An Exploratory Investigation of Learned Optimism
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Learned Helplessness: Theory and Evidence
\, Journal ol Experimental Psychology: General 1976, Vol. 105, No. 1, 3-46 Learned Helplessness: Theory and Evidence Steven F. Maier Martin E. P. Seligman University of Colorado University of Pennsylvania SUMMARY In 1967, Overmier and Seligman found that dogs exposed to inescapable and unavoidable electric shocks in one situation later failed to learn to escape shock in a different situation where escape was possible. Shortly thereafter Seligman and Maier (1967) demonstrated that this effect was caused by the uncontrollability of the original shocks. In this article we review the effects of exposing organisms to aversive events which they cannot control, and we review the explanations which have been offered. There seem to be motivational, cognitive, and emotional effects of uncontrol- lability. (a) Motivation. Dogs that have been exposed to inescapable shocks do not subsequently initiate escape response in the presence of shock. We review parallel phenomena in cats, fish, rats, and man. Of particular interest is the discussion of learned helplessness in rats and man. Rats are of interest because learned helplessness has been difficult to demonstrate in rats. However, we show that inescapably shocked rats do fail to learn to escape if the escape task is rea- sonably difficult. With regard to man, we review a variety of studies using in- escapable noise and unsolvable problems as agents which produce learned helpless- ness effects on both instrumental and cognitive tasks, (b) Cognition. We argue that exposure to uncontrollable events interferes with the organism's tendency to perceive contingent relationships between its behavior and outcomes. Here we review a variety of studies showing such a cognitive set. -
Psychology of Strengths and Virtues Syllabus
Course Syllabus PY 420/791-2A, Psychology of Strengths and Virtues Spring 2003 Instructor: Gitendra Uswatte, Ph.D. Class time: T, Th 8-9:15 am Class location: CH157 Office: Center for Psychiatric Medicine (CPM), Rm. 722, 1713 6th Ave. S. (My office is located in the Taub Training Clinic on the 7th fl. of CPM. CPM is at the corner of 6th Ave S. and 17th St.) Office hours: W, F 1-3:30 pm Email: [email protected] Telephone: (205) 975-5089 Books: Required- Seligman, M. E. P. (1998). Learned Optimism. New York: Pocket Books. Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (2002). Handbook of Positive Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Other assigned readings will be on reserve at Sterne Library. Course Overview: What makes people happy? How do you foster hope? Is being curious adaptive or maladaptive? Is studying “positive” psychology scientific? Psychology, in the last half-century, has developed a rich body of knowledge about human frailties such as depression, anxiety, and other maladaptive mental states. Although humanistic psychologists and others have addressed human strengths, such as spirituality and a drive for self-actualization, “positive” aspects of human psychology have not, until relatively recently, been studied using empirical methods. This course will provide an introduction to “Positive Psychology,” the empirical study of what permits humans to flourish or, as described by Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000), “[the] science of positive subjective experience, positive traits, and positive institutions.” We will concentrate on studying positive traits or virtues but will also touch on research on positive subjective experience and positive institutions. -
Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects Of
Buffalo Law Review Volume 64 Number 2 Article 5 4-1-2016 Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Major Depressive Disorder on Refugees and Explain Why These Disorders Should Qualify as Extraordinary Circumstances Excusing Untimely Asylum Applications Brandon R. White University at Buffalo School of Law (Student) Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.law.buffalo.edu/buffalolawreview Part of the Immigration Law Commons Recommended Citation Brandon R. White, Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Major Depressive Disorder on Refugees and Explain Why These Disorders Should Qualify as Extraordinary Circumstances Excusing Untimely Asylum Applications, 64 Buff. L. Rev. 413 (2016). Available at: https://digitalcommons.law.buffalo.edu/buffalolawreview/vol64/iss2/5 This Comment is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at Digital Commons @ University at Buffalo School of Law. It has been accepted for inclusion in Buffalo Law Review by an authorized editor of Digital Commons @ University at Buffalo School of Law. For more information, please contact [email protected]. COMMENT Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Major Depressive Disorder on Refugees and Explain Why These Disorders Should Qualify as Extraordinary Circumstances Excusing Untimely Asylum Applications BRANDON R. WHITE† INTRODUCTION Imagine you are a refugee. For some personal reason outside of your control, say your race or religion, you are targeted by others and suffer some harm. This harm almost certainly involves some level of violence. Perhaps you are beaten, raped, or tortured. Maybe you watched your family and friends suffer the same, maybe some of these family and friends were murdered in front of your eyes. -
Mood and Self-Regulation Changes in Underrecovery: an Intervention Model
