. Cu- inescapable shock. The learned helplessness rarization during exposure to inescapable hypothesis makes a different prediction. Even shock did not prevent or even reduce the though the dogs have learned an incompat- subsequent learned helplessness effect. It ible motor response, they have also learned seems to us that this experiment is a direct that they have control over shock; and even disconfjrmation of the Bracewell and Black if they should be slow to learn to escape in (1974) hypothesis." However, Bracewell the shuttle box, they should eventually learn. and Black have argued that this curare ex- In contrast, dogs given inescapable shocks periment does not necessarily contradict should not learn to escape in the new situa- their position.. Their argument is that move- tion ; experiences of failure to escape on the ment-related neural processes might be early trials in the new situation should adventitiously reinforced or punished during strengthen the assumed cause of interference. curarization: "A possibility exists, there- The results of this experiment can be fore, that the onset and termination of shock seen in Figure 8. The group (labeled DRO) could act inadvertently to reinforce super- trained to perform a response incompatible stitious movement related neural responses with shuttling was slow to learn shuttling, in curarized dogs" (Bracewell & Black, but all subjects in this group eventually 1974, p. 55). It should be carefully noted learned. In contrast, half of the subjects that this is different from their original posi- given inescapable shock entirely failed to tion. The Bracewell and Black hypothesis learn across the 12 days of testing. Thus the training of a response demonstrably in- compatible with shuttling (negative transfer slt should be noted that this experiment has no was produced) did not duplicate the effects implications for the Anisman and Waller (1973) competing motor response theory. There is no of inescapable shock, and this is inconsistent reason to believe that curarization during inescap- with competing motor response explanations able shock should reduce any subsequent freezing. 28 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN argued that an incompatible response is ac- count for appetitive analogues to the learned quired because it produces a positive out- helplessness effect (see p. 12) in animals or come of reduced pain or discomfort. Thus learned helplessness effects in human beings Bracewell and Black provided a mechanism induced by exposure to loud noises (see p. whereby the incompatible response is ac- 9). Moreover, it is difficult to see how quired, an explicit reinforcement mechanism. any incompatible motor response view could But in the case of the curarized subject account for the noise-escape failure produced Bracewell and Black do not argue that "the by unsolvable discrimination problems in movement related neural processes" reduce human subjects (see p. 11). It should be pain and are therefore explicitly reinforced noted that the incompatible motor response since there is no obvious way that such hypotheses which we have discussed were processes could reduce pain. Thus Brace- designed to account only for the effects of well and Black argue that in the case of inescapable shock in animals, and it could curarized subjects movement-related neural be argued that these other effects are differ- processes are adventitiously rather than ex- ent phenomena and therefore need not all be plicitly reinforced, and this seems gratuitous. explained by the same theory. Such a move Although there is evidence that movement- seems ad hoc and our preference is for that related neural processes can be explicitly theory which is able to integrate the largest punished (Black, Young, & Batenchuk, amount of data. 1970), there is no evidence that they can be adventitiously or superstitiously reinforced. Motor Activation Deficit Hypothesis Further, even if such processes could be A somewhat different motor response ex- adventitiously reinforced or punished there is planation of the learned helplessness phe- no reason to assume that the overt movement nomenon has been offered by Weiss et al., produced by such a process would be incom- (1975). Their explanation of the learned patible with shuttling. It could just as easily helplessness effect is as follows: facilitate shuttling. To assume that it will be incompatible is, again, gratuitous. When animals were exposed to severe inescapable 5. We have already described an experi- shock, a deficiency in central naradrenergic activity occurred, deriving in part at least, from the de- ment by Rosellini and Seligman (1975) (see pletion in the level of norepinephrine seen in this p. 10) in which rats were given escapable condition. As a consequence of this noradrenergic shock, inescapable shock, or no shock, and deficiency the animals could mediate only a limited were then trained to traverse a runway for amount of motor activity, an amount insufficient for learning and performance of the correct re- food. Following training, the rats were sponse in the shuttle avoidance task on which they extinguished and then allowed to escape were tested. On the other hand, animals exposed from the presumably frustrating goal box. to the same shocks but able to control then! did The result was that the rats previously ex- not develop this noradrenergic deficiency and there- posed to inescapable shock were slow to fore could mediate sufficient motor behavior to perform adequately, (p. 7) escape the goal box but did not differ from controls in either acquisition or extinction. A number of factors led Weiss et al. (1975) If a motor response is established during to develop this explanation of the learned exposure to inescapable shock, it is difficult helplessness effect. First, Miller and Weiss to see why it would interfere with escape (1969) argued that the dissipation of the from the goal box but not interfere with learned helplessness effect over a 48-hour runway acquisition and extinction. interval following one session of exposure 6. The incompatible motor response to inescapable shock in dogs suggests media- theories have difficulty dealing with situa- tion by a time-dependent physiological change tions that do not employ electric shocks or rather than by learning. They argued that other noxious unconditioned stimuli. The things that are learned do not disappear particular competing response hypotheses across an interval as short as 48 hours. offered by Bracewell and Black (1974) and Second, it is known that central catechol- by Anisman and Waller (1973) cannot ac- amines have a time course of recovery fol- LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 29 lowing depletion (e.g., Rech, Bovys, & expect a forced swim in cold water to pro- Moore, 1966) and are involved in the media- duce a learned helplessness effect. However, tion of movement (e.g., Herman, 1970). a forced swim in warm water (28 °C) does Third, Weiss (1968, 1971a, 1971b, 1971c) not produce a depletion of brain norepi- has shown that rats given inescapable shock nephrine and so the motor activation deficit undergo greater stress than do rats given hypothesis would not expect a warm swim to equivalent escapable shocks, as measured by produce a learned helplessness effect. Weiss gastric lesions, loss of body weight, plasma et al. argued that animals forced to swim in steroid levels, and fearfulness. Finally, this either warm or cold water should experi- led Weiss, Stone, and Harrell (1970) to ence the same lack of control, and that the assay whole brain norepinephrine following learned helplessness hypothesis would thus exposure to escapable or yoked inescapable expect similar effects resulting from a warm shock. They found norepinephrine levels to or a cold swim. In Experiment 1, rats were be lower following exposure to inescapable put in either warm (28 °C) or cold (2 °C) shock. This finding has been replicated in water for either 3.5 or 6.5 minutes. Thirty separate brain regions by Weiss, Pohorecky, minutes later the rats were all given an FR-1 Dorros, Williams, Emmel, Whittlesey, and shuttle box shock escape/avoidance acquisi- Case (Note 6) and has been shown to oc- tion test. The results were that the rats ex- cur regardless of the amount of motor activ- posed to cold swim were severely retarded ity required to escape and avoid shock in shuttle box acquisition, while the warm- (Weiss, Pohorecky, Emmel, & Miller, cited swim subjects showed normal acquisition. in Weiss et al., 1975). Following these Experiment 1 led Weiss et al. (1975) to demonstrations of effect on endogenous conclude that cold swim duplicated the fail- norepinephrine levels, Weiss, Pohorecky, ure-to-learn aspect of the learned helpless- McMenima, Berkeley, and Jaffe (cited in ness phenomenon. Another feature of the Weiss et al., 1975) estimated norepinephrine learned helplessness phenomenon is that reuptake and release in vivo by infusion of prior training to escape in the shuttle box radioactive (3H) norepinephrine. They eliminates the interfering effects of sub- found that reuptake occurred at a higher sequent exposure to inescapable shock. In rate in rats previously exposed to inescap- Experiment 2 Weiss et al. gave rats 25 able than escapable shocks. This reuptake trials of FR-1 shuttle box training 24 hours difference could account for the differences before a 3.5 minute cold swim. The rats in the level of endogenous norepinephrine. were again tested in the shuttle box 30 Weiss et al. (1975) present 12 experi- minutes after the cold swim. The pretrain- ments in support of the notion that the ing in the shuttle box reduced the deficit learned helplessness effect occurs because produced by cold swim. Another feature of inescapable shock is a severe stressor, pro- the learned helplessness phenomenon is that duces a temporary depletion of norepi- it has a time course in dogs following a nephrine, and thus depresses the movement single session of inescapable shock. In Ex- the organism can emit below what is re- periment 3 Weiss et al. gave rats a cold quired for shuttle box acquisition. We will swim and then tested them in a shuttle box describe each of these 12 experiments. In 30 minutes, 2 hours, or 48 hours later. With the first three experiments, Weiss et al. a 48-hour delay, there was no longer any attempted to show that a procedure which detectable effect of the cold swim. Further, deplete norepinephrine but does not involve rats tested at the 30-minute point were re- the factors stressed by the learned helpless- ness hypothesis duplicates the learned help- tested 48 hours later. A deficit was still lessness phenomenon. It is known that a present upon retest, as was the case with forced swim in 2 °C water rapidly depletes learned helplessness. These three experi- brain norepinephrine levels and reduces its ments led Weiss et al. to conclude that cold release (Stone, 1970a, 1970b). Thus the swim duplicates the effects of inescapable motor activation deficit hypothesis would shocks. 30 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

