Plant Foods Consumed by Pan: Exploring the Variation of Nutritional Ecology Across Africa

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Plant Foods Consumed by Pan: Exploring the Variation of Nutritional Ecology Across Africa AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 000:000–000 (2010) Plant Foods Consumed by Pan: Exploring the Variation of Nutritional Ecology Across Africa Gottfried Hohmann,1* Kevin Potts,2 Antoine N’Guessan,1,3 Andrew Fowler,1,4 Roger Mundry,1 Joerg U. Ganzhorn,5 and Sylvia Ortmann6 1Department of Primatology, Max-Planck-Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany 2Department of Anthropology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 3Swiss Centre of Scientific Research in Coˆte d’Ivoire (CSRS) and University of Cocody, 01 BP 1303, Abidjan 01, Coˆte d’Ivoire 4Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK 5Department of Animal Ecology and Conservation, Hamburg University, 20146 Hamburg, Germany 6Leibniz Institute for Zoo and Wildlife Research, 10315 Berlin, Germany KEY WORDS chimpanzee; bonobo; frugivory; diet quality; geographic variation ABSTRACT It has been shown that differences in nutrient components. Overall plant samples collected at resource density and nutrient supply affect variation in each site differed in terms of macronutrient content. ranging patterns, habitat use, and sociality. Among However, nutritious quality and gross energy content of nonhuman primates, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)and food samples were similar suggesting that dietary qual- bonobos (P. paniscus) have often been used as models ity reflects selectivity rather than habitat ecology. The for the link between social system and habitat ecology. quality of plant foods consumed by bonobos was within Field reports suggest that resource density is higher in the range of chimpanzees and the quality of plant foods habitats occupied by bonobos (compared to chimpanzee consumed by western chimpanzees was not higher than habitats), and in the West (compared to the East) of the that of eastern chimpanzees. While the results showed range of chimpanzees. In this study we compared diet significant variation across forests inhabited by Pan, quality at the level of species and populations using in- theydidnotmatchwithgeographicalpatternsbetween formation from nutritional analyses of fruit and leaves and within Pan species as proposed in previous studies. consumed by chimpanzees (three) and bonobos (one This suggests that the nutritional quality of the habitat population). Quality of plant foods was assessed on the is not always a reliable predictor of the quality of the basis of a) the concentration of macronutrients, diet. Am J Phys Anthropol 000:000–000, 2010. VC 2009 fiber, and anti-feedants, and b) associations of different Wiley-Liss, Inc. The diet of medium or small body sized vertebrates that fluctuate in time and space (Hashimoto et al., 2004; tends to include relatively large amounts of fruit and the Lehmann et al., 2007). preference for a fruit-based diet is usually related to the Information on the feeding ecology of Pan has been high content of easily digestible macronutrients, such as reported from a large number of sites across Africa, nonstructural carbohydrates and lipids, and to low levels including deciduous, woodland, and evergreen forests in of indigestible fibers and antifeedants (Milton, 1987). West, East, and Central Africa (Nishida and Uehara, Despite these nutritional advantages, frugivory tends to 1983; Newton-Fisher, 1999; Hunt and McGrew, 2002; be facultative in vertebrates, even among so-called fruit Morgan and Sanz, 2006). Corresponding information specialists, with constraints such as the spatio-temporal from bonobos comes from sites in the North (Badrian distribution of reproductive plant parts limiting the and Malenky, 1984; Kano, 1992), and South (Sabater Pi extent to which animals can subsist on an entirely fruit- and Vea, 1993; Hohmann et al., 2006) of the species’ dis- based diet (Rode and Robbins, 2000). Large body size generally permits a low-energy, low-nutrient density diet Grant sponsors: Max-Planck-Society; L.S.B. Leakey Foundation; (Clauss et al., 2008). As a consequence, large animals National Geographic Society; Volkswagen Foundation; Wenner Gren can subsist