Davis, H., Botterill, C., & MacNeill, K. (2002). Mood and self-regulation changes in underrecovery: An intervention model. In M. Kellmann (Ed.), Enhancing recovery: Preventing underperformance in athletes (pp. 161-179). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. 9 Mood and Self-Regulation Changes in Underrecovery: An Intervention Model Henry Davis IV, Cal Botterill, and Karen MacNeill Vince Lombardi (as cited in Maraniss, 1999, p. havioral components. This chapter identifies em- 217) is quoted as having said in 1959, “Fatigue pirical research that serves as the scientific base makes cowards of us all.” Did Lombardi know for the responsible intervention in UR; case ex- about underrecovery back then? The athlete in amples follow. We begin by elaborating on the high-intensity training provides a wonderful ex- concept of empirically supported treatment as it ample for the elucidation of psychological factors applies to intervention in UR. that can adversely affect performance when the Our position on cause and effect is that poor self- athlete is underrecovered. Lombardi knew well regulation and low mood may sometimes result in that fatigue can trigger many emotions. During poor athletic performance and prolong inadequate training camps high-performance athletes incur recovery; at other times, these factors may directly training loads that surpass general training lev- result from inadequate recovery. Recovery occurs els; this high-intensity training compromises physi- at physical, psychological, and social levels. We ological recovery and can bring a deterioration of therefore propose continuous two-way interactions mood (Kellmann & Kallus, 1999). Such loads de- between psychological and physiological states. mand physical and psychological adaptive re- sponses to restore homeostasis as described in chapter 1 of this volume. -
Bullying and Peer Victimization
JIVXXX10.1177/0886260515572476Journal of Interpersonal ViolenceRadliff et al. 572476research-article2015 Article Journal of Interpersonal Violence 2016, Vol. 31(11) 1983 –2005 Bullying and Peer © The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: Victimization: An sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0886260515572476 Examination of Cognitive jiv.sagepub.com and Psychosocial Constructs Kisha M. Radliff, PhD,1 Cixin Wang, PhD,2 and Susan M. Swearer, PhD3 Abstract Research has demonstrated a link between internalizing factors and bullying perpetration and peer victimization; however, few studies have examined predictors of cognitive and psychosocial factors, such as locus of control and hopelessness. The current study examined cognitive and psychosocial factors in bullying perpetration and peer victimization in a sample of 469 middle school students. A mediator model of hopelessness was also investigated. Students involved in bullying reported a greater external locus of control compared with peers who were not involved in bullying. Bully- victims endorsed the highest externality. Results showed that hopelessness fully mediated the relationship between verbal/relational victimization and external locus of control for the victim group, but not the bully-victim group. Implications for bullying prevention and intervention efforts are discussed. Keywords bullying, hopelessness, locus of control, children, adolescents 1The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA 2University of California, Riverside, CA, USA 3University of Nebraska–Lincoln, NE, USA Corresponding Author: Kisha M. Radliff, Assistant Professor of School Psychology, The Ohio State University, PAES Bldg. Rm. 436, 305 W. 17th Ave., Columbus, OH, 43210, USA. Email: [email protected] 1984 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 31(11) Within the last few decades, bullying among school-aged youth has increas- ingly been recognized as an important problem in schools and presents in many forms (i.e., physical, verbal, relational, and cyber). -
Behavioral Models of Depression: a Critique of the Emphasis on Positive Reinforcement
International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy Volume 4, No. 2, 2008 Behavioral Models of Depression: A Critique of the Emphasis on Positive Reinforcement Paulo Roberto Abreu & Carlos E. Santos Abstract This paper provides a review of behavioral models of depression highlighting the problems associated with its historical emphasis on lowered frequencies of positive reinforcement. We analyzed the models of Ferster and Lewinsohn in their theoretical approach, methodology and application. We conducted a review of the suppressive characteristics exercised by punishment, by the presentation of non-contingent aversive stimulation and by operant extinction. A number of recommendations are made concerning how these processes can inform interventions. We conclude that interventions geared towards identifying aversive control play a promising role in clinical change in the treatment of depression. Keywords: Aversive Control, Response-Contingent Positive Reinforcement, Depression, Clinical Behavior Analysis, Functional Assessment, Behavioral Activation. The present article aims to revisit the principles of behavioral models of depression by emphasizing advancements in the field, as well as providing a critique of the dominant models. With this goal in mind, we aim to re-direct the conceptual analysis towards an understanding of the environmental variables frequently observed in the behaviors of depressed individuals. Drawing from the existing literature, we propose possible variables associated with behavioral suppression and its mechanisms during the development and course of depressive behavior repertories. Three particular behavioral processes are considered: (1) suppressive characteristics exercised by punishment, (2) the presentation of non-contingent aversive stimulation, and (3) operant extinction. We begin with a discussion of the dominant models of depression, followed by a critique, and we conclude with a discussion of how each of these processes can inform clinical interventions. -
