The Roles of Traditional Knowledge Systems in Orang-Utan Pongo Spp

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Roles of Traditional Knowledge Systems in Orang-Utan Pongo Spp The roles of traditional knowledge systems in orang-utan Pongo spp. and forest conservation: a case study of Danau Sentarum, West Kalimantan, Indonesia E LIZABETH L. YULIANI,HASANTOHA A DNAN,RAMADHANI A CHDIAWAN D ENNY B AKARA,VALENTINUS H ERI,JIM S AMMY M. AGUS S ALIM and T ERRY S UNDERLAND Abstract The orang-utan Pongo spp. is protected by nation- Introduction al and international legislation, yet populations continue to decline. Many reports implicate local people in the poaching he orang-utan Pongo spp. is the only remaining Asian and illegal trade in orang-utans, yet community participa- Tgreat ape and is found only on the islands of Sumatra tion has been promoted as an alternative conservation and Borneo (Rijksen & Meijaard, ; Singleton et al., strategy. To explore how community-based orang-utan ). Both species, the Sumatran orang-utan Pongo abelii conservation could be developed, we conducted a study to and the Bornean orang-utan Pongo pygmaeus, are categor- understand informal institutions, particularly local people’s ized as Critically Endangered on the IUCN Red List perceptions, traditional beliefs, taboos, norms and knowl- (Singleton et al., ; Ancrenaz et al., ). There are es- edge, related to orang-utan conservation within and around timated to be c. , Sumatran orang-utans in the wild, and – the wetlands of Danau Sentarum. The majority of Dayak , , north-west Bornean orang-utans P. pygmaeus communities interviewed practised traditional taboos, pygmaeus, , south-west Bornean orang-utans P. pyg- which supported the protection of orang-utans and their maeus wurmbii and , north-east Bornean orang-utans habitat. Statistical analysis using generalized linear model- P. pygmaeus morio (Wich et al., ). ling indicated that more orang-utan nests were found in In Indonesia the orang-utan is protected by national le- areas with both good habitat condition and strong informal gislation but its habitat is not, unless it is within protected institutions. Despite applying traditional systems that are areas. Orang-utan populations continue to decline, mainly similar to conservation, local people have negative percep- as a result of habitat loss for agriculture, plantations and tions about the term ‘conservation’. We describe the under- mining, as well as hunting, killing and trading. Although lying causes of these negative perceptions and highlight policies for protection are in place, enforcement is weak their implications for conservation programmes and pol- and inconsistent (e.g. Rijksen & Meijaard, ; Robertson icies. We conclude that conservation of orang-utans and & van Schaik, ; Nijman, ). The importance of pro- other species should not focus on single species but on tecting orang-utans (and other key species) is not reflected maintaining social and natural capital, cultural diversity in government programmes or land-use policies. National and ecological functions at various institutional levels and and district land use planning prioritizes large-scale indus- across geographical scales. trial plantations and mining, with little (if any) protection of orang-utan habitat outside protected areas by either the Keywords Biocultural diversity, customary beliefs, Danau National Planning Agency or the agriculture and mining Sentarum, forest, governance and institutions, local people, sectors. Thus, orang-utans are regarded as pests when orang-utan, taboo they are present in commercial plantations, and hundreds have been killed (e.g. Tribun Kaltim, ; Gayle, ). However, it is rare that the perpetrators are prosecuted (Antara Kaltim, ). This shows that formal institutions ELIZABETH L. YULIANI* (Corresponding author), HASANTOHA ADNAN,RAMADHANI are weak, infractions are often ignored and sanctions are ACHDIAWAN,M.AGUS SALIM and TERRY SUNDERLAND† Center for International Forestry Research, Sindang Barang, Bogor, Indonesia insignificant. DENNY BAKARA,VALENTINUS HERI and JIM SAMMY Riak Bumi Foundation, Local people have been implicated in the hunting, poach- Pontianak, Indonesia ing and illegal trade of orang-utans (Rijksen & Meijaard, *Also at: Radboud University of Nijmegen, Huygens Building, Heyendaalseweg ; Nijman, ). Whether or not they hunt may be re- 135, 6525AJ Nijmegen, The Netherlands. E-mail [email protected] † lated to their ethnicity and religion (Sugardjito & van Also at: College of Marine and Environmental Sciences, James Cook University, Cairns, Australia Schaik, ). An alternative discourse frames local commu- Received February . Revision requested March . nities as a potential solution. Their informal institutions, in- Accepted June . First published online November . cluding nature-related social taboos, customary beliefs and This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence Downloaded from(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), https://www.cambridge.org/core. IP address: which permits 170.106.34.90 