on low-quality plant foods with a high fiber Foundation; American Society of Primatologists; Swiss Centre of content and relatively low nutrient density while smaller Scientific Research; Chester Zoo Nigeria Biodiversity Programme; animals have to search for higher quality items (Dem- Dean’s travel Fund of UCL and Leventis Ltd.; Pro Natura Interna- ment and van Soest, 1985). In this regard chimpanzees tional/Lagos. (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (P. paniscus) are excep- tional. In spite of their large body mass (Smith and *Correspondence to: Gottfried Hohmann, Department of Primatol- Jungers, 1997) they are among the most consistently fru- ogy, Max-Planck-Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany. E-mail: [email protected] givorous primates, in that they maintain their frugivo- rous habits even at times of low fruit availability (White Received 20 November 2008; accepted 21 July 2009 and Lanjouw, 1994; Wrangham et al., 1998). This adher- ence to frugivory requires flexibility in ranging and DOI 10.1002/ajpa.21168 grouping patterns, and the fission-fusion system of Pan Published online in Wiley InterScience is likely to be one adaptation for exploiting food patches (www.interscience.wiley.com). VC 2009 WILEY-LISS, INC. 2 G. HOHMANN ET AL. tributional range, which is entirely restricted to the ing on the abundance, distribution, and quality of avail- Democratic Republic of Congo. able plant foods, ranging from fruit to herbaceous vege- Habitats occupied by Pan differ in terms of their floris- tation (Malenky et al., 1994; Wrangham, 2000; Lambert, tic composition (Rodman, 2002) and resource abundance 2007). However, the original model that emphasized con- (Doran et al., 2002) and this variation is thought to sistent disparity between the two sister species affect habitat use, diet composition, and sociality both (Wrangham, 1986) is not unchallenged. First, although a within and between the species (Wrangham, 1986; comparison of Lomako bonobos with chimpanzees from Boesch, 1996). Comparison of hair samples from two Gombe (Tanzania) suggested that bonobos have access to populations of savanna chimpanzees revealed different larger food patches of tree fruits than chimpanzees (White isotope signatures which were related to foraging behav- & Wrangham, 1988), a comparison of Lomako bonobos ior and nutrient intake (Schoeninger et al., 1999). The with chimpanzees from Kanyawara (Uganda) did not con- two chimpanzee populations at Gombe and Mahale differ firm the proposed differences in grouping and food distri- in terms of food selection and food processing (Nishida et bution (Chapman et al., 1994). Similarly, though Malenky al., 1983) and similar differences in the use of plant (1990) proposed that the quality of fruit consumed by foods have been reported from other communities Lomako bonobos exceeds that consumed by East African (Newton-Fisher, 1999; Boesch et al., 2006). Although chimpanzees, nutritional analyses of food from a bonobo other factors are likely to be involved, ecological parame- population from Salonga and a population of Nigerian ters are thought to be the key underlying the observed chimpanzees from Gashaka (Nigeria) indicated both simi- variation. Despite the considerable attention given to the larities and differences (Hohmann et al., 2006). While influence of food distributional patterns in space and plant foods of the two species were similar in terms of time as key ecological determinants of these intra- and their overall composition, they differed in terms of how interspecific variations (Chapman et al., 1995; Doran et macronutrients and antifeedants were associated. Results al., 2002), no explanatory patterns have yet emerged. from this study indicate that it is not nutrient content but Assessments of spatial and temporal patterns of resource the association of macronutrients with antifeedants that abundance within and between sites are frequently affects fruit quality between the two species. based on rainfall data and phenological accounts such as Second, recent information from long-term chimpanzee leaf flushing and fruit production (Tutin and Fernandez, study sites has yielded a complex scenario and has 1993). While these approaches address some quantitative shifted key assumptions of previous models (Mitani et aspects of food availability, an often overlooked source of al., 2000; Lehmann and Boesch, 2004; Murray et al., variability among sites is the nutrient content and nutri- 2006). There is considerable variation in the grouping ent density of plant foods. Studies on herbivores have patterns and bonding relationships between and within shown how variation of food quality can promote or con- the sexes among populations of chimpanzees. The varia- strain activity budgets, ranging patterns and sociality, tion in chimpanzee sociality has been related to differen- such as female gregariousness, in a similar manner as ces in resource distribution (Balcomb et al., 2000; Leh- food distribution and predation pressure (Hamel and mann et al., 2007), and demography (Lehmann and Cote, 2007; Bailey and Provenza, 2008). The ideal free Boesch, 2004), leading to a proposed dichotomy between theory offers a framework that explains the relationship East and West African chimpanzees (Doran et al., 2002). between habitat quality, population density and resource While the ecological differentiation fits the original competition (Fretwell and Lucas, 1970; Fretwell, 1972). model, the geographic location
Recommended publications
  • EAZA Best Practice Guidelines Bonobo (Pan Paniscus)
    EAZA Best Practice Guidelines Bonobo (Pan paniscus) Editors: Dr Jeroen Stevens Contact information: Royal Zoological Society of Antwerp – K. Astridplein 26 – B 2018 Antwerp, Belgium Email: [email protected] Name of TAG: Great Ape TAG TAG Chair: Dr. María Teresa Abelló Poveda – Barcelona Zoo [email protected] Edition: First edition - 2020 1 2 EAZA Best Practice Guidelines disclaimer Copyright (February 2020) by EAZA Executive Office, Amsterdam. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced in hard copy, machine-readable or other forms without advance written permission from the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA). Members of the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria (EAZA) may copy this information for their own use as needed. The information contained in these EAZA Best Practice Guidelines has been obtained from numerous sources believed to be reliable. EAZA and the EAZA APE TAG make a diligent effort to provide a complete and accurate representation of the data in its reports, publications, and services. However, EAZA does not guarantee the accuracy, adequacy, or completeness of any information. EAZA disclaims all liability for errors or omissions that may exist and shall not be liable for any incidental, consequential, or other damages (whether resulting from negligence or otherwise) including, without limitation, exemplary damages or lost profits arising out of or in connection with the use of this publication. Because the technical information provided in the EAZA Best Practice Guidelines can easily be misread or misinterpreted unless properly analysed, EAZA strongly recommends that users of this information consult with the editors in all matters related to data analysis and interpretation.
    [Show full text]
  • Recent Developments in the Study of Wild Chimpanzee Behavior
    Evolutionary Anthropology 9 ARTICLES Recent Developments in the Study of Wild Chimpanzee Behavior JOHN C. MITANI, DAVID P. WATTS, AND MARTIN N. MULLER Chimpanzees have always been of special interest to anthropologists. As our organization, genetics and behavior, closest living relatives,1–3 they provide the standard against which to assess hunting and meat-eating, inter-group human uniqueness and information regarding the changes that must have oc- relationships, and behavioral endocri- curred during the course of human evolution. Given these circumstances, it is not nology. Our treatment is selective, and surprising that chimpanzees have been studied intensively in the wild. Jane Good- we explicitly avoid comment on inter- all4,5 initiated the first long-term field study of chimpanzee behavior at the Gombe population variation in behavior as it National Park, Tanzania. Her observations of tool manufacture and use, hunting, relates to the question of chimpanzee and meat-eating forever changed the way we define humans. Field research on cultures. Excellent reviews of this chimpanzee behavior by Toshisada Nishida and colleagues6 at the nearby Mahale topic, of central concern to anthropol- Mountains National Park has had an equally significant impact. It was Nishida7,8 ogists, can be found elsewhere.12–14 who first provided a comprehensive picture of the chimpanzee social system, including group structure and dispersal. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION No single issue in the study of wild Two generations of researchers erything about the behavior of these chimpanzee behavior has seen more have followed Goodall and Nishida apes in nature. But this is not the case. debate than the nature of their social into the field.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Representation of Magnitude in Gorillas and Orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes A,B, , Rachel F.L
    Cognition 168 (2017) 312–319 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Original Articles Spatial representation of magnitude in gorillas and orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes a,b, , Rachel F.L. Diamond c,g, Jasmine M. Hope d, Damien Caillaud e,f, Tara S. Stoinski a,e, Robert R. Hampton c,g a Zoo Atlanta, Atlanta, GA, United States b Bucknell University, Lewisburg, PA, United States c Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States d Neuroscience Graduate Program, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States e Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund International, Atlanta, GA, United States f Department of Anthropology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States g Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, GA, United States article info abstract Article history: Humans mentally represent magnitudes spatially; we respond faster to one side of space when process- Received 1 March 2017 ing small quantities and to the other side of space when processing large quantities. We determined Revised 24 July 2017 whether spatial representation of magnitude is a fundamental feature of primate cognition by testing Accepted 25 July 2017 for such space-magnitude correspondence in gorillas and orangutans. Subjects picked the larger quantity in a pair of dot arrays in one condition, and the smaller in another. Response latencies to the left and right sides of the screen were compared across the magnitude range. Apes showed evidence of spatial repre- Keywords: sentation of magnitude. While all subjects did not adopt the same orientation, apes showed consistent Space tendencies for spatial representations within individuals and systematically reversed these orientations SNARC Ape in response to reversal of the task instruction.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ledger and Times, April 16, 1953
    Murray State's Digital Commons The Ledger & Times Newspapers 4-16-1953 The Ledger and Times, April 16, 1953 The Ledger and Times Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.murraystate.edu/tlt Recommended Citation The Ledger and Times, "The Ledger and Times, April 16, 1953" (1953). The Ledger & Times. 1272. https://digitalcommons.murraystate.edu/tlt/1272 This Newspaper is brought to you for free and open access by the Newspapers at Murray State's Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in The Ledger & Times by an authorized administrator of Murray State's Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. :g hi A .0.• ..•••••• Aks AWL It. 11,f5N Selected As Best All sound Kentucky Community Newspaper for 1947 We Weather Are KN^ETUCKV: Fair with -ir -- terrepersteoires-neetr -Or a little Helping To 1411NNsorst• below freezing tomeht. low 30 to 34 in the east and a Build Murray , • `1,:*"\ ‘7441"--"7- 32 to 38 in the east portion. Friday fair and continued Each Day cool. r• YOUR PROGRESSIVE HOME NEWSPAPER United Press IN ITS 74th YEAR Murray, Ky., Thursday Afternioon, April 16, 1953 MURRAY POPULATION . - - 8,000 Vol. XXIX; No. 91 Vitality Dress Shoes IKE CHALLENGES RE!)s IN PEACE MOVE Basque, Red Calf and , als; In Flight Blue Calf Soon de Hoard I Now.4)':,t7".. Lions Will Be Six Point Program Listed $10.95 By LEIO, PANMUNJOM, ,./iApril 16 Sold To Aid By President To End War Al'OUttd • / (UP)-Red trucks b. /ambulances today delivered the first of 805 By MERRIMAN SMITH ,hopes with mere words and prom- I Allied sick and wounded prisoners Health Center WASHINGTON April 16 iUPI- ises and gestures," he said.