Learned Optimism
LEARNED OPTIMISM Did you know: • The way we think, especially about health, changes our health? • Optimists have better health habits than pessimists do? • Optimists tend to do better at school, at work, and on the playing field? • Optimism is a learned skill, one that can be permanently acquired? Learned helplessness is the giving-up reaction, the quitting response that follows from the belief that whatever you do doesn’t matter. Explanatory style is the manner in which you habitually explain to yourself why events happen. Learned optimism is the power of “non-negative thinking,” using the skills of changing the destructive things you say to yourself when you experience the losses and setbacks of life. Using learned optimism you can change your explanatory style to resist helplessness by: • understanding that the causes of bad events may be temporary; • realizing that your problems in one area aren’t universal; • affirming that bad events aren’t totally your fault. Practicing the thinking skills of learned optimism can give you the energy to keep on succeeding at life goals. You can strengthen your self-esteem because you know you can solve temporary problems in creative ways. You can learn to be more productive and less depressed, more hopeful and less discouraged, to achieve more successes than failures. Learned optimism allows you to change your self-talk and beliefs so you can face adversity in responsible and realistic ways. Your explanatory style is not fixed and unchangeable. You can gain a set of skills which free you from habitual pessimism and allows you to use optimism when you choose. -
The Relationship Between Cognitive Variables in the Context of Depressive Symptoms John C
UNF Digital Commons UNF Graduate Theses and Dissertations Student Scholarship 2016 Thinking About How You Feel: The Relationship between Cognitive Variables in the Context of Depressive Symptoms John C. Horton University of North Florida Suggested Citation Horton, John C., "Thinking About How You Feel: The Relationship between Cognitive Variables in the Context of Depressive Symptoms" (2016). UNF Graduate Theses and Dissertations. 630. https://digitalcommons.unf.edu/etd/630 This Master's Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Scholarship at UNF Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in UNF Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of UNF Digital Commons. For more information, please contact Digital Projects. © 2016 All Rights Reserved Running head: THINKING ABOUT HOW YOU FEEL Thinking About How You Feel: The Relationships between Cognitive Variables in the Context of Depressive Symptoms John C Horton University of North Florida THINKING ABOUT HOW YOU FEEL 2 Abstract Major depressive disorder is a mental disorder characterized by multiple symptoms such as psychomotor retardation, sleep disturbances, and cognitive deficits in decision making. The current study explores the relationships between cognitive variables and depressive symptomology and seeks to determine what predictive relationships exist between these constructs and if items from these constructs can accurately classify depressed persons. A normal sample of N = 116 participants were administered the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) as well as the Adult Hope Scale (ADH; Snyder et al., 1991), the Index of Autonomous Functioning(IAF; Weinstein, Przybylski, & Ryan, 2012), the Life Orientation Test-Revised(LOT-R; Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994), the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory(ZTPI; Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999), the Rumination Reflection Questionnaire(RRQ; Trapnell & Campbell, 1999), and the Automated Working Memory Assessment-II (AWMA; Alloway, 2012b). -
Study of Relationship Between Optimism and Resilience in the Times of COVID-19 Among University Students
The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (Online) | ISSN: 2349-3429 (Print) Volume 8, Issue 3, July- Sep, 2020 DIP: 18.01.157/20200803, DOI: 10.25215/0803.157 http://www.ijip.in Research Paper Study of relationship between optimism and resilience in the times of COVID-19 among university students Dr. Aruna Maheshwari1*, Ms. Varda Jutta2 ABSTRACT The purpose of present research is to study the effect of COVID-19 Pandemic on the relationship between Optimism and Resilience among the university students. Optimism and Resilience of 100 university students (36 males and 64 females) were assessed by using optimism pessimism scale and a brief resilience scale. Digital survey method was adopted to reach out to the university students between 18-23 yrs. of age group and Pearson correlation coefficient method was used to establish the relationship between the variables. As a result, it was found that optimism and resilience share a positive relationship (0.507) not only otherwise but in the challenging times too. Findings revealed that to increase the resilience one has to increase the positive and optimistic thoughts. The research can add value to the detailed study on optimism and its relation with resilience and psychological well-being during unforeseen period. This study also gives further opportunity for a comparative study between the optimism and resilience among different age groups. Keywords: Optimism, Resilience, COVID-19, Pandemic, Positive Psychology he pandemic of COVID-19 posed a different range of challenges and adjustments for the entire world. Many countries had to take harsh steps to safeguard the life of their T citizens and for that they imposed complete lockdown on their citizens, just like India. -