The next six experiments employed a dif- deficit produced by cold swim or inescap- ferent strategy. In these experiments, Weiss able shock. In addition, these experiments et al. attempted to find situations in which confirmed the finding that norepinephrine is the motor deficit and the learned helpless- not depleted after repeated stress. ness hypotheses make different or opposed A variety of drug treatments were ex- predictions. The motor activation deficit plored by Weiss et al. in the final three ex- hypothesis implies that the magnitude and the periments. Experiment 10 found that tetra- likelihood of finding a failure to learn to benazine, a relatively nonspecific depletor of escape depends on the amount of motor monoamines, produced poor performance in activity required to escape. Weiss et al. a shuttle box 30 minutes after administra- claimed that the learned helplessness hy- tion. Experiment 11 showed that repeated pothesis would not expect the amount of administration of tetrabenazine did not af- effort required to escape to be a potent fect shuttle box performance. Finally, variable. Experiment 4 by Weiss et al. Experiment 12 found that pargyline, a drug demonstrated that the detrimental effects of which inhibits monoamine oxidase and there- cold swim are strengthened by increasing the fore prevents intraneuronal degradation of height of the barrier in the shuttle box, and monoamines, prevented inescapable shock Experiment 5 showed that cold swim did not from affecting FR-1 shuttle box perform- produce a deficit in learning to escape shock ance. when the escape response only required re- Discussion of the motor activation deficit strained rats to poke their nose through a hypothesis. We have presented the entire hole in the front of the tube in which they logical and evidential bases of the motor were confined. In Experiment 6 Weiss deficit hypothesis without comment or in- et al. attempted to demonstrate that exposure terruption because it presents, at least on the to inescapable shock interferes with acquisi- surface, a convincing case and should be tion of escape behavior in their FR-1 test seen as such by the reader. However, we task. They mentioned that they have had feel that every step in the argument which difficulty in producing such an effect, but suggested the motor deficit hypothesis and showed that a 50-min. exposure to 4.0 mA each piece of data which seems to support 2.0-sec shocks occurring every 20 sec pro- the hypothesis is questionable and open to duces an escape deficit when the rat is tested alternative interpretation, and we will dis- 30 min later. Experiment 7 showed that cuss each, point by point. Following this such shocks interfered with shuttle box but discussion we will present a variety of lines not with nose-poke acquisition, as predicted of evidence which we feel are inconsistent by the motor deficit hypothesis. with the motor deficit hypothesis. The next three experiments by Weiss et It will be recalled that a physiological de- al. are based on data (Zigmond & Harvey, pletion hypothesis was first suggested by 1970) which indicate that the central nora- Miller and Weiss (1969), who argued that drenergic response to stress habituates with the time course of the learned helplessness repeated exposure to stress. That is, the effect is not consistent with the notion that degree of norepinephrine depletion produced something learned during exposure to in- by a stressor is much reduced if the organ- escapable shock is the cause of the effect. ism has been repeatedly stressed previously. They argued that if something is learned it This leads the motor activation deficit hy- should still be present 48 hours later. Al- pothesis to predict that repeated exposures though this point is not crucial to the motor to the stressful condition should reduce any deficit hypothesis, it should be pointed out subsequent deficit in a test task. The learned that it is highly debatable. Proactive and helplessness hypothesis surely does not make retroactive interference produce memory such a prediction. In Experiments 8 and 9 losses in animals (Maier, Allaway, & Gleit- Weiss et al. found that 14 sessions of ex- man, 1967; Maier & Gleitman, 1967) as posure to cold swim or inescapable shock well as man, and both proactive and retro- eliminated the FR-1 shuttle box escape active interference effects generally increase LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 31 with time since learning. It is possible that shock with mongrel dogs not raised in the prior experiences with learning to control laboratory. The time course data taken as events could interfere with the retention of a whole does not invalidate a learning inter- the learning that shocks are uncontrollable pretation and may be more consistent with after only one sesison with inescapable the learned helplessness hypothesis than with shocks. In addition, there is ample evidence the motor activation deficit hypothesis. in animals for retention losses over short Of greater importance in establishing the intervals of time (see D'Amato, 1973; Spear, plausibility of the motor activation deficit 1973). hypothesis are the several demonstrations by More importantly, Weiss et al. (1975) Weiss and his colleagues that exposure to ignored aspects of the learned helplessness inescapable shock is highly stressful and re- time course data which are not as congenial duces endogenous norepinephrine levels to their hypothesis as is the fact that the while exposure to escapable shock is less effect does not occur after 48 hours follow- stressful and does not lead to such a reduc- ing a single experience with inescapable tion. It is this that allows the motor deficit shocks. Seligman and Groves (1970) have hypothesis to explain why the learned help- shown that the time course of the learned lessness effect occurs and is determined by helplessness effect is eliminated in dogs if the controllability of the initial shock. Sev- the dogs are given four sessions rather than eral comments are in order. Weiss et al. one session of inescapable shock. Such dogs (1975) argue that the learned helplessness failed to learn to escape even when tested effects which we have found are caused by 7 days after their last exposure to inescap- norepinephrine depletion produced by the able shock. Further, if dogs are raised in inescapable shock which we deliver. There- laboratory cages and thus deprived of ex- fore, it is required that Weiss and his col- tensive experience controlling events, only leagues demonstrate that our inescapable two sessions of inescapable shock are re- shock conditions produce intense stress and quired to produce nontransient learned help- norepinephrine depletion. It is thus crucial lessness effects (Seligman & Groves, 1970). to inquire whether the experiments by Weiss It should be noted that these are precisely and his colleagues employed conditions the results that would be expected from a roughly similar to ours. Table 3 summarizes memory interpretation of the time course and some of the relevant aspects of our rat help- are not explicable by the motor deficit hy- lessness experiments and the Weiss studies. pothesis in any obvious way. Norepi- As can be seen, the behavioral helplessness nephrine levels should certainly be recovered studies use 1.0-mA shocks with session by 7 days following exposure to inescapable lengths lasting from 1 to 1-J hours. The shock. Finally, Seligman and Beagley rat's behavior is tested 24 hours or longer (1975) failed to find a time course of learned after the inescapable shock session. The helplessness with rats as subjects after only conditions used by Weiss and his colleagues one session of inescapable shock. Rats bear no resemblance at all to these condi- given inescapable shock failed to learn to tions. All but one of the norepinephrine escape when tested 7 days after exposure to studies exposed the subjects to a minimum of a single session of inescapable shock. This \20 hours of inescapable shock, and shock in- finding is particularly damaging to the motor tensity was a minimum of 3.0 mA. Further, deficit hypothesis because this hypothesis norepinephrine was typically assayed im- rests on a base of data obtained with rats, mediately following the end of the inescap- not dogs, as subjects. It would be interest- able shock session. It is worth noting that ing if rats raised in a natural environment, the one norepinephrine study which deviated rather than in laboratory cages, showed a from these parameters, even though still transient helplessness effect. In summary, very different from our parameters (Weiss the transient effect required by the motor et al., 1970, Experiment 1), did not find deficit hypothesis occurs only under one depletion of norepinephrine in the group condition—a single session of inescapable given inescapable shocks. This experiment 32 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

TABLE 3 SUMMARY OF THE PARAMETERS USED IN THE RAT LEARNED HELPLESSNESS EXPERIMENTS