    [Show full text]
  • Orangutan Positional Behavior and the Nature of Arboreal Locomotion in Hominoidea Susannah K.S
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 000:000–000 (2006) Orangutan Positional Behavior and the Nature of Arboreal Locomotion in Hominoidea Susannah K.S. Thorpe1* and Robin H. Crompton2 1School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK 2Department of Human Anatomy and Cell Biology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 3GE, UK KEY WORDS Pongo pygmaeus; posture; orthograde clamber; forelimb suspend ABSTRACT The Asian apes, more than any other, are and orthograde compressive locomotor modes are ob- restricted to an arboreal habitat. They are consequently an served more frequently. Given the complexity of orangu- important model in the interpretation of the morphological tan positional behavior demonstrated by this study, it is commonalities of the apes, which are locomotor features likely that differences in positional behavior between associated with arboreal living. This paper presents a de- studies reflect differences in the interplay between the tailed analysis of orangutan positional behavior for all age- complex array of variables, which were shown to influence sex categories and during a complete range of behavioral orangutan positional behavior (Thorpe and Crompton [2005] contexts, following standardized positional mode descrip- Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127:58–78). With the exception tions proposed by Hunt et al. ([1996] Primates 37:363–387). of pronograde suspensory posture and locomotion, orang- This paper shows that orangutan positional behavior is utan positional behavior is similar to that of the African highly complex, representing a diverse spectrum of posi- apes, and in particular, lowland gorillas. This study sug- tional modes. Overall, all orthograde and pronograde sus- gests that it is orthogrady in general, rather than fore- pensory postures are exhibited less frequently in the pres- limb suspend specifically, that characterizes the posi- ent study than previously reported.
    [Show full text]
  • The Bonobo and Me
    THE BONOBO AND ME - COMPARING FAMILY TREES A PICTORIAL PRESENTATION With Marian Brickner A Campus Outreach Program in Science Education In cooperation with The University of Missouri-St. Louis Grade Level: Pre K-6 Pre-Visit Activities: Discuss what a family tree is and help students construct one of their family if possible. Presentation Time Period: Varies from 30-60 minutes as wanted. Materials: 1. A black “portfolio” case with several pictures 13 x 19, several 8 x 10's 2. A round “circular portfolio” case with a 5-foot U.S. map with ribbons on it showing where zoos are located that have Bonobo families. 3. Life size portrait of an adult Bonobo. 4. A weighted chain the average length of an adult male Bonobo (119cm) and one for the average adult female (111cm). 5. Large globe. 6. Skeletal hands, feet and skull replicas of humans (Homo sapiens), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and Bonobo (Pan paniscus). 7. One weight of 2 1/2 pounds and one 7 pounds. Introduction to Presentation: “Hi, I am Marian Brickner, a photographer. I am working on a book about a Bonobo family tree. Today I will introduce you to a family you may never have met yet.” Has anyone heard of a Bonobo? What do you think a Bonobo might be? What can you tell me about them? “Bonobos are one of the Great Apes.” Who can tell me an example of a great ape? “Scientists call them by their scientific name Pan paniscus.” What is the scientific name of human beings? Historical Perspective: “I have been following the family of a Bonobo named Linda.” (Show picture of Linda.) “Linda lives in Milwaukee and was born in 1956 in the Democratic Republic of the Congo, (Zaire) Africa in the central Congo Basin.” If she was born in 1956, how old is Linda today? Can anyone show me on the globe where she came from? Where do we live? “Linda has seven children, five girls and two boys.
    [Show full text]
  • LING 001 Introduction to Linguistics
    LING 001 Introduction to Linguistics Lecture #6 Animal Communication 2 02/05/2020 Katie Schuler Announcements • Exam 1 is next class (Monday)! • Remember there are no make-up exams (but your lowest exam score will be dropped) How to do well on the exam • Review the study guides • Make sure you can answer the practice problems • Come on time (exam is 50 minutes) • We MUST leave the room for the next class First two questions are easy Last time • Communication is everywhere in the animal kingdom! • Human language is • An unbounded discrete combinatorial system • Many animals have elements of this: • Honeybees, songbirds, primates • But none quite have language Case Study #4: Can Apes learn Language? Ape Projects • Viki (oral production) • Sign Language: • Washoe (Gardiner) (chimp) • Nim Chimpsky (Terrace) (chimp) • Koko (Patterson) (gorilla) • Kanzi (Savage-Rumbaugh) (bonobo) Viki’s `speech’ • Raised by psychologists • Tried to teach her oral language, but didn’t get far... Later Attempts • Later attempts used non-oral languages — • either symbols (Sarah, Kanzi) or • ASL (Washoe, Koko, Nim). • Extensive direct instruction by humans. • Many problems of interpretation and evaluation. Main one: is this a • miniature/incipient unbounded discrete combinatorial system, or • is it just rote learning+randomness? Washoe and Koko Video Washoe • A chimp who was extensively trained to use ASL by the Gardners • Knew 132 signs by age 5, and over 250 by the end of her life. • Showed some productive use (‘water bird’) • And even taught her adopted son Loulis some signs But the only deaf, native signer on the team • ‘Every time the chimp made a sign, we were supposed to write it down in the log… They were always complaining because my log didn’t show enough signs.