unrestricted, on 25 re-use, Sep distribution,2021 at 19:55:06 and reproduction, subject to the in any Cambridge medium, Core provided terms th eof original use, available work is at properly cited. https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0030605316000636 Oryx, 2018, 52(1), 156–165 © 2016 Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S0030605316000636 Traditional knowledge in conservation 157 traditional knowledge systems, are an important compo- nent of their social capital and have important applications for biodiversity conservation (Gadgil et al., ; Colding & Folke, ; Wadley & Colfer, ; Berkes, ; Luo et al., ; Parotta, ). However, informal institutions have been largely neglected in conservation planning in biodiversity-rich, developing countries (Alcorn ; Robbins ). To improve the protection of great apes, in- cluding orang-utans, efforts focusing on the preservation of traditional values and long-term support for community in- itiatives have been recommended (Caldecott, ). Studies have shown positive correlations between trad- itional knowledge and biodiversity conservation (Nyhus et al., ; Riley, ; Etiendem et al., ; Stacey et al., ), and highlighted the need for location- and species- specific understanding of traditional belief systems in asses- sing their potential application to conservation efforts FIG. 1 Danau Sentarum National Park and its surrounding areas, (Uyeda et al., ). However, there is a lack of such studies West Kalimantan, Indonesia. Source of spatial data: Margono specifically related to orang-utan protection. We aim to fill et al. () and Hansen et al. (). this knowledge gap by () describing perceptions and infor- mal institutions of the major ethnic groups around Danau Sentarum wetlands, including traditional beliefs, taboos, designated a National Park. The Dayak live in the surround- norms, knowledge and practices, which are related to orang- ing hills, and their main income is from fish, rubber, pepper, utan and broader forest conservation, and () evaluating and paid labour in Malaysia. Subsistence rice production is how these informal institutions could contribute to improv- an important part of their livelihoods (Eilenberg & Wadley, ing the protection of orang-utans and their habitat. We ). The Malay live on the riversides, and their main in- focus on nature-related social taboos as a subset of informal come comes from fish, honey and rubber extraction (Yuliani institutions (Colding & Folke, ), and adopt the defin- & Erman, ; Indriatmoko, ). Some Malay villages in- ition of taboo as ‘a prohibition against touching, saying or side the Park were initially seasonal settlements of fishers doing something for fear of immediate harm from a super- who came from the Kapuas river to fish in the wetlands in natural force; or a prohibition imposed by social custom or the s and s, but some are several hundred years old as a protective measure’ (Merriam-Webster, ). (Giesen, ). All have strong ties with larger Malay towns along the Kapuas river (R.L. Wadley, unpubl. data). Study area Methods The Danau Sentarum wetlands are located in West To explore customary beliefs and institutions related to Kalimantan, km north-east of Pontianak, the provincial orang-utans we conducted in-depth semi-structured interviews capital city (Fig. ). The wetlands comprise intercon- and gender-disaggregated focus group discussions ( with nected seasonal lakes, hill forests, lowland dipterocarp for- Dayaks and nine with Malays) and undertook participant ob- ests, peat swamp and swamp forests, and dwarf forests servation from November until February .Wese- (Giesen & Aglionby, ). In the core area of , lected interview and discussion group participants based on km was gazetted as a National Park by the Indonesian gov- the following criteria: () they possessed good knowledge of cul- ernment. The Park has at least fish species (Kottelat & ture, traditional norms and belief systems, and histories of the Widjanarti, ), reptile and mammal species community or the hamlet specifically related to orang-utans; (Meijaard & Jeanes, ), and bird species (van () they had good knowledge of formal regulations (or indivi- Balen & Dennis, ). In it was home to an estimated duals that represent formal institutions); ()theyrepresenteda , orang-utans (Russon et al., ). However, the wet- diversity of ethnic groups and points of view; and ()theywere lands’ catchment areas were subjected to illegal logging dur- willing to participate in our study. We first consulted the chief ing – and the development of large-scale oil palm of the hamlet
Recommended publications
  • Recent Developments in the Study of Wild Chimpanzee Behavior