Maximum Session Time between inescapable Experiment shock level length shock and test Behavioral helplessness Maier, Albin, & Testa 1.0mA IJhr. 24 hr. Goeckner, Greenough, & Maier 1.0mA 1 hr. 24 hr. Testa, Juraska, & Maier 1.0mA Hhr. 24 hr. Maier & Testa 1.0mA l| hr. 24 hr. Seligman & Beagley 1.0mA IJhr. 24 hr. Seligman, Rosellini, & Kozak 1,0mA lihr. S min.; 1 hr. ; 24 hr. ; 168 hr. Seligman, Hannum, & Rosellini 1.0mA IJhr, 65 days Ulcers, steroids Weiss (1968), Experiment 2 1.6mA 21 hr. 12 hr. Weiss (1971a) 4.0 mA 48 hr. none Weiss (1971b) 4.0 mA 48 hr. none Weiss (1971c) 4.0mA 48 hr. none Norepinephrine Weiss, Stone & Harrell, Experiment 1 3.0mA 3hr. 20 min. ; 40 min. Weiss, Pohorecky, Dorros, Williams, Emmel, Whittlesey, & Case 3.4mA 20 hr. none Weiss, Pohorecky, Emmel, & Miller not known 24 hr. none Weiss, Pohorecky, Emmel, McMenima, Berkeley, & Jaffe not known 20 hr. 2 min. found a reliable overall difference in norepi- tion to only the dog helplessness experi- nephrine levels between inescapable, escap- ments, and not the rat helplessness experi- able, and no-shock groups. However, the ments. However, all of the data obtained inescapable group did not differ reliably from by Weiss and his colleagues came from rats, the no-shock group, and the overall differ- and so we feel that a comparison of their ence was due to an elevation of norepi- parameters with those used in our rat studies nephrine in the escapable shock group. has been proper. How can one argue that It should be clear that, in order to ex- data obtained from rats apply to data ob- plain the learned helplessness effect, the tained from dogs but not to parallel data ob- motor activation deficit hypothesis requires tained from rats? Further, the parameters that a 1-hour session employing 1.0-mA used in our dog studies also differ massively shocks (occurring about one per minute) from those used by Weiss et al.—we have produce a depletion in norepinephrine which typically used 64 shocks spread over about is still present 24 hours following the termi- 1^ hours and have tested the dogs 24 hours nation of the inescapable shock session. later. There is not a shred of evidence for such an Moreover, Weiss et al, (1975) have re- effect. It is a curious argument that holds ported that endogenous levels of norepi- a behavioral deficit which occurs 24 hours nephrine are depleted in rats given inescap- after a 1-hour session with 1.0-mA shocks is able shock only if the rats were group housed attributable to norepinephrine depletion be- prior to the experiment. That is, in all of cause norepinephrine levels are low when the neurochemical studies referred to above, measured immediately after massive 20-hour the rats were group housed before treatment. sessions with 3.4- or 4.0-mA shocks. If the rats were individually housed before At this point it should be noted that Weiss treatment inescapable shock did not deplete et al. (1975) have restricted their explana- endogenous norepinephrine levels. This may LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 33 be because individual housing in rats induces certainly not equally aversive, and warm increased activity of tyrosine hydroxylase swim may not be aversive at all. Further, (Segal, Knapp, Kuczenski, & Mandell, although we do not wish to argue in this 1973), a rate-limiting enzyme in the syn- direction, it is not clear to us why cold swim thesis of norepinephrine. It should be noted cannot be described as a condition in which that the rats in all of our experiments have escape from the highly aversive freezing wa- been individually housed prior to treatment. ter is independent of behavior. Cold swim In fact, Goeckner, Greenough, and Maier produces muscular debilitation while warm (1974) manipulated housing conditions from swim does not. Thus the trauma in a cold weaning to adulthood and found that in- swim may indeed be more uncontrollable escapable shock produced a learned helpless- than in a warm swim. ness effect whether the rats were group More important than whether the learned housed or individually housed. helplessness hypothesis predicts equivalent We conclude that the neurochemical data effects of cold and warm swim, we do not said to provide plausibility to the motor believe that Weiss et al. (1975) have demon- activation deficit explanation of learned help- strated that cold swim produces a behavioral lessness, in fact, provide little if any support. deficit that is the same as the learned help- We now turn to a consideration of the 12 lessness effect. Learned helplessness is de- experiments reported by Weiss et al. (1975) fined as an effect resulting from the uncon- as supporting the motor deficit interpreta- trollability of aversive events. As noted tion of the learned helplessness effect. Re- previously, FR-1 shuttle box acquisition may call that the first three experiments were not be sensitive to such effects in rats, and a designed to show two things: (a) that a variety of experimenters have failed to find treatment which depletes norepinephrine any effect of inescapable shock on subse- (cold swim) but cannot be said to lead to quent FR-1 shuttle box acquisition. There the learning that aversive events and re- are a few experiments which find an effect sponding are independent produces all of the of inescapable shock on subsequent FR-1 characteristics of the learned helplessness shuttle box acquisition (e.g., Weiss et al., phenomenon; (b) that a procedure which 1975, Experiment 6) but these experiments involves the same degree of uncontrol- omit comparison of the effects of equivalent lability as the first but does not deplete escapable and inescapable shock so it can- norepinephrine (warm swim) does not pro- not be concluded that a learned helplessness duce a learned helplessness effect. We do effect with FR-1 shuttling has been demon- not feel that the experiments actually demon- strated. Since Weiss et al. employed FR-1 strate that which is claimed. shuttling as their dependent variable, they Recall that Weiss et al. compared the ef- may have been using a test that is not sensi- fects of a cold swim and a warm swim on tive to learned helplessness effects. FR-1 shuttle box acquisition and argued Further, Weiss et al. (1975) used a 30- that the learned helplessness hypothesis minute interval between cold swim and the would expect equivalent effects from these behavioral task, whereas our learned help- two treatments since they involve equal un- lessness experiments use a 24-hour interval controllability. This is not correct. The between inescapable shock exposure and test. learned helplessness hypothesis does not Thus Weiss et al. must demonstrate that argue that failure to learn to escape shock cold swim produces a deficit 24 hours later, results from exposure to sheer uncontrol- not 30 minutes later. Experiment 3 of lability, but by exposure to uncontrollable Weiss et al. examined the time course of the aversive events. For example, the learned cold-swim effect, but it does not help us helplessness hypothesis would not expect here because Weiss et al. did not have a 24- failure to escape shock to result from ex- hour time point. Their experiment only posure to a light presented independently of involved testing at 30 minutes, 2 hours, or behavior. Cold swim and warm swim are 48 hours. An examination of the Weiss et al. 34 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

COLD 30'—» rats that failed to learn to escape 30 minutes following cold swim after an additional 48 GOLD 24°"-"° hours. A deficit was still present and Weiss SHOCK 30 — et al. concluded that this duplicates the cor- SHOCK 24-— responding characteristic of the learned help- 25 lessness phenomenon in dogs. We do not think so. In the dog experiments, there was 20 no improvement at all during retest; the dogs showed no tendency to learn. This 15 was not true of the rats in the Weiss et al. study. There were 25 trials on the retest, 10 and by the last block of 5 trials the response

-^. ——•*.., latency of the cold swim rats was down to 5 about 7 sec, as compared with about 4 sec in a control group. In fact, in a later ex- periment (Experiment 7) the retest effect 1 23456 disappeared completely, although Weiss et al. did not comment on this aspect of the BLOCKS OF 5 TRIALS data. The final experiment (Experiment 2 in FIG. 9. Mean shuttle box FR-1 escape response Weiss et al., 1975) said to reveal the identity latency for rats given either cold swim or inescap- able shocks 3 minutes or 24 hours before the shut- of the cold swim and learned helplessness tle box test (Jackson & Maier, unpublished data effects is the immunization effect. It is 1975). (Only the rats given cold swim 30 minutes worth noting that although Weiss et al. before the test failed to respond.) found prior training in the shuttle box to reduce the effects of cold swim on shuttle data reveals that the cold-swim effect was box performance, the reduction was not almost absent at 2 hours. However, the complete; some debilitation still occurred. 24-hour point is still needed, and Maier has The immunizing effect on learned helpless- now supplied the missing point. Jackson ness is complete in both dogs (Seligman & and Maier (unpublished data) gave rats 3.5 Maier, 1967) and rats (Seligman et al., minutes of cold swim in 2 °C water and 1975) when the immunization treatment is tested them in a FR-1 shuttle box task 30 given in the shuttle box. minutes or 24 hours later. The results can We conclude that the experiments pro- be seen in Figure 9. Jackson and Maier vided by Weiss et al. (1975) do not demon- found the same debilitation as found by strate that the behavioral deficit produced Weiss et al. 30 minutes after cold swim, but by cold swim is the same as the deficit pro- there was no deficit at all if 24 hours had duced by the uncontrollable shocks used in intervened. It should also be noted that the the learned helplessness experiments. Even learned helplessness effect in rats, the effects more convincing to us than the discrepancy of exposure to uncontrollable shock on sub- between the Weiss et al. and the learned help- sequent escape acquisition in a task sensitive lessness experiments are our observations of to controllability, may not show any dissipa- subjects 30 minutes after cold swim, the tion with time. point at which Weiss et al. tested such sub- Another aspect of Experiment 3 is worthy jects. A severe motor debilitation does ap- of comment. Weiss et al. (1975) note that pear to be present. The rats have great dif- dogs that were tested 24 hours after ex- ficulty moving and simply remain motionless