    [Show full text]
  • Western Lowland Gorilla (Gorilla Gorilla Gorilla) Diet and Activity Budgets: Effects of Group Size, Age Class and Food Availability in the Dzanga-Ndoki
    Western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) diet and activity budgets: effects of group size, age class and food availability in the Dzanga-Ndoki National Park, Central African Republic Terence Fuh MSc Primate Conservation Dissertation 2013 Dissertation Course Name: P20107 Title: Western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) diet and activity budgets: effects of group size, age class and food availability in the Dzanga-Ndoki National Park, Central African Republic Student Number: 12085718 Surname: Fuh Neba Other Names: Terence Course for which acceptable: MSc Primate Conservation Date of Submission: 13th September 2013 This dissertation is submitted in part fulfilment of the regulations for an MSc degree. Oxford Brookes University ii Statement of originality Except for those parts in which it is explicitly stated to the contrary, this project is my own work. It has not been submitted for any degree at this or any other academic or professional institution. ……………………………………………. ………………… Signature Date Regulations Governing the Deposit and Use of Oxford Brookes University Projects/ Dissertations 1. The “top” copies of projects/dissertations submitted in fulfilment of programme requirements shall normally be kept by the School. 2. The author shall sign a declaration agreeing that the project/ dissertation be available for reading and photocopying at the discretion of the Head of School in accordance with 3 and 4 below. 3. The Head of School shall safeguard the interests of the author by requiring persons who consult the project/dissertation to sign a declaration acknowledging the author’s copyright. 4. Permission for any one other then the author to reproduce or photocopy any part of the dissertation must be obtained from the Head of School who will give his/her permission for such reproduction on to an extent which he/she considers to be fair and reasonable.
    [Show full text]
  • Bonobo (Pan Paniscus)
    Bonobo (Pan paniscus) Conservation Strategy 2012–2022 About IUCN IUCN, International Union for Conservation of Nature, helps the world find pragmatic solutions to our most pressing environment and development challenges. IUCN’s work focuses on valuing and conserving nature, ensuring effective and equitable governance of its use, and deploying nature- based solutions to global challenges in climate, food and development. IUCN supports scientific research, manages field projects all over the world, and brings governments, NGOs, the UN and companies together to develop policy, laws and best practice. IUCN is the world’s oldest and largest global environmental organization, with more than 1,200 government and NGO Members and almost 11,000 volunteer experts in some 160 countries. IUCN’s work is supported by over 1,000 staff in 45 offices and hundreds of partners in public, NGO and private sectors around the world. IUCN Species Survival Commission The Species Survival Commission (SSC) is the largest of IUCN’s six volunteer commissions with a global membership of 8,000 experts. SSC advises IUCN and its members on the wide range of technical and scientific aspects of species conservation and is dedicated to securing a future for biodiversity. SSC has significant input into the international agreements dealing with biodiversity conservation. www.iucn.org/themes/ssc IUCN Species Programme The IUCN Species Programme supports the activities of the IUCN Species Survival Commission and individual Specialist Groups, as well as implementing global species conservation initiatives. It is an integral part of the IUCN Secretariat and is managed from IUCN’s international headquarters in Gland, Switzerland.
    [Show full text]
  • Does the Bonobo Have a (Chimpanzee-Like) Theory of Mind?