Learned Helplessness in Humans: Critique and Reformulation
Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1978, Vol. 87, No. 1, 49-74 Learned Helplessness in Humans: Critique and Reformulation Lyn Y. Abramson and Martin E. P. Seligman University of Pennsylvania John D. Teasdale Oxford University, England The learned helplessness hypothesis is criticized and reformulated. The old hypothesis, when applied to learned helplessness in humans, has two major problems: (a) It does not distinguish between cases in which outcomes are uncontrollable for all people and cases in which they are uncontrollable only - for some people (univervsal vs. personal helplessness), and (b) it does not explain when helplessness is general and when specific, or when chronic and when acute. A reformulation based on a revision of attribution theory is pro- posed to resolve these inadequacies. According to the reformulation, once people perceive noncontingency, they attribute their helplessness to a cause. This cause can be stable or unstable, global or specific, and internal or external. The attribution chosen influences whether expectation of future helplessness will be chronic or acute, broad or narrow, and whether helplessness will lower self-esteem or not. The implications of this reformulation of human helplessness for the learned helplessness model of depression are outlined. Over the past 10 years a large number of 1967). Paralleling the experimental findings experiments have shown that a variety of orga- with dogs, the debilitating consequences of nisms exposed to uncontrollable events often uncontrollable events have been demonstrated exhibit subsequent disruption of behavior (see in cats (Masserman, 1971; Seward & Hum- Maier & Seligman, 1976, for a review of the phrey, 1967; Thomas & Dewald, 1977), in infrahuman literature). -
Learned Resourcefulness and Coping with Stress in Mothers of Children with Disabilities
Eurasian Journal of Educational Research, Issue 55, 2014, 243-262 Learned Resourcefulness and Coping with Stress in Mothers of Children with Disabilities Yüksel EROĞLU* Sırrı AKBABA** Orhan ADIGÜZEL*** Adem PEKER**** Suggested Citation: Eroğlu, Y., Akbaba, S., Adıgüzel, O., & Peker, A. (2014). Learned resourcefulness and coping with stress in mothers of children with disabilities. Eurasian Journal of Educational Research, 55, 243-262. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.14689/ejer.2014.55.14 Abstract Problem Statement: Research has indicated that some mothers can cope with stressful life conditions and continue their lives normally, whereas others are unable to overcome such challenging conditions. Recent research has shown that mothers of children with disabilities are likely to know more about why some mothers have relatively well-adjusted lives despite stressful conditions, as well as why they cope better with these conditions than others. However, to date, studies in Turkey have only concentrated on the degree to which mothers of children with disabilities feel stress and which coping ways they use to curb the negative effects of stress. Purpose of the Study: This study aimed to examine the relationships between learned resourcefulness and ways of coping with stress. Methods: The study sample consisted of 222 mothers of children attending special education institutions during the 2011–2012 academic year in Sakarya, Turkey. The Self-Control Schedule and Ways of Coping Inventory were used to assess learned resourcefulness and coping strategies, the -
Martin E.P. Seligman Curriculum Vitae Updated: January 29, 2018
Martin E.P. Seligman Curriculum Vitae Updated: January 29, 2018 Office Address: Positive Psychology Center University of Pennsylvania 3701 Market St., Second Floor Philadelphia, PA 19104 Phone: 215-898-7173 E-mail: [email protected] [email protected] Website: http://ppc.sas.upenn.edu/ Personal Information: Born: August 12, 1942 in Albany, New York Married (Mandy McCarthy Seligman), seven children: Amanda, David, Lara, Nicole, Darryl, Carly, and Jenny DEGREES A.B., Princeton University, summa cum laude (Philosophy), 1964 Ph.D., University of Pennsylvania (Psychology), 1967 Ph.D., Honoris Causa, Uppsala University, Sweden, 1989 Doctor of Humane Letters, Honoris Causa, Massachusetts College of Professional Psychology, 1997 Ph.D., Honoris Causa, Complutense University, Madrid, 2004 Doctor of Science, Honoris Causa, University of East London, 2006 Doctor of Humane Letters, Honoris Causa, Lewis and Clark University, 2012 Doctor of Public Service, Honoris Causa, Widener University, 2015 Doctor of Science, Honoris Causa, University of Buckingham, 2018 APPOINTMENTS—recent 2017- Chair, Education Committee, Global Happiness Council, United Nations M.E.P. Seligman — curriculum vitae Page 2 of 65 2016- Honorary Director, Tsinghua Happiness Technology Lab (Beijing) 2015- Director, The Imagination Institute, National Philanthropic Trust 2014- Distinguished Senior Advisor, International Positive Education Network 2012- Chair, Steering Committee on Prospection, Templeton Foundation 2011- Member, Technical Advisory Group, Measuring National Well-being