posure to inescapable shock and that failed when placed on a table. Further, such rats to learn to escape, would fail to learn again have some difficulty righting themselves if retested later. That is, the failure be- when placed on their sides. When they are came permanent. Weiss et al. retested the removed from the 2 °C water they are com- LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 35 pletely limp. Stone (1970b) has extensively livered whereas the learned helplessness ex- studied the effects of cold swim. Observing periments with rats used 64 to 80 1.0-mA the rats after a 20-minute swim in 14.5 °C shocks. The level of shock used by Weiss water, Stone notes: et al. is almost of tetanizing intensity and may indeed lead to a trauma-induced motor Shortly after the stress, the swim stressed rats appear exhausted; they lie in a prostrate position, deficit 30 minutes later. Our dog experi- are unable to move their rear limbs, and have dif- ments used 64 6-mA shocks, and testing oc- ficulty righting themselves when laid on one side. curred 24 hours later. It is difficult to com- During the later stages of recovery, the animals pare shock intensities across species as dif- remain immobile in a crouched position and show ptosis with continued and violent shivering, (p. 53) ferent as dogs and rats, but our observa- tions indicate 4 mA in rats to be much more We have observed similar behavior after severe than 6 mA in dogs. Jackson and 3.5 minutes in 2 °C water. Maier (unpublished data) investigated The inescapable shocks used in our learned whether exposure to shocks characteristic of helplessness experiments do not produce any- learned helplessness experiments would pro- thing like these obvious signs of motor debil- duce the sort of deficit reported by Weiss itation. As noted previously, the shocks et al. Rats were exposed to 64 1.0-mA which we use have no effect on runway ac- shocks and tested in an FR-1 shuttle box quisition 24 hours later, nor do they affect task either 30 minutes or 24 hours later. As FR-1 shuttle box performance. We doubt can be seen in Figure 10, these shocks did that Weiss's subjects could even locomote to not produce a deficit even 30 minutes later. the end of a runway 30 minutes after cold Thus Experiments 6 and 7 employed condi- swim. Thus we conclude that Weiss et al. tions very different from those employed in (1975) are indeed correct in arguing that the learned helplessness experiments with cold swim produces a motor deficit, perhaps rats and may indeed be correctly interpreted via norepinephrine depletion. However, in terms of a motor deficit. The fact that there is no reason to believe that the levels 150 4.0-mA shocks produce a motor deficit of inescapable shock used in the learned 30 minutes later does not imply that 64 1.0- helplessness dog or rat experiments produce mA shocks produce a motor deficit 24 hours a motor deficit 24 hours later. later. The factors just discussed are also im- The conceptual base for Experiments 8 portant in interpreting the subsequent ex- and 9 may indeed be adequate for a test be- periments by Weiss et al. in which the motor tween the motor activation deficit and activation deficit hypothesis and the learned learned helplessness explanations of the helplessness hypothesis were said to make learned helplessness phenomenon. Weiss et opposed predictions. Experiments 4 and 5 al. noted that the reduction in norepinephrine demonstrated that the amount of motor levels produced by exposure to stressors activity required to perform the task re- habituates with repeated exposure to the sponse given 30 minutes after cold swim stressor. Thus if inescapable shock pro- determined whether a deficit occurred, and duces a learned helplessness effect because thus supported the notion that cold swim re- it produces a motor deficit through norepi- sults in a temporary motor deficit. These nephrine depletion, repeated exposure to in- experiments do support this notion, but escapable shock should not produce a learned there is nothing in the learned helplessness helplessness effect. The learned helpless- position which denies that a cold swim pro- ness hypothesis certainly does not make this duces a motor deficit. The learned helpless- ness position does not predict this, but there prediction. It will be recalled that Weiss is no reason why it should. et al. found that rats repeatedly exposed to Experiments 6 and 7 of Weiss et al. cold swim or to 4.0-mA shocks did indeed (1975) use electric shocks during pretreat- perform well when tested on FR-1 shuttling. ment, but 150 4.0-mA shocks were de- On the surface this result seems quite deci- 36 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

Many of the comments made thus far are 60 also appropriate to Experiments 10 through 12. Recall that in these experiments Weiss 50 et al. (1975) found that pharmacological INESC. depletion of monoamines produced poor FR- 1 shuttle box performance 30 minutes later, • 40 and that pharmacological blockade of mono- amine degradation prevented the effects of 5 30 inescapable shock 30 minutes later in a FR-1 Ul shuttle box task. This is consistent with the 2 20 notion that Weiss et al. are studying a motor debilitation mediated by monoamines, but as CONTROL already noted, this may be a very different i 10 phenomenon from learned helplessness. We conclude that the experiments pre- sented by Weiss et alt do not strongly sup- 1234 port the motor activation deficit hypothesis BLOCKS OF 10 TRIALS as an explanation of the learned helplessness effect. It should be stressed that we are FIGURE 10. Mean FR-3 lever press latencies for rats given 15 days of inescapable shocks or no not arguing that the learned helplessness shock (Seligman & Rosellini, unpublished data). hypothesis can account for the data presented (The inescapable shock group did not learn.) by Weiss et al. We think it likely that Weiss et al. were quite correct in their interpretation of their own data. Weiss sive. However, it should be recognized that et al. have demonstrated a number of very the Weiss et al. logic demands that repeated interesting effects, but they may not be exposure to the same shock conditions as employed in learned helplessness experiments prevents a deficit on a task known to be INESC.- sensitive to the degree of control which 35 ESC.-^ organisms have over initial shock experience. Thus we have each independently repeated 30 CONTROL the Weiss et al. (1975) experiment with the C9 shock parameters normally used in our 25 laboratories and with test tasks known to be J sensitive to learned helplessness of control- 20 lability effects. Rosellini and Seligman (un- published data) gave one group 15 daily 15 sessions of 1.0-mA inescapable shocks. A second group received no treatment. All 10- three groups were then tested in an FR-3 level pressing task. Maier employed 10 days 5" of exposure to either escapable, inescapable, or no shocks in a wheel-turn apparatus and • i^ i i i i then tested his subjects in an FR-2 shuttling 123456 task. The shock level was 1.0 mA. The results of these experiments can be seen in BLOCKS OF 5 TRIALS Figures 10 and 11. It is clear that repeated FIGURE 11. Mean FR-2 shuttle box response exposure to 1.0-mA shocks does not reduce latencies for rats given 10 days of escapable, yoked inescapable, or no shocks (Maier, unpublished data, the deficit seen in tasks known to be sensi- 1975). (The inescapable shock subjects failed to tive to learned helplessness effects. learn.) LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 37

closely related to learned helplessness effects. shock in a shuttle box eliminated the inter- The basic similarity is that in both cases fering effects of subsequent inescapable animals failed to escape shock, but there shocks on shuttle box performance. The may be many roots to producing such a motor activation deficit hypothesis is able to deficit, and they may not reduce to one ex- account for this immunizing effect by argu- planation. The deficit produced by expos- ing that pretraining in the shuttle box reduces ure to extremely traumatic events as used the amount of motor behavior required dur- by Weiss et al. may be produced by a very ing testing because the subject does not different mechanism from the deficits pro- have to search for the correct response—it duced by exposure to much less traumatic has already been trained. We have pre- uncontrollable aversive events in the learned sented new data (p. 26) indicating that prior helplessness experiments. experience with controllable shock has an Other evidence. There are a number of immunizing effect even when such prior ex- findings not considered by Weiss et al. perience is given with a different response which are difficult for the motor activation from that later used in testing. The motor deficit hypothesis 'to explain. Many of these activation deficit hypothesis cannot explain have already been described and only a brief this effect. Prior training to turn a wheel discussion is necessary. with the paws should not reduce the amount of motor activity required to find the corre'ct 1. We have already described experi- response in a shuttle box. ments by Maier and Testa (1975) which 3. Seligman, Maier, and Geer (1968) and indicate that although inescapably shocked Seligman and Beagley (1975) found that rats are poor at learning FR-2 shuttling, escape deficits in dogs and rats already fail- they will learn as well as controls when there ing to escape could be eliminated by forcible is a brief break in shock after the first re- exposure to the escape contingency. It is sponse of the FR-2. In an analogous fash- hard to see how the motor deficit hypothesis ion, inescapably shocked rats readily learned can account for this therapy effect. Does FR-1 shuttling when shock terminated im- forcible exposure to the escape contingency mediately after the response, but failed to produce a rapid recovery of norepinephrine learn when shock termination was briefly levels? It is interesting to note the Abram- delayed. The motor activation deficit hy- son and Seligman (unpublished data) have pothesis cannot explain why the behavior of found that forcible exposure to the escape inescapably shocked subjects