Learned Optimism

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Learned Optimism LEARNED OPTIMISM Did you know: • The way we think, especially about health, changes our health? • Optimists have better health habits than pessimists do? • Optimists tend to do better at school, at work, and on the playing field? • Optimism is a learned skill, one that can be permanently acquired? Learned helplessness is the giving-up reaction, the quitting response that follows from the belief that whatever you do doesn’t matter. Explanatory style is the manner in which you habitually explain to yourself why events happen. Learned optimism is the power of “non-negative thinking,” using the skills of changing the destructive things you say to yourself when you experience the losses and setbacks of life. Using learned optimism you can change your explanatory style to resist helplessness by: • understanding that the causes of bad events may be temporary; • realizing that your problems in one area aren’t universal; • affirming that bad events aren’t totally your fault. Practicing the thinking skills of learned optimism can give you the energy to keep on succeeding at life goals. You can strengthen your self-esteem because you know you can solve temporary problems in creative ways. You can learn to be more productive and less depressed, more hopeful and less discouraged, to achieve more successes than failures. Learned optimism allows you to change your self-talk and beliefs so you can face adversity in responsible and realistic ways. Your explanatory style is not fixed and unchangeable. You can gain a set of skills which free you from habitual pessimism and allows you to use optimism when you choose. Resource: Seligman, M. Harborview Center for Sexual Assault and Traumatic Stress 03 .
Recommended publications
  • Learned Helplessness: Theory and Evidence
    \, Journal ol Experimental Psychology: General 1976, Vol. 105, No. 1, 3-46 Learned Helplessness: Theory and Evidence Steven F. Maier Martin E. P. Seligman University of Colorado University of Pennsylvania SUMMARY In 1967, Overmier and Seligman found that dogs exposed to inescapable and unavoidable electric shocks in one situation later failed to learn to escape shock in a different situation where escape was possible. Shortly thereafter Seligman and Maier (1967) demonstrated that this effect was caused by the uncontrollability of the original shocks. In this article we review the effects of exposing organisms to aversive events which they cannot control, and we review the explanations which have been offered. There seem to be motivational, cognitive, and emotional effects of uncontrol- lability. (a) Motivation. Dogs that have been exposed to inescapable shocks do not subsequently initiate escape response in the presence of shock. We review parallel phenomena in cats, fish, rats, and man. Of particular interest is the discussion of learned helplessness in rats and man. Rats are of interest because learned helplessness has been difficult to demonstrate in rats. However, we show that inescapably shocked rats do fail to learn to escape if the escape task is rea- sonably difficult. With regard to man, we review a variety of studies using in- escapable noise and unsolvable problems as agents which produce learned helpless- ness effects on both instrumental and cognitive tasks, (b) Cognition. We argue that exposure to uncontrollable events interferes with the organism's tendency to perceive contingent relationships between its behavior and outcomes. Here we review a variety of studies showing such a cognitive set.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects Of
    Buffalo Law Review Volume 64 Number 2 Article 5 4-1-2016 Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Major Depressive Disorder on Refugees and Explain Why These Disorders Should Qualify as Extraordinary Circumstances Excusing Untimely Asylum Applications Brandon R. White University at Buffalo School of Law (Student) Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.law.buffalo.edu/buffalolawreview Part of the Immigration Law Commons Recommended Citation Brandon R. White, Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Major Depressive Disorder on Refugees and Explain Why These Disorders Should Qualify as Extraordinary Circumstances Excusing Untimely Asylum Applications, 64 Buff. L. Rev. 413 (2016). Available at: https://digitalcommons.law.buffalo.edu/buffalolawreview/vol64/iss2/5 This Comment is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at Digital Commons @ University at Buffalo School of Law. It has been accepted for inclusion in Buffalo Law Review by an authorized editor of Digital Commons @ University at Buffalo School of Law. For more information, please contact [email protected]. COMMENT Using Learned Helplessness to Understand the Effects of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Major Depressive Disorder on Refugees and Explain Why These Disorders Should Qualify as Extraordinary Circumstances Excusing Untimely Asylum Applications BRANDON R. WHITE† INTRODUCTION Imagine you are a refugee. For some personal reason outside of your control, say your race or religion, you are targeted by others and suffer some harm. This harm almost certainly involves some level of violence. Perhaps you are beaten, raped, or tortured. Maybe you watched your family and friends suffer the same, maybe some of these family and friends were murdered in front of your eyes.