    Evolutionary Anthropology 9 ARTICLES Recent Developments in the Study of Wild Chimpanzee Behavior JOHN C. MITANI, DAVID P. WATTS, AND MARTIN N. MULLER Chimpanzees have always been of special interest to anthropologists. As our organization, genetics and behavior, closest living relatives,1–3 they provide the standard against which to assess hunting and meat-eating, inter-group human uniqueness and information regarding the changes that must have oc- relationships, and behavioral endocri- curred during the course of human evolution. Given these circumstances, it is not nology. Our treatment is selective, and surprising that chimpanzees have been studied intensively in the wild. Jane Good- we explicitly avoid comment on inter- all4,5 initiated the first long-term field study of chimpanzee behavior at the Gombe population variation in behavior as it National Park, Tanzania. Her observations of tool manufacture and use, hunting, relates to the question of chimpanzee and meat-eating forever changed the way we define humans. Field research on cultures. Excellent reviews of this chimpanzee behavior by Toshisada Nishida and colleagues6 at the nearby Mahale topic, of central concern to anthropol- Mountains National Park has had an equally significant impact. It was Nishida7,8 ogists, can be found elsewhere.12–14 who first provided a comprehensive picture of the chimpanzee social system, including group structure and dispersal. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION No single issue in the study of wild Two generations of researchers erything about the behavior of these chimpanzee behavior has seen more have followed Goodall and Nishida apes in nature. But this is not the case. debate than the nature of their social into the field.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Representation of Magnitude in Gorillas and Orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes A,B, , Rachel F.L
    Cognition 168 (2017) 312–319 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Original Articles Spatial representation of magnitude in gorillas and orangutans ⇑ Regina Paxton Gazes a,b, , Rachel F.L. Diamond c,g, Jasmine M. Hope d, Damien Caillaud e,f, Tara S. Stoinski a,e, Robert R. Hampton c,g a Zoo Atlanta, Atlanta, GA, United States b Bucknell University, Lewisburg, PA, United States c Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States d Neuroscience Graduate Program, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States e Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund International, Atlanta, GA, United States f Department of Anthropology, University of California Davis, Davis, CA, United States g Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, GA, United States article info abstract Article history: Humans mentally represent magnitudes spatially; we respond faster to one side of space when process- Received 1 March 2017 ing small quantities and to the other side of space when processing large quantities. We determined Revised 24 July 2017 whether spatial representation of magnitude is a fundamental feature of primate cognition by testing Accepted 25 July 2017 for such space-magnitude correspondence in gorillas and orangutans. Subjects picked the larger quantity in a pair of dot arrays in one condition, and the smaller in another. Response latencies to the left and right sides of the screen were compared across the magnitude range. Apes showed evidence of spatial repre- Keywords: sentation of magnitude. While all subjects did not adopt the same orientation, apes showed consistent Space tendencies for spatial representations within individuals and systematically reversed these orientations SNARC Ape in response to reversal of the task instruction.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ledger and Times, April 16, 1953
    Murray State's Digital Commons The Ledger & Times Newspapers 4-16-1953 The Ledger and Times, April 16, 1953 The Ledger and Times Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.murraystate.edu/tlt Recommended Citation The Ledger and Times, "The Ledger and Times, April 16, 1953" (1953). The Ledger & Times. 1272. https://digitalcommons.murraystate.edu/tlt/1272 This Newspaper is brought to you for free and open access by the Newspapers at Murray State's Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in The Ledger & Times by an authorized administrator of Murray State's Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. :g hi A .0.• ..•••••• Aks AWL It. 11,f5N Selected As Best All sound Kentucky Community Newspaper for 1947 We Weather Are KN^ETUCKV: Fair with -ir -- terrepersteoires-neetr -Or a little Helping To 1411NNsorst• below freezing tomeht. low 30 to 34 in the east and a Build Murray , • `1,:*"\ ‘7441"--"7- 32 to 38 in the east portion. Friday fair and continued Each Day cool. r• YOUR PROGRESSIVE HOME NEWSPAPER United Press IN ITS 74th YEAR Murray, Ky., Thursday Afternioon, April 16, 1953 MURRAY POPULATION . - - 8,000 Vol. XXIX; No. 91 Vitality Dress Shoes IKE CHALLENGES RE!)s IN PEACE MOVE Basque, Red Calf and , als; In Flight Blue Calf Soon de Hoard I Now.4)':,t7".. Lions Will Be Six Point Program Listed $10.95 By LEIO, PANMUNJOM, ,./iApril 16 Sold To Aid By President To End War Al'OUttd • / (UP)-Red trucks b. /ambulances today delivered the first of 805 By MERRIMAN SMITH ,hopes with mere words and prom- I Allied sick and wounded prisoners Health Center WASHINGTON April 16 iUPI- ises and gestures," he said.