is so strongly contingency eliminates the behavioral deficit controlled by the nature of the contingency even in rats whose norepinephrine levels between responding and shock termination have been depleted by injection of a-Methyl- in the test task. The same amount of motor para-tyrosine. activity is required to perform an FR-2 re- sponse whether or not there is a brief break 4. We have several times referred to the in shock, but the outcomes are very differ- fact that the learned helplessness effect does ent. Similarly, the amount of motor activity not have a time course in dogs when the dogs are given a number of sessions of in- required to make a single crossing of a shut- tle box is the same whether or not shock escapable shock (Seligman & Groves, 1970) and may not have a time course in rats at termination is slightly delayed, but the out- all (Seligman & Beagley, 1975). This is come is dependent on this factor. Again, directly opposed to the expectations of the mention should be made that Weiss et al. restricted their explanation to learned help- motor deficit hypothesis. lessness in the dog, but this is ad hoc and it 5. Hannum et al. (in press) found that does not seem justifiable to restrict consider- rats given four sessions,of inescapable shock ation to only a small part of the data. shortly after weaning failed to learn to escape 2. Recall that Seligman and Maier (1967) as adults. Such a failure did not result if found that prior experience with controllable the shocks were escapable. Do four sessions 38 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN of inescapable shock shortly after weaning ferent situation. Further, the data pre- produce a permanent depletion of norepi- sented by Weiss et al. do convincingly point nephrine ? to the conclusion that severe trauma induces 6. Although Weiss et al. (1975) have a transient motor deficit perhaps mediated explicitly limited the score of their hy- by norepinephrine depletion. What we dis- pothesis to learned helplessness experiments pute is that these processes are sufficient to employing electric shocks to animals (even, account for all or even many of the learned on an ad hoc basis, to dogs), mention should helplessness effects. We also dispute that be made that there are a large number of these hypotheses handle the data better than experiments outside this domain which can the learned helplessness hypothesis, and we be integrated by the learned helplessness feel that a stronger case has been presented hypothesis but not by the motor activation for the learned helplessness hypothesis. deficit hypothesis. Examples are the effects It should be noted, however, that the of exposure to inescapable noise and un- learned helplessness hypothesis is not with- solvable problems on problem-solving be- out its problems. We will list those that havior in humans (see p. 13), and the pos- loom largest. sible analogues to shock-induced learned 1. The time course of learned helplessness helplessness produced by delivery of appeti- effects has yet to be satisfactorily explained. tive events independently of behavior (see p. Recall that one session of inescapable shock 12) in animals. produces only a transient effect in dogs, while four sessions of inescapable shock seem to CONCLUSIONS produce a more permanent effect. More- We feel that the burden of the evidence is over, there are data to indicate that the that the motor theories here discussed cannot learned helplessness effect may be perma- account for the learned helplessness phe- nent in rats even after one session of inescap- nomenon. It seems to us that the central able shock. shortcoming of these theories is that they This pattern of results is difficult to ex- are performance theories. That is, they plain. The learned helplessness theory claim that inescapably shocked subjects are could argue (see pp. 30-31) that previous later less likely to emit the test task response, experience controlling events proactively in- not that they are less likely to learn from an terferes with the memory that shock is ines- exposure to the contingency between re- capable. Proactive interference is known to sponding and relief if they do respond. The increase with time from learning and to learned helplessness hypothesis also argues decrease with additional training, and so the for a performance deficit (the reduced incen- dog effects can be explained. But, why do tive motivation proposition), but in addition rats show a permanent effect after one ses- argues that learning will be undermined (the sion? A possibility concerns the rearing associative interference proposition). We conditions of our subjects. Our dogs were feel that the Maier and Testa (1975) ex- not laboratory- or cage-reared, while the rats periments (see p. 24) offer conclusive evi- we used were raised in cages. Perhaps this dence that exposure to inescapable shock reduced the likelihood that the rats en- has an effect on associative processes as well countered many immunizing experiences as on motivational processes. with controllable events. Consistent with This should not be taken to mean that we the argument, Seligman and Groves (1970) feel that the processes discussed by Brace- found that only two sessions of inescapable well and Black (1974), Anisman and Wal- shock were required to produce a permanent ler (1973), and Weiss et al. (1975) do not learned helplessness effect in cage-reared exist and do not influence behavior. Organ- beagles while four sessions were required to isms can, of course, learn motor responses in produce a permanent effect in mongrels one situation that are able to interfere with raised outside the laboratory. However, the acquisition of motor responses in a dif- such an explanation of the time course effects LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 39

because a clear acquisition function for nose- poke responding was found. It might seem that the amount of physical 40' activity involved in these various tasks is a 35' determining factor. However, recall that u z 30-\ Maier and Testa (1975) found that inescap- LU ably shocked rats learn FR-2 shuttling if a I— 25 < brief break in shock occurs after the first • 1 20 response, and they fail to learn FR-1 shut- Z tling if shock termination is delayed. Here IS- physical activity cannot be a factor. The 10- Maier and Testa experiments suggest that 5 the amount of feedback produced by the response could be a crucial factor. A re- cent experiment by Jackson, Tomie, and FR-1 1 23456789 10 Maier (Note 7) supports this notion. Jack- son et al. began by replicating the Seligman BLOCKS OF 10 TRIALS FR-3 effect. As shown in Figure 12, rats given inescapable shock were very poor at FIG. 12. Mean FR-3 lever press latencies for FR-3 lever press escape responding. Another rats previously given either inescapable (I) or no (C) shocks and then given noise feedback (F) or group of inescapably shocked rats was also no feedback (NF) during lever press escape tested in the FR-3 lever pressing situation. training (Jackson, Tomie, & Maier, Note 7). However, a 100-msec burst of white noise (Tone feedback eliminated failure to escape.) was sounded following each lever press, thus providing feedback. As can be seen in is entirely post hoc and is not supported by Figure 12, these rats learned rapidly. It an independent body of evidence. Additional should be noted that a warning signal was experimental support will be required be- used in the Weiss et al. (1975) nose-poke fore any confidence can be placed in this experiment. This signal terminated with the interpretation. response, thus possibly providing feedback. 2. Exposure to inescapable shock pro- Most interesting is the fact that the only duces a deficit on some escape tasks but not study in the literature (Bracewell & Black, on others. Maier et al. (1973) found that 1974) to find an effect of inescapable shock exposure to inescapable shock interfered on FR-1 shuttling in the rat did not use a with FR-2 shuttling but not with FR-1 shut- warning signal. Again, much more work tling in rats. Maier et al. were able to ac- will be needed before any confidence can be count for this difference in terms of the placed in such an explanation. elicited nature of FR-1 shuttling in rats. In Mention should be made that the helpless- an analogous fashion, Seligman and Beagley ness effect has seemed more fragile in rats (1975) found that inescapable shock has no than in dogs or humans. Both of us have effect on FR-1 lever pressing in rats. How- encountered occasional difficulty in produc- ever, Seligman and Beagley did not find a ing a reliable phenomenon. The feedback differential effect of escapable and inescap- effect discussed previously may help to ex- able shock on FR-2 lever pressing. A clear plain this fragility—we may be using test learned helplessness effect only emerged with tasks with a relatively large amount of in- FR-3 lever pressing. It is not obvious why trinsic feedback. this should be so (Seligman & Beagley, 3. The learned helplessness hypothesis is 1975). Further, Weiss et al. (1975) found vague in its specification of boundary condi- no effect of inescapable shock on a nose-poke tions. Further, there is little empirical evi- escape response. The Maier et al. explana- dence regarding such boundary conditions. tion does not apply to the Weiss et al. data Should exposure to loud noise presented in- 40 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