    [Show full text]
  • Mood and Self-Regulation Changes in Underrecovery: an Intervention Model
    Davis, H., Botterill, C., & MacNeill, K. (2002). Mood and self-regulation changes in underrecovery: An intervention model. In M. Kellmann (Ed.), Enhancing recovery: Preventing underperformance in athletes (pp. 161-179). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. 9 Mood and Self-Regulation Changes in Underrecovery: An Intervention Model Henry Davis IV, Cal Botterill, and Karen MacNeill Vince Lombardi (as cited in Maraniss, 1999, p. havioral components. This chapter identifies em- 217) is quoted as having said in 1959, “Fatigue pirical research that serves as the scientific base makes cowards of us all.” Did Lombardi know for the responsible intervention in UR; case ex- about underrecovery back then? The athlete in amples follow. We begin by elaborating on the high-intensity training provides a wonderful ex- concept of empirically supported treatment as it ample for the elucidation of psychological factors applies to intervention in UR. that can adversely affect performance when the Our position on cause and effect is that poor self- athlete is underrecovered. Lombardi knew well regulation and low mood may sometimes result in that fatigue can trigger many emotions. During poor athletic performance and prolong inadequate training camps high-performance athletes incur recovery; at other times, these factors may directly training loads that surpass general training lev- result from inadequate recovery. Recovery occurs els; this high-intensity training compromises physi- at physical, psychological, and social levels. We ological recovery and can bring a deterioration of therefore propose continuous two-way interactions mood (Kellmann & Kallus, 1999). Such loads de- between psychological and physiological states. mand physical and psychological adaptive re- sponses to restore homeostasis as described in chapter 1 of this volume.
    [Show full text]
  • Bullying and Peer Victimization
    JIVXXX10.1177/0886260515572476Journal of Interpersonal ViolenceRadliff et al. 572476research-article2015 Article Journal of Interpersonal Violence 2016, Vol. 31(11) 1983 –2005 Bullying and Peer © The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: Victimization: An sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0886260515572476 Examination of Cognitive jiv.sagepub.com and Psychosocial Constructs Kisha M. Radliff, PhD,1 Cixin Wang, PhD,2 and Susan M. Swearer, PhD3 Abstract Research has demonstrated a link between internalizing factors and bullying perpetration and peer victimization; however, few studies have examined predictors of cognitive and psychosocial factors, such as locus of control and hopelessness. The current study examined cognitive and psychosocial factors in bullying perpetration and peer victimization in a sample of 469 middle school students. A mediator model of hopelessness was also investigated. Students involved in bullying reported a greater external locus of control compared with peers who were not involved in bullying. Bully- victims endorsed the highest externality. Results showed that hopelessness fully mediated the relationship between verbal/relational victimization and external locus of control for the victim group, but not the bully-victim group. Implications for bullying prevention and intervention efforts are discussed. Keywords bullying, hopelessness, locus of control, children, adolescents 1The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA 2University of California, Riverside, CA, USA 3University of Nebraska–Lincoln, NE, USA Corresponding Author: Kisha M. Radliff, Assistant Professor of School Psychology, The Ohio State University, PAES Bldg. Rm. 436, 305 W. 17th Ave., Columbus, OH, 43210, USA. Email: [email protected] 1984 Journal of Interpersonal Violence 31(11) Within the last few decades, bullying among school-aged youth has increas- ingly been recognized as an important problem in schools and presents in many forms (i.e., physical, verbal, relational, and cyber).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Models of Depression: a Critique of the Emphasis on Positive Reinforcement
    International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy Volume 4, No. 2, 2008 Behavioral Models of Depression: A Critique of the Emphasis on Positive Reinforcement Paulo Roberto Abreu & Carlos E. Santos Abstract This paper provides a review of behavioral models of depression highlighting the problems associated with its historical emphasis on lowered frequencies of positive reinforcement. We analyzed the models of Ferster and Lewinsohn in their theoretical approach, methodology and application. We conducted a review of the suppressive characteristics exercised by punishment, by the presentation of non-contingent aversive stimulation and by operant extinction. A number of recommendations are made concerning how these processes can inform interventions. We conclude that interventions geared towards identifying aversive control play a promising role in clinical change in the treatment of depression. Keywords: Aversive Control, Response-Contingent Positive Reinforcement, Depression, Clinical Behavior Analysis, Functional Assessment, Behavioral Activation. The present article aims to revisit the principles of behavioral models of depression by emphasizing advancements in the field, as well as providing a critique of the dominant models. With this goal in mind, we aim to re-direct the conceptual analysis towards an understanding of the environmental variables frequently observed in the behaviors of depressed individuals. Drawing from the existing literature, we propose possible variables associated with behavioral suppression and its mechanisms during the development and course of depressive behavior repertories. Three particular behavioral processes are considered: (1) suppressive characteristics exercised by punishment, (2) the presentation of non-contingent aversive stimulation, and (3) operant extinction. We begin with a discussion of the dominant models of depression, followed by a critique, and we conclude with a discussion of how each of these processes can inform clinical interventions.