    [Show full text]
  • Orangutan Positional Behavior and the Nature of Arboreal Locomotion in Hominoidea Susannah K.S
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 000:000–000 (2006) Orangutan Positional Behavior and the Nature of Arboreal Locomotion in Hominoidea Susannah K.S. Thorpe1* and Robin H. Crompton2 1School of Biosciences, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK 2Department of Human Anatomy and Cell Biology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 3GE, UK KEY WORDS Pongo pygmaeus; posture; orthograde clamber; forelimb suspend ABSTRACT The Asian apes, more than any other, are and orthograde compressive locomotor modes are ob- restricted to an arboreal habitat. They are consequently an served more frequently. Given the complexity of orangu- important model in the interpretation of the morphological tan positional behavior demonstrated by this study, it is commonalities of the apes, which are locomotor features likely that differences in positional behavior between associated with arboreal living. This paper presents a de- studies reflect differences in the interplay between the tailed analysis of orangutan positional behavior for all age- complex array of variables, which were shown to influence sex categories and during a complete range of behavioral orangutan positional behavior (Thorpe and Crompton [2005] contexts, following standardized positional mode descrip- Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127:58–78). With the exception tions proposed by Hunt et al. ([1996] Primates 37:363–387). of pronograde suspensory posture and locomotion, orang- This paper shows that orangutan positional behavior is utan positional behavior is similar to that of the African highly complex, representing a diverse spectrum of posi- apes, and in particular, lowland gorillas. This study sug- tional modes. Overall, all orthograde and pronograde sus- gests that it is orthogrady in general, rather than fore- pensory postures are exhibited less frequently in the pres- limb suspend specifically, that characterizes the posi- ent study than previously reported.
    [Show full text]
  • Western Lowland Gorilla (Gorilla Gorilla Gorilla) Diet and Activity Budgets: Effects of Group Size, Age Class and Food Availability in the Dzanga-Ndoki
    Western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) diet and activity budgets: effects of group size, age class and food availability in the Dzanga-Ndoki National Park, Central African Republic Terence Fuh MSc Primate Conservation Dissertation 2013 Dissertation Course Name: P20107 Title: Western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) diet and activity budgets: effects of group size, age class and food availability in the Dzanga-Ndoki National Park, Central African Republic Student Number: 12085718 Surname: Fuh Neba Other Names: Terence Course for which acceptable: MSc Primate Conservation Date of Submission: 13th September 2013 This dissertation is submitted in part fulfilment of the regulations for an MSc degree. Oxford Brookes University ii Statement of originality Except for those parts in which it is explicitly stated to the contrary, this project is my own work. It has not been submitted for any degree at this or any other academic or professional institution. ……………………………………………. ………………… Signature Date Regulations Governing the Deposit and Use of Oxford Brookes University Projects/ Dissertations 1. The “top” copies of projects/dissertations submitted in fulfilment of programme requirements shall normally be kept by the School. 2. The author shall sign a declaration agreeing that the project/ dissertation be available for reading and photocopying at the discretion of the Head of School in accordance with 3 and 4 below. 3. The Head of School shall safeguard the interests of the author by requiring persons who consult the project/dissertation to sign a declaration acknowledging the author’s copyright. 4. Permission for any one other then the author to reproduce or photocopy any part of the dissertation must be obtained from the Head of School who will give his/her permission for such reproduction on to an extent which he/she considers to be fair and reasonable.
    [Show full text]
  • Does the Bonobo Have a (Chimpanzee-Like) Theory of Mind?