dependently of behavior interfere with the ments for the learned helplessness hypothe- acquisition of responses that escape electric sis. Why all this confusion? We feel that shock? Should exposure to uncontrollable a large part of this confusion stems from an shock interfere with the acquisition of re- error of inference noted previously. The sponses that procure food ? Should exposure logical structure of many of the experiments to noncontingent food interfere with the ac- which have been presented as inconsistent quisition of responses that terminate shock? with the learned helplessness hypothesis have Should exposure to uncontrollable shock the following form: (a) A procedure is interfere with Pavlovian conditioning ? Many found which produces an escape deficit, (b) further questions of this sort could easily be this procedure is hard to interpret in terms posed. The learned helplessness hypothesis of the learning of independence between re- does not make any predictions regarding the sponding and shock termination, and (c) results of such experiments. Further, there therefore the learned helplessness hypothesis are very few experiments directed at answer- is contradicted. This seems to us to be poor ing these questions. The learned helpless- logic. The learned helplessness hypothesis ness hypothesis will have to become more does not argue that all escape deficits are specific, and experiments designed to de- produced by learning that responding and lineate these boundary conditions will have shock termination are independent. There to be conducted. are undoubtedly many ways to produce poor 4. A final problem concerns the relation- escape performance and there is no reason to ship between the subject's perception of in- expect that all of these operate through a dependence between responding and out- single mechanism. come and the conditions which lead to such Just as there may be many different ways a perception (see p. 17). In our experi- to produce escape-learning deficits, exposure ments we have arranged conditions in which to inescapable shock may itself produce a the relationship between responding and out- variety of changes in the organism, not just come is objectively independent. However, one. Under some conditions, organisms subjects do not always respond to random might learn a motor response, stress might relationships as if they were random • (e.g., be induced, and the organism might learn Bruner & Revusky, 1961; Hake & Hyman, that its behavior is independent of shock 1953; Naylor & Clark, 1968), and it is well termination. A complete explanation of the known that subjective probabilities do not influence of exposure to inescapable aversive reflect objective probabilities with accuracy events on later behavior might involve a (cf. Kahneman & Tversky, 1972). Thus we consideration of each of these factors. Thus will have to specify the conditions under we do not view the various theoretical posi- which the perception of independence devel- tions that have been here discussed as incom- ops—only these conditions should lead to patible: They might be complementary. learned helplessness. The existence of one of these processes does not imply the nonexistence of the others. Before closing a few comments are in We feel that we have presented convincing order. evidence for the reality of the processes Tt must seem to the reader that a great specified by the learned helplessness hypothe- deal of theoretical confusion surrounds the sis; however, this evidence does not imply learned helplessness phenomenon. A num- that other processes do not also occur. ber of investigators have proposed alterna- It might seem that this makes the learned tive accounts of the same behavioral phe- helplessness hypothesis difficult to test, but nomenon and have presented experimental evidence which they feel supports their posi- this is not so. A variety of predictions made tion and refutes the learned helplessness hy- pothesis. We, on the other hand, have 8 Thanks go to Christopher Peterson for these questioned the implications of these experi- references. LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 41 by the learned helplessness hypothesis have 4. Carder, B., & Leaf, R. Personal communication, been tested and confirmed (e.g., Hiroto & 1973. 5. Weisman, R. Personal communication, 1974. Seligman, 1975; Maier, 1970; Maier et al., 6. Weiss, J. M., Pohorecky, L. A., Dorros, K., 1973; Maier & Testa, 1975; Overmier & Williams, S., Emmel, D., Whittlesey, M., & Seligman, 1967; Seligman & Beagley, 1975; Case, E. Coping behavior and brain norepi- Seligman & Groves, 1970; Seligman & nephrine turnover. Paper presented at the meet- Maier, 1967; Seligman, Maier, & Geer, ing of the Eastern Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., May 1973. 1968; Testa, Juraska, & Maier, 1974) Fail- 7. Jackson, R. L., Tomie, A., & Maier, S. F. ures of these predictions would have been Learned helplessness in rats: Effects of re- evidence against the learned helplessness sponse produced feedback. Paper presented at position. In addition there are many new Rocky Mountain Psychological Association, ways in which the hypothesis could be tested. Salt Lake City, May 1975. For example the logic, if not the execution, underlying Experiments 8 and 9 of Weiss et REFERENCES al. (1975) seems to us to be adequate to test Amsel, A., Rashotte, N. E., & MacKinnon, J. R. the learned helplessness position. The com- Partial reinforcement effects within subject and ing years should see a number of such tests between subjects. Psychological Monographs, 1966, 80 (20, Whole No. 628). and a more detailed elaboration of the learned Anderson, D. C., Cole, J., & McVaugh, W. Varia- helplessness hypothesis. tions in unsignaled inescapable preshock as de- Finally, we would like to say a word about terminants of responses to punishment. Journal the cognitive nature of our position. The of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 65 (3, Pt. 2). learned helplessness hypothesis has been Anderson, D. C., & Paden, P. Passive avoidance stated in cognitive languages, whereas most response learning as a function of prior tumbling of the alternative views have been stated in trauma. Psychonomic Science, 1966, 4, 129-130. S-R language. We have found it difficult Anisman, H. Effects of pretraining compatible and to even approach the sort of phenomena that incompatible responses on subsequent one-way and shuttle-avoidance performance in rats. Jour- we have tried to explain within an S-R nal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, framework, and have found the cognitive 1973, 82, 95-104. theorizing to be more fruitful and to reflect Anisman, H., & Waller, T. G. Facilitative and more accurately those processes that we feel disruptive effect of prior exposure to shock on subsequent avoidance performance. Journal of to be reflected in behavior. Many other in- Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1972, vestigators of learning and motivation in 78, 113-122. lower organisms seem to have reached a Anisman, H., & Waller, T. G. Effects of inescap- similar conclusion (e.g., Bolles, 1972; Irwin, able shock on subsequent avoidance performance: 1971; Kamin, 1969; Seligman & Johnston, Role of response repertoire changes. Behavioral Biology, 1973, 9, 331-355. 1973; Terry & Wagner, 1975; Wagner, Averill, J. R. Personal control over aversive Rudy, & Whitlow, 1973). The next few stimuli and its relationship to stress. Psycho- years should determine the value of such an logical Bulletin, 1973, SO, 286-303. approach. Averill, J. R., & Rosenn, M. Vigilant and non- vigilant coping strategies and psychophysiological stress reactions during the anticipation of electric REFERENCES NOTES shock. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- 1. Miller, W., & Seligman, M. E. P. Depression, chology, 1972, 23, 128-141. _ learned helplessness, and the perception of re- Bainbridge, P. L. Learning in the rat: Effect of inforcement. Manuscript submitted for publica- early experience with an unsolvable problem. tion, 1975. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- 2. James, W. H. DeKalb survey tests: Student chology, 1973, 82, 301-307. opinion survey form I-E, I. Author's copyright, Bandler, R. J., Jr., Madaras, G. R., & Bern, D. J. 1963. Self-observation as a source of pain perception. 3. Anderson, D. C, Schwendiman, G., & Payne, R. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Prior unsignalled inescapable shock trauma in 1968, 9, 205-209. the rat. Paper presented at the meeting of the Behrend, E. R., & Bitterman, M. E. Sidman Western Psychological Association, San Diego, avoidance in the fish. Journal of the Experi- March 1968. mental Analysis of Behavior, 1963, 13, 229-242. 42 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

Bintz, J. Between- and within-subject effect of Corah, N. L., & Boffa, J. Perceived control, self- shock intensity on avoidance in goldfish (Caras- observation, and response to aversive stimula- sius auratus). Journal of Comparative and tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Physiological Psychology, 1971, 75, 92-97. chology, 1970, 16, 1-4. Black, A. H., Young, G. A., & Batenchuk, C. D'Amato, M. Delayed matching and short-term Avoidance training of hippocampal theta waves memory in monkeys. In G. H. Bower, (Ed.), in flaxedilized dogs and its relation to move- The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. ment. Journal of Comparative and Physiological 7). New York: Academic Press, 1973. Psychology, 1970, 70, 15-24. Desiderate, O., & Newman, A. Conditioned sup- Bolles, R. C. Species-specific defense reactions in pression produced in rats by tones paired with avoidance learning. Psychological Review, 1970, escapable or inescapable shock. Journal of Com- 77, 32-48. parative and Physiological Psychology, 1971, 77, Bolles, R. C. Reinforcement, expectancy, and 427-443. learning, Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 394- De Toledo, L., & Black, A. H. Effects of pre- 409. shock on subsequent avoidance conditioning. Bracewell, R. J., & Black, A. H. The effects of Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- restraint and noncontingent pre-shock on sub- chology, 1967, 63, 493-499. sequent escape learning in the rat. Learning and Dinsmoor, J. A., & Campbell, S. L. Escape-from- Motivation, 1974, 5, 53-69. shock training following exposure to inescap- able shock. Psychological Reports, 1956, 2, 43- Brady, J. V., Porter, R. W., Conrad, D. G., & 49. (a) Mason, J. W. Avoidance behavior and the Dinsmoor, J. A., & Campbell, S. L. Level of cur- development of gastroduodenal ulcers. Journal rent and time between sessions as factors in of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1958, "adaptation" to shock. Psychological Reports 1, 69-72. 1956,2,441-444. (b) Braud, W., Wepmann, B., & Russo, D. Task and Dweck, C. S. The role of expectations and attribu- species generality of the "helplessness" phe- tions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. nomenon. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 16, 154- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 155. 1975, 31, 674-685. Brimer, C. J., & Kamin, L. J. Disinhibition, habitu- Dweck, C. S., & Repucci, N. D. Learned helpless- ation, sensitization and the conditioned emotional ness and reinforcement responsibility in children. response. Journal of Comparative and Physio- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, logical Psychology, 1963, 56, 508-516. 1973, 25,109-116. Brookshire, K. H., Littman, R. A., & Stewart, C. Elliot, R. Tonic heart rate: Experiments on the N. Residue of shock trauma in the white rat: effects of collative variables lead to an< hy- A three factor theory. Psychological Mono- pothesis about its motivational significance. Jour- graphs, 1961, 75, (10, Whole No. 514). nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1969, Brown, P., & Jenkins, H. M. Autoshaping of the #211-228. pigeons key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Engberg, L. A., Hansen, G., Welker, R. L., & Analysis of Behavior, 1968, 11, 1-8. Thomas, D. R. Acquisition of key-pecking via Bruner, A., & Revusky, S. H. Collateral be- autoshaping as a function of prior experience: havior in humans. Journal of the Experimental "Learned laziness"? Science, 1973, 178, 1002- Analysis of Behavior, 1961, 4, 349-350. 1004. Catania, A. C. Elicitation, reinforcement, and Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of Re- stimulus control. In R. Glaser (Ed.), The inforcement. New York: Appleton-Century- nature of reinforcement. New York: Academic Crofts, 1957. Press, 1971. Fosco, F., & Geer, J. H. Effects of gaining con- Church, R. M. Response suppression. In B. A. trol over aversive stimuli after differing amounts Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment of no control. Psychological Reports, 1971, 29, and aversive behavior. New York: Appleton- 1153-1154. Century-Crofts, 1969. Frumkin, K., & Brookshire, K. H. Conditioned Church, R. M., LoLordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., fear training and later avoidance learning in Solomon, R. L., & Turner, L. H. Cardiac re- goldfish. Psychonomic Science, 1969, 16, 159- sponses to shock in curarized dogs: Effects of 160. shock intensity and duration, warning signal, Gamzu, E. R., Williams, D. A., & Schwartz, B and prior experience with shock. Journal of Pitfalls of organism and concept: "Learned Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1966, laziness"? Science, 1973, 101, 367-368. 62, 1-7. Geer, J. H., Davison, G. C., & Gatchel, R. I. Cohen, P. S. Interference effects of escapable Reduction of stress in humans through non- shock upon subsequent acquisition of escape- veridical perceived control of aversive stimula- avoidance responding. Journal of Comparative tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- and Physiological Psychology, 1970, 71, 484-486. chology, 1970, 16, 731-738. LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 43

Geer, J. H., & Maisel, E. Evaluating the effects Kelley, H. H. Causal schemata and the attribu- of the prediction-control confound. Journal of tion process. Morristown, N. J.: General Learn- Personality and Social Psychology, 1972, 23, 314- ing Press, 1972. 319. Kemler, D., & Shepp, B. The learning and trans- Gibbon, J., Berryman, R., & Thompson, R. L. fer of dimensional relevance and irrelevance in Contingency spaces and measures in classical and children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, instrumental conditioning. Journal of the Ex- 1971, 90, 120-127. perimental Analysis of Behavior, 1974, 21, 585- Klein, D. C., Fencil-Morse, E., & Seligman, M. E. 605. P. Depression, learned helplessness, and the at- Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. Urban stress: Ex- tribution of failure. Journal of Personality and periments on noise and social stressors. New Social Psychology, in press. York: Academic Press, 1972. Krantz, D. S., Glass, D. C., & Snyder, M. L. Goeckner, D. J,, Greenough, W. T., & Maier, S. Helplessness, stress level, and the coronary prone F. Escape learning deficit after overcrowded behavior pattern. Journal of Experimental Social rearing in rats: Test of a helplessness hypothe- Psychology, 1974, 10, 284-300. sis. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1974, Langer, E. J. The illusion of control. Unpub- 3, 54-56. lished doctoral dissertation, Yale University, Hake, H. W., & Hyman, R. Perception of the 1974. statistical structure of a random series of binary Lazarus, R. S. Psychological stress and the coping symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology, process. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966. 1953, 45, 64-74. Lefcourt, H. N. Internal versus external control Hannum, R. D., Rosellini, R. A., & Seligman, M. of reinforcement: A review. Psychological E. P. Retention and immunization of learned Bulletin, 1966, 65, 206-220. helplessness from weaning to adulthood. Devel- Liddell, H. S., James, W. T., & Anderson, O. D. opmental Psychology, in press. The comparative physiology of the conditioned Hearst, E. Stress induced breakdown of an ap- motor reflex: Based on experiments with the petitive discrimination. Journal of the Experi- pig, dog, sheep, goat and rabbit. Comparative mental Analysis of Behavior, 1965, 8, 135-146. Physiology Monograph, 1934, 11(1}, Serial No. Herman, Z. The effects of noradrenaline on rat's 89. behaviour. Psychopharmacologia, 1970, 16, 369- Lindner, M. Hereditary and environmental in- 374. fluences upon resistance to stress. Unpublished Hiroto, D. S. Locus of control and learned help- doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, lessness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1968. 1974, 102, 187-193. Looney, T. A., & Cohen, P. S. Retardation of Hiroto, D. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. Generality jump-up escape responding in rats pretreated of learned helplessness in man. Journal of with different frequencies of noncontingent Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 311- electric shock. Journal of Comparative and 327. Physiological Psychology, 1972, 78, 317-322. Hokanson, J. E., DeGood, D. E., Forrest, M. S., Loucks, R. B. The experimental delimitation of & Brittain, T. M. Availability of avoidance be- neural structures essential for learning: The haviors in modulating vascular-stress responses. attempt to condition striped muscle responses Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, with faradization of the sigmoid gyri. Journal 1971,19, 60-68. of Psychology, 1935, 1, 5-44. Honig, W. K. (Ed.). Operant conditioning: Re- MacKintosh, N. J. Stimulus selection: Learning search and application. New York: Appleton- to ignore stimuli that predict no change in rein- Century-Crofts, 1966. forcement. In R. A. Hindle & J. Stevenson- Irwin, F. W. Intentional behavior and motiva- Hinde (Eds.), Constraints on learning. New tion: A cognitive theory, Philadelphia: Lip- York: Academic Press, 1973. pincott, 1971. Maier, S. F. Failure to escape traumatic shock: Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. Subjective prob- Incompatible skeletal motor response or learned ability: A judgment of representativeness. helplessness? Learning and Motivation, 1970, , 1972, 3, 430-454. 1, 157-170. Kamin, L. J. Predictability, surprise, attention, Maier, S. F., Albin, R. W., & Testa, T. J. Fail- and conditioning. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. ure to learn to escape in rats previously exposed Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive be- to inescapable shock depends on nature of escape havior, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, response. Journal of Comparative and Physio- 1969. logical Psychology, 1973, 85, 581-592. Kelley, H. H. Attribution theory in social psy- Maier, S. F., Allaway, T. A., & Gleitman, H. chology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Sym- Proactive inhibition in rats after prior reversal: posium on Motivation. Lincoln: University of A critique of the spontaneous recovery hypotlie- Nebraska Press, 1967. sis. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9, 63-65. 44 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

Maier, S. F., Anderson, C., & Lieberman, D. A. ence strategies in interval learning tasks as a Influence of control of shock on subsequent function of validity magnitude and sign. Organ- shock-elicited aggression. Journal of Compara- isational Behavior and Human Performance. tive and Physiological Psychology, 1972, 81, 94- 1968, 3, 378-399. 100. Overmier, J. B. Interference with avoidance be- Maier, S. F., & Gleitman, H. Proactive interfer- havior: Failure to avoid traumatic shock. Jour- ence in rats. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 7, 25- nal of Experimental Psychology, 1968, 78, 340- 26. 343. Maier, S. F., Seligman, M. E. P., & Solomon, R. Overmier, J. B., & Seligman, M. E. P. Effects of L. Pavlovian fear conditioning and learned inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and helplessness. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and (Eds.), Punishment. New York: Appleton- Physiological Psychology, 1967, 63, 28-33. Century-Crofts, 1969. Padilla, A. M. Effects of prior and interpolated Maier, S. F., & Testa, T. J. Failure to learn to shock exposures on subsequent avoidance learn- escape by rats previously exposed to inescapable ing by goldfish. Psychological Reports, 1973, shock is partly produced by associative inter- 32, 451-456. ference. Journal of Comparative and Physio- Padilla, A. M., Padilla, C., Ketterer, T., & Gia- logical Psychology, 1975, 88, 554-564. colone, D. Inescapable shocks and subsequent Masserman, J. H. Behavior and neurosis. Chicago: avoidance conditioning in goldfish (Carrasius University of Chicago Press, 1943. auratus). Psychonomic Science, 1970, 20, 295- Masserman, J. H. The principle of uncertainty in 296. Neurotegenesis. In H. D. Kimmel (Ed.), Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned reflexes. New York: Experimental psychopathology. New York: Dover, 1927. Academic Press, 1971. Payne, R. J. Alterations of Sidman avoidance McCulloch, T. L., & Bruner, J. S. The effects of baselines by CS paired with avoidable or un- electric shock upon subsequent learning in the avoidable shock. Psychological Reports, 1972, rat. Journal of Psychology, 1939, 7, 333-336. 