    [Show full text]
  • Learned Helplessness in Humans: Critique and Reformulation
    Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1978, Vol. 87, No. 1, 49-74 Learned Helplessness in Humans: Critique and Reformulation Lyn Y. Abramson and Martin E. P. Seligman University of Pennsylvania John D. Teasdale Oxford University, England The learned helplessness hypothesis is criticized and reformulated. The old hypothesis, when applied to learned helplessness in humans, has two major problems: (a) It does not distinguish between cases in which outcomes are uncontrollable for all people and cases in which they are uncontrollable only - for some people (univervsal vs. personal helplessness), and (b) it does not explain when helplessness is general and when specific, or when chronic and when acute. A reformulation based on a revision of attribution theory is pro- posed to resolve these inadequacies. According to the reformulation, once people perceive noncontingency, they attribute their helplessness to a cause. This cause can be stable or unstable, global or specific, and internal or external. The attribution chosen influences whether expectation of future helplessness will be chronic or acute, broad or narrow, and whether helplessness will lower self-esteem or not. The implications of this reformulation of human helplessness for the learned helplessness model of depression are outlined. Over the past 10 years a large number of 1967). Paralleling the experimental findings experiments have shown that a variety of orga- with dogs, the debilitating consequences of nisms exposed to uncontrollable events often uncontrollable events have been demonstrated exhibit subsequent disruption of behavior (see in cats (Masserman, 1971; Seward & Hum- Maier & Seligman, 1976, for a review of the phrey, 1967; Thomas & Dewald, 1977), in infrahuman literature).
    [Show full text]
  • Learned Resourcefulness and Coping with Stress in Mothers of Children with Disabilities
    Eurasian Journal of Educational Research, Issue 55, 2014, 243-262 Learned Resourcefulness and Coping with Stress in Mothers of Children with Disabilities Yüksel EROĞLU* Sırrı AKBABA** Orhan ADIGÜZEL*** Adem PEKER**** Suggested Citation: Eroğlu, Y., Akbaba, S., Adıgüzel, O., & Peker, A. (2014). Learned resourcefulness and coping with stress in mothers of children with disabilities. Eurasian Journal of Educational Research, 55, 243-262. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.14689/ejer.2014.55.14 Abstract Problem Statement: Research has indicated that some mothers can cope with stressful life conditions and continue their lives normally, whereas others are unable to overcome such challenging conditions. Recent research has shown that mothers of children with disabilities are likely to know more about why some mothers have relatively well-adjusted lives despite stressful conditions, as well as why they cope better with these conditions than others. However, to date, studies in Turkey have only concentrated on the degree to which mothers of children with disabilities feel stress and which coping ways they use to curb the negative effects of stress. Purpose of the Study: This study aimed to examine the relationships between learned resourcefulness and ways of coping with stress. Methods: The study sample consisted of 222 mothers of children attending special education institutions during the 2011–2012 academic year in Sakarya, Turkey. The Self-Control Schedule and Ways of Coping Inventory were used to assess learned resourcefulness and coping strategies, the
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Experimental Psychology Copyright © 1974 by the American Psychological Association, Inc