    Chapter 6 Does the bonobo have a (chimpanzee-like) theory of mind? Christopher Krupenye, Evan L. MacLean and Brian Hare Abstract Theory of mind—the ability to reason about the thoughts and emotions of others—is central to what makes us hu- man. Chimpanzees too appear to understand some psychological states. While less is known about bonobos, several lines of evi- dence suggest that the social-cognitive abilities of the two sister taxa may differ in key respects. This chapter outlines a framework to guide future research on bonobo social cognition based on the predictions of two potentially complementary hypotheses. The self-domestication hypothesis suggests that selection against aggression and for prosociality in bonobos may have impacted the on- togeny of their social-cognitive skills relative to chimpanzees. The empathizing–systemizing hypothesis links degree of prenatal brain masculinization, a potential result of self-domestication, to adult cognition. Specifically, relative feminization may yield more flexible theory of mind skills in bonobos than chimpanzees. Finally, directions for future study, including development of new paradigms that maximize ecological validity for bonobos, are discussed. La théorie de l’esprit—le pouvoir de raisonner les pensées et émotions des autres—est centrale à notre nature humaine. Il parait que les chimpanzés peuvent comprendre quelques états psychologiques. Tandis que nous savons moins des bonobos, plusieurs té- moignages suggèrent que les capacités socio-­cognitives des deux taxons soeur peuvent différer dans des aspects clefs. Nous traçons un cadre pour guider les prochaines recherches sur la cognition sociale des bonobos, basé sur les prédictions de deux hypothèses potentiellement complémentaires.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparing the Imitative Skills of Children and Nonhuman Apes
    Revue de primatologie Numéro 1 (2009) Varia ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... Malinda Carpenter et Josep Call Comparing the imitative skills of children and nonhuman apes ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... Avertissement Le contenu de ce site relève de la législation française sur la propriété intellectuelle et est la propriété exclusive de l'éditeur. Les œuvres figurant sur ce site peuvent être consultées et reproduites sur un support papier ou numérique sous réserve qu'elles soient strictement réservées à un usage soit personnel, soit scientifique ou pédagogique excluant toute exploitation commerciale. La reproduction devra obligatoirement mentionner l'éditeur, le nom de la revue, l'auteur et la référence du document. Toute autre reproduction est interdite sauf accord préalable de l'éditeur, en dehors des cas prévus par la législation en vigueur en France. Revues.org est un portail de revues en sciences humaines et sociales développé par le CLEO, Centre pour l'édition électronique ouverte (CNRS, EHESS, UP, UAPV). ..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Innovation in Wild Chimpanzees {Pan Troglodytes)
    International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 16, No. 1, 1995 Innovation in Wild Chimpanzees {Pan troglodytes) Christophe Boesch1 Received April 27, 1993; revised November 23, 1993; accepted December 7, 1993 Innovations of behavior have major implications for the concept of culture in animals. Innovation has rarely been documented in wild animal populations. The chimpanzees of the Tai National Park, Cote d'lvoire, spontaneously included new patterns of behavior in their repertoire during our study. Some innovations were incorporated into a minority of the group members' repertoire, whereas others, such as building day nests in trees and on the ground more frequently, became general behaviors. Similarly, new contexts for use of leaf-clipping behavior and novel ways of feeding on some leaves appeared and rapidly became part of the behavior of most group members. The environmental parameters were too stable to explain these new forms of behavior, as most of them took place within 1 month during the same dry season. In a similar way, leaf-grooming acquired a new function in Gombe chimpanzees. A process similar to social conventions could explain the emergence of these new functions for an existing behavior in a way rather similar to human "fashion." Three major characteristics of human culture — the absence of individual variations in the performance of the behavior, the general use of the behavior by group members, and the ability to modify the function of a behavior — characterize leaf-clipping in Tai chimpanzees and leaf-grooming in Gombe chimpanzees. KEY WORDS: chimpanzee; culture; innovation; tool-use; cross-population comparisons. INTRODUCTION Innovation is the introduction of a new behavior in the repertoire of one or more individuals that may be, for example, a solution to a new ¹Institute of Zoology, University of Basel, Basel Switzerland.
    [Show full text]