31, 291-294. Mellgren, R. L., & Ost, J. W. P. Discriminative Payne, R., Anderson, D. C., & Murcurio, J. Pre- stimulus preexposure and learning of an operant shock-produced alterations in pain-elicited fight- discrimination in the rat. Journal of Compara- ing. Journal of Comparative and Physiological tive and Physiological Psychology, 1971, 77, 179- Psychology, 1970, 71, 258-266. 187. Pervin, L. A. The need to predict and control Miller, S., & Konorski, J. Sur une forme particu- under conditions of threat. Journal of Person- liere des reflexes conditionals. Comptes rendus ality, 1963, 31, 570-585. hebdomadaires des stances et memoires de la Powell, D. A., & Creer, T. L. Interaction of devel- Societe de Biologie et filiales, 1928, 99, 1155- opmental and environmental variables in shock- 1157. elicited aggression. Journal of Comparative and Miller, W., & Seligman, M. E. P. Depression in Physiological Psychology, 1969, 69, 219-225. humans. Journal of , 1975, Racinskas, J. R. Maladaptive consequences of loss 84,228-238. or lack of control over aversive events. Un- Miller, W., & Weiss, J. M. Effects of somatic or published doctoral dissertation, Waterloo Uni- visceral responses to punishment. In B. A. versity, Ontario, Canada, 1971. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment Rech, R. H., Bovys, H. K., & Moore, K. Altera- and aversive behavior. New York: Appleton- tions in behavior and brain catecholamine levels Century-Crofts, 1969. in rats treated with a-methyltyrosine. Journal Moore, B. R. The role of directed Pavlovian re- of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, actions in simple instrumental learning in the 1966, 153, 412-419. pigeon. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian conditioning and its (Eds.), Constraints on learning. New York: proper control procedures. Psychological Re- Academic Press, 1973. view, 1967, 74, 71-79. Moot, S. A., Ceballa, R. P., & Crabtree, J. M. Rescorla, R. A. Probability of shock in the pres- Instrumental control and ulcertation in rats. ence and absence of the CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- chology, 1970, 71, 405-410. chology, 1968, 66, 1-5. Mowrer, O. H., & Viek, P. An experimental Rodin, J. Overcrowding, perceived choice and analogue of fear from a sense of helplessness. response to controllable and uncontrollable out- Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, comes. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- 1948, 43, 193-200. chology, in press. Mullin, A. D., & Mogenson, G. J. Effects of fear Rosellini, R., & Seligman, M. E. P. Learned help- conditioning on avoidance learning. Psychologi- lessness and escape from frustration. Journal cal Reports, 1963, 13, 707-710. of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Naylor, J. C., & Clark, R. D. Intuitive interfer- Processes, 1975, 1, 149-158. LEARNED HELPLESSNESS: THEORY AND EVIDENCE 45

Roth, S. The effects of experimentally induced Seward, J. P., & Humphrey, G. L. Avoidance learn- expectancies of control: Facilitation of control- ing as a function of pretraining in the cat. ling behavior or learned helplessness? Unpub- Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- lished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern Uni- chology, 1967, 63, 338-341. versity, 1973. Sines, J. O., Cleeland, C., & Adkins, J. The be- Roth, S., & Bootzin, R. R. Effects of experi- havior of normal and stomach lesion susceptible mentally induced expectancies of external con- rats in several learning situations. Journal of trol: An investigation of learned helplessness. Genetic Psychology, 1963, 102, 91-94. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Spear, N. E. Retrieval of memory in animals. 1974, 29, 253-264. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 163-194. Roth, S., & Kubal, L. The effects of noncontingent Stone, E. A. Behavioral and neurochemical ef- reinforcement on tasks of differing importance: fects of acute swim stress are due to hypothermia. Facilitation and learned helplessness effects. Life Sciences, 1970, 9, 877-888. (a) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Stone, E. A. Swim-stress-induced inactivity: Re- 1975, 32, 680-691. lation to body temperature and brain norepi- Rotter, J. B. Generalized expectancies for internal nephrine, and effects of d-amphetamine. Psy- versus external control of reinforcement. Psy- chosomatic Medicine, 1970, 32, 51-59. (b) chological Monographs, 1966, 80, (1, Whole No. Stotland, E. The psychology of hope. San Fran- 609). cisco: Jossey-Bass, 1969. Schwartz, B., & Williams, D. R. Two different Stotland, E., & Blumenthal, A. The reduction of kinds of key peck in the pigeon: Some prop- anxiety as a result of the expectation of making erties of responses maintained by negative and a choice. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1964, positive response-reinforcer contingencies. Jour- 18, 139-145. nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Stroebel, C. F. Biologic rhythm correlates of 1972, 18, 201-216. disturbed behavior in the rhesus monkey. Biblio- Segal, D. S., Knapp, S., Kuczenski, R. T., & theca Primatologica, 1969, 9, 91-105. Mandell, A. J., The effects of environmental Terry, W. S., & Wagner, A. R, Short-term isolation on behavior and regional rat brain memory for "surprising" versus "expected" un- tyrosine hydroxylase and tryptophan hydroxylase conditioned stimuli in Pavlovian conditioning. activities. Behavioral Biology, 1973, 8, 47-53. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Seligman, M. E. P. Helplessness. On depression, Behavior Process, 1975, 1, 122-134. development, and death. San Francisco: W. H. Testa, T. J., Juraska, J. M., & Maier, S. F. Prior exposure to inescapable electric shock in rats Freeman, 1975. effects extinction behavior after the successful Seligman, M. E. P., & Beagley, G. Learned help- acquisition of an escape response. Learning and lessness in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Motivation, 1974, 5, 380-392. Physiological Psychology, 1975, 88, 534-541. Thomas, D. R., Freeman, F., Svinicki, J. G., Seligman, M. E. P., & Groves, D. Non-transient Burr, D. E. S., & Lyons, J. Effects of extra- learned helplessness. Psychonomic Science, 1970, dimensional training on stimulus generalization. 19,191-192. Journal of Experimental Psychology Mono- Seligman, M. E. P., & Johnston, J. C. A cognitive graph, 1970, 83, (1, Pt. 2). theory of avoidance learning. In F. J. Mc- Thomas, E., & Baiter, A. Learned helplessness: Guigan & D. B. Lumsden (Eds.), Contemporary Amelioration of symptoms by cholinergic blockade approaches to conditioning and learning. Wash- of the septum. Science, in press. ington, D. C.: V. H. Winston, 1973. Thornton, J. W., & Jacobs, P. D. Learned help-, Seligman, M. E. P., & Maier, S. F. Failure to lessness in human subjects. Journal of Experi- escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental mental Psychology, 1971, 87, 369-372. Psychology, 1967, 74, 1-9. Wagner, A. R. Stimulus selection and a "modi- Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F., & Geer, J. The fied continuity theory". In G. H. Bower & J. T. alleviation of learned helplessness in the dog. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, motivation (Vol. 3). New York: Academic 1968, 73, 256-262. Press, 1969. Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F., & Solomon, R. Wagner, A. R., Rudy, J. W., & Whitlow, J. W. L. Unpredictable and uncontrollable aversive Rehearsal in animal conditioning. Journal of events. In F. R. Brush (Ed.), Aversive condi- Experimental Psychology, 1973, 97, 407-426. tioning and learning. New York: Academic Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, Press, 1971. S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. Perceiving the causes Seligman, M. E. P., Rosellini, R. A., & Kozak, _M. of success and failure. Morristown, N. J.: Gen- Learned helplessness in the rat: Reversibility, eral Learning Press, 1971. time course, and immunization. Journal of Com- Weiss, J. M. Effects of coping responses on stress. parative and Physiological Psychology, 1975, 88, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- 542-547. chology, 1968, 65, 251-260. 46 STEVEN F. MAIER AND MARTIN E. P. SELIGMAN

Weiss, J. M. Effects of coping behavior in dif- Weiss, J. M., Stone, E. A., & Harrell, N. Coping ferent warning signal conditions on stress behavior and brain norepinephrine in rats. Jour- pathology in rats. Journal of Comparative and nal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- Physiological Psychology, 1971, 77, 1-13. (a) chology, 1970, 72, 153-160. Weiss, J. M. Effects of coping behavior with and Welker, R. E. "Learned lasiness": A replication without a feedback signal on stress pathology in and extension. Unpublished doctoral disserta- rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological tion, University of Colorado, 1974. Psychology, 1971, 77, 22-30. (b) Woodbury, C. B. A note on "passive" condition- Weiss, J. M. Effects of punishing the coping ing. Journal of General Psychology, 1942, 27, response (conflict) on stress pathology in rats. 359-361. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- Zielinski, K., & Soltysik, S. The effect of pretrain- chology, 1971, 77, 14-21. (c) ing on the acquisition and extinction of the avoid- Weiss, J. M., Glazer, H. I., & Pohorecky, L. A. ance reflex. Acta Blologiae Experlmentalls, Coping behavior and neurochemical changes: An 1964, 24, 73-87. alternative explanation for the original "learned Zigmond, M., & Harvey, J. Resistance to central helplessness" experiments. In Relevance of the norepinephrine depletion and decreased mortality psychopathological animal model to the human. in rats chronically exposed to electric foot shock. New York: Plenum Press, 197S. Journal of Neuro-Vlsceral Relations, 1970, 31, Weiss, J. M., Krieckhaus, E. E., & Conte, R. 373-381. Effects of fear conditioning on subsequent avoid- ance behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 65, 413-421. (Received June 30, 1975)