    Journal of Experimental Psychology Copyright © 1974 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. VOL. 102, No. 2 FEBRUARY 1974 LOCUS OF CONTROL AND LEARNED HELPLESSNESS ' DONALD S. HIROTO2 University of Portland Failure to escape, the defining characteristic of learned helplessness, was investigated with perceived and instructed locus of control 5s in a learned- helplessness paradigm. Three groups, equally divided between internals and externals and counterbalanced for sex, received different treatments with an aversive tone prior to the testing for helplessness. The first group could neither escape nor avoid an aversive tone, the second group could escape the tone, and the third group was not exposed to the treatment. Eighteen escape-avoidance trials followed, using a human analogue to an animal shuttle box, in which 6s received an instructional set describing the task as skill or chance determined. In addition to a complete replication of learned helplessness in man, externals were significantly more helpless than internals, and chance-set 5s were more helpless than skill-set 5s. Since uncontrol- lability of noise, externality, and chance instructional set all impaired escape- avoidance in parallel ways, it was speculated that a common state may underlie all 3 dimensions—expectancy that responding and reinforcement are independent. Overmier and Seligman (1967) and incentive for initiating responses had been Seligman and Maier (1967) demonstrated a lowered, and the ability to associate re- profound interference with shuttle box spending and shock had been proactively escape-avoidance behavior of dogs given impaired. "Learned helplessness" was prior inescapable electric shock. They chosen as the descriptive label for the hypothesized that dogs given inescapable phenomenon and also as the hypothesized shock failed to escape later because they process by which learning of independence had learned that shock termination was between responding and reinforcement independent of responding.
    [Show full text]
  • Learned Helplessness at Fifty: Insights from Neuroscience
    Psychological Review © 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 123, No. 4, 349–367 0033-295X/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rev0000033 Learned Helplessness at Fifty: Insights From Neuroscience Steven F. Maier Martin E. P. Seligman University of Colorado at Boulder University of Pennsylvania Learned helplessness, the failure to escape shock induced by uncontrollable aversive events, was discovered half a century ago. Seligman and Maier (1967) theorized that animals learned that outcomes were independent of their responses—that nothing they did mattered—and that this learning undermined trying to escape. The mechanism of learned helplessness is now very well-charted biologically, and the original theory got it backward. Passivity in response to shock is not learned. It is the default, unlearned response to prolonged aversive events and it is mediated by the serotonergic activity of the dorsal raphe nucleus, which in turn inhibits escape. This passivity can be overcome by learning control, with the activity of the medial prefrontal cortex, which subserves the detection of control leading to the automatic inhibition of the dorsal raphe nucleus. So animals learn that they can control aversive events, but the passive failure to learn to escape is an unlearned reaction to prolonged aversive stimulation. In addition, alterations of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex-dorsal raphe pathway can come to subserve the expectation of control. We speculate that default passivity and the compensating detection and expec- tation of control may have substantial implications for how to treat depression. Keywords: depression, dorsal raphe nucleus, hope, learned helplessness, ventromedial prefrontal cortex In the early 1960s, Richard Solomon and his students at the shuttlebox and passively waited the shock out (Leaf, 1964; Over- University of Pennsylvania wanted to know how prior Pavlovian mier & Leaf, 1965).