    Chapter 6 Does the bonobo have a (chimpanzee-like) theory of mind? Christopher Krupenye, Evan L. MacLean and Brian Hare Abstract Theory of mind—the ability to reason about the thoughts and emotions of others—is central to what makes us hu- man. Chimpanzees too appear to understand some psychological states. While less is known about bonobos, several lines of evi- dence suggest that the social-cognitive abilities of the two sister taxa may differ in key respects. This chapter outlines a framework to guide future research on bonobo social cognition based on the predictions of two potentially complementary hypotheses. The self-domestication hypothesis suggests that selection against aggression and for prosociality in bonobos may have impacted the on- togeny of their social-cognitive skills relative to chimpanzees. The empathizing–systemizing hypothesis links degree of prenatal brain masculinization, a potential result of self-domestication, to adult cognition. Specifically, relative feminization may yield more flexible theory of mind skills in bonobos than chimpanzees. Finally, directions for future study, including development of new paradigms that maximize ecological validity for bonobos, are discussed. La théorie de l’esprit—le pouvoir de raisonner les pensées et émotions des autres—est centrale à notre nature humaine. Il parait que les chimpanzés peuvent comprendre quelques états psychologiques. Tandis que nous savons moins des bonobos, plusieurs té- moignages suggèrent que les capacités socio-­cognitives des deux taxons soeur peuvent différer dans des aspects clefs. Nous traçons un cadre pour guider les prochaines recherches sur la cognition sociale des bonobos, basé sur les prédictions de deux hypothèses potentiellement complémentaires.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparing the Imitative Skills of Children and Nonhuman Apes
    Revue de primatologie Numéro 1 (2009) Varia ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... Malinda Carpenter et Josep Call Comparing the imitative skills of children and nonhuman apes ............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................... Avertissement Le contenu de ce site relève de la législation française sur la propriété intellectuelle et est la propriété exclusive de l'éditeur. Les œuvres figurant sur ce site peuvent être consultées et reproduites sur un support papier ou numérique sous réserve qu'elles soient strictement réservées à un usage soit personnel, soit scientifique ou pédagogique excluant toute exploitation commerciale. La reproduction devra obligatoirement mentionner l'éditeur, le nom de la revue, l'auteur et la référence du document. Toute autre reproduction est interdite sauf accord préalable de l'éditeur, en dehors des cas prévus par la législation en vigueur en France. Revues.org est un portail de revues en sciences humaines et sociales développé par le CLEO, Centre pour l'édition électronique ouverte (CNRS, EHESS, UP, UAPV). ..............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Innovation in Wild Chimpanzees {Pan Troglodytes)
    International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 16, No. 1, 1995 Innovation in Wild Chimpanzees {Pan troglodytes) Christophe Boesch1 Received April 27, 1993; revised November 23, 1993; accepted December 7, 1993 Innovations of behavior have major implications for the concept of culture in animals. Innovation has rarely been documented in wild animal populations. The chimpanzees of the Tai National Park, Cote d'lvoire, spontaneously included new patterns of behavior in their repertoire during our study. Some innovations were incorporated into a minority of the group members' repertoire, whereas others, such as building day nests in trees and on the ground more frequently, became general behaviors. Similarly, new contexts for use of leaf-clipping behavior and novel ways of feeding on some leaves appeared and rapidly became part of the behavior of most group members. The environmental parameters were too stable to explain these new forms of behavior, as most of them took place within 1 month during the same dry season. In a similar way, leaf-grooming acquired a new function in Gombe chimpanzees. A process similar to social conventions could explain the emergence of these new functions for an existing behavior in a way rather similar to human "fashion." Three major characteristics of human culture — the absence of individual variations in the performance of the behavior, the general use of the behavior by group members, and the ability to modify the function of a behavior — characterize leaf-clipping in Tai chimpanzees and leaf-grooming in Gombe chimpanzees. KEY WORDS: chimpanzee; culture; innovation; tool-use; cross-population comparisons. INTRODUCTION Innovation is the introduction of a new behavior in the repertoire of one or more individuals that may be, for example, a solution to a new ¹Institute of Zoology, University of Basel, Basel Switzerland.