    [Show full text]
  • The Loss of Autonomy in Abused Persons: Psychological, Moral, and Legal Dimensions
    humanities Article The Loss of Autonomy in Abused Persons: Psychological, Moral, and Legal Dimensions Michelle Ciurria School of Humanities and Languages, UNSW, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia; [email protected] Received: 3 March 2018; Accepted: 30 April 2018; Published: 17 May 2018 Abstract: This paper tries to resolve a tension in popular conceptions of intimate partner violence (IPV). On the one hand, we correctly assume that all abused persons are not the same: they have irreducibly plural personalities. On the other hand, we correctly assume that abused persons suffer from a loss of autonomy. The puzzle is: if abused persons share deficits in autonomy, why does it not follow that they share a set of personality traits? I argue that the psychological states implicated in autonomy-impairment in abused persons are situation-sensitive responses to salient eliciting conditions, not personality traits. This view has substantive moral and legal implications, as it implies that abusers are responsible for inflicting severe moral harms on victim-survivors, and they may also be liable for unlawful abduction and rape, in case the abused person lives with or has sexual contact with the abuser. This is because the conditions of abuse undermine the victim-survivor’s ability to autonomously consent to cohabitation and sexual contact with the abuser. I argue that the best way of protecting people from autonomy-undermining abuse is public education. Keywords: intimate partner violence; victim-survivor; autonomy; social psychology; social cognition; personality 1. Tension: Psychological Pluralism versus Shared Autonomy Deficits Amy Schumer’s recent autobiography, ‘Girl with the Lower Back Tattoo’ (2016), is a mix of light-hearted anecdotes and narratives of personal struggle.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hopelessness Theory of Depression: a Quarter- Century in Review
    The Hopelessness Theory of Depression: A Quarter- Century in Review Richard T. Liu, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University; Bradley Hospital Evan M. Kleiman, Department of Psychology, Harvard University Bridget A. Nestor and Shayna M. Cheek, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University; Bradley Hospital Since the formulation of the hopelessness theory of ness theory and the reformulated learned helplessness depression (Psychological Review, 96, 1989, 358) a theory. quarter-century ago, it has garnered considerable inter- Key words: cognitive vulnerability, depression, hope- est. The current article presents a systematic review of lessness theory, inferential style. [Clin Psychol Sci Prac – this theory, including its subsequent elaborations (Rose 22: 345 365, 2015] and Abramson’s [Rochester Symposium of Develop- Major depressive disorder is a highly prevalent clinical mental Psychopathology, 1992, University of Rochester condition, with estimates for lifetime rates of its occur- Press, Rochester, NY] developmental elaboration; Abela rence ranging from 13.2% to 16.6% (Hasin, Goodwin, and Sarin’s [Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 2002, Stinson, & Grant, 2005; Kessler et al., 2005). Further- 811] weakest link approach; Panzarella, Alloy and more, it is a growing public health concern. Indeed, Whitehouse’s [Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, depression is currently the leading cause of the global 2006, 307] expansion of the hopelessness theory; and burden of disease (World Health Organization, 2008). the hopelessness theory of suicide (Suicide Science: Arriving at a better understanding of the mechanisms Expanding Boundaries, 2000, Kluwer Academic, Bos- underlying risk for first onset and recurrence of this ton]), followed by recommendations for future study.
    [Show full text]
  • Helpless Behavior
    Behav. Res. Thu. Vol. 31, No. 3, pp. 289-295, 1993 OOOS-7%7/93 S6.00 + 0.00 Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved Copyright @ 1993 Pergamon Press Ltd HELPLESS BEHAVIOR CHRISTOPHERPETERSGN Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 580 Union Drive, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1346, U.S.A. (Received 23 April 1992) Stuutnary-In the present research, we investigated helpless behavior and the responses it elicits from others. Study One used the act-frequency approach to develop a self-report measure of prototypically helpless behaviors. In a sample of college students (n = 75). this measure was validated against ratings by others. Many of the prototypically helpless behaviors we identified were interpersonal in nature, implying that they entail dependency and perhaps manipulativeness. In Study Two, we asked young adult (n = 249) Ss how they responded to people who showed varying amounts of helplessness. The most frequent response was trying to help the helpless individual feel better. As the frequency of helpless behavior increased, people were less likely to try to make him or her feel better, less likely to go along with the helpless individual, more likely to become angry and more likely to ignore or avoid this individual. Implications of these results for applications of the learned helplessness model were discussed, in particular the need to be cautious in assuming that all instances of observed passivity are good examples of learned helplessness. INTRODUCTION One of the most popular theories in contempory psychology is the learned helplessness model (Maier & Seligman, 1976). It provides a succinct explanation for the inappropriate passivity that may follow in the wake of people’s experience with uncontrollable events.
    [Show full text]