    [Show full text]
  • Bonobo Food Items, Food Availability and Bonobo Distribution in the Lake
    14 The Open Conservation Biology Journal, 2009, 3, 14-23 Open Access Bonobo Food Items, Food Availability and Bonobo Distribution in the Lake Tumba Swampy Forests, Democratic Republic of Congo Bila-Isia Inogwabini*,1,2 and Bewa Matungila2 1Durrell Institute for Conservation and Ecology, University of Kent, UK; 2World Wide Fund for Nature, DRC Program Abstract: Data on food items were collected from the Lac Tumba Swampy Forests (LTSF) with the objective to gauge the effect of food type and availability on the distribution of the bonobos. Bonobos at the LTSF feed on at least 61 plant species and eat more Terrestrial Herbaceous Vegetation (THV) than at other sites (t = 0.676, df = 3, p = 0.548 > 0.05; in- significant). Fruits were available for most of the year at sites within the LTSF. At the Mbou-Mon-Tour (MMT), a site with higher bonobo density, the mean density of 1.42 fruits/m2 per month (range 0.62 – 3.82 fruits/m2 per month) was re- corded, higher than in other sites where bonobos occur (general univariate linear model = 0.422, t = 1.543, df = 11, p = 0.151, non-significant). In-site differences between MMT and other sub-sites in the LTSF were significant (t = 2.793, df = 12, p = 0.016 < 0.05). Fruit abundance in the LTSF ( X =138 fruits/km, SD = 13.80) was higher than in the Salonga Na- tional Park (SNP) ( X =83 fruits/km, SD = 6.49). There were five species of THV in the LTSF, with the most abundant being Megaphrynium macrostachyum (41.18%), which was scarce in the Lomako Forests.
    [Show full text]
  • The Animal Welfare Act Is Lacking: How to Update the Federal Statute to Improve Zoo Animal Welfare
    Golden Gate University Environmental Law Journal Volume 12 Issue 1 Article 4 July 2020 THE ANIMAL WELFARE ACT IS LACKING: HOW TO UPDATE THE FEDERAL STATUTE TO IMPROVE ZOO ANIMAL WELFARE Rebecca L. Jodidio Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.law.ggu.edu/gguelj Part of the Animal Law Commons, and the Environmental Law Commons Recommended Citation Rebecca L. Jodidio, THE ANIMAL WELFARE ACT IS LACKING: HOW TO UPDATE THE FEDERAL STATUTE TO IMPROVE ZOO ANIMAL WELFARE, 12 Golden Gate U. Envtl. L.J. 53 (2020). https://digitalcommons.law.ggu.edu/gguelj/vol12/iss1/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Academic Journals at GGU Law Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Golden Gate University Environmental Law Journal by an authorized editor of GGU Law Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Jodidio: The Animal Welfare Act is Lacking THE ANIMAL WELFARE ACT IS LACKING: HOW TO UPDATE THE FEDERAL STATUTE TO IMPROVE ZOO ANIMAL WELFARE REBECCA L. JODIDIO1 I. INTRODUCTION Visiting the zoo is a beloved national pastime — American zoos attract 183 million people annually.2 For many Americans, zoos provide the first, and sometimes only, opportunity for individuals to be in the presence of animals outside of domesticated cats and dogs. However, for the animals themselves, zoos can cause suffering. Two philosophies support the protection of wild animals in captiv- ity: an anthropocentric and ecocentric view. According to the former, anthropocentric view, wild animals hold an extrinsic value and when they cease to be valuable to humans, or conflict with our other values, their interests can be sacrificed.3 The latter, ecocentric view, holds that wild animals have intrinsic value, can be morally harmed, and how we treat them should not be judged solely by the benefit to humans of a 1 Rebecca L.
    [Show full text]
  • Year 5 History Week Beginning 4Th May L.O.: I Can Learn
    YEAR 5 HISTORY WEEK BEGINNING 4TH MAY L.O.: I CAN LEARN ABOUT ANIMALS IN SPACE AND DEBATE WHETHER THIS IS ACCEPTABLE. As we learnt last week, there was a great Space Race between USA and USSR. Both wanted to be the best at Space travel and exploration! We learnt about the humans who went in to Space, but animals have played a very important part too… Both countries were using experimental technology that was extremely dangerous and a lot was at stake. Whoever got there first, without casualties, would be the better country - and the whole world was watching. In the 1940s and 50s, Space flight was still too dangerous to risk an astronaut’s life… so animals were sent instead. The USA sent fruit flies into space in 1947 so they were technically the first animals in space. In 1949 however, a rhesus monkeys called Albert 1 reached 30– 39 miles (48–63 km) altitude (height above Earth). Another monkey, Albert II, reached about 83 miles (134 km) on a different space mission. Albert II died on impact after a parachute failure. The USSR decided to send the spacecraft 'Sputnik' into space with a stray dog called Laika as it's passenger. By using a dog, their scientists could see the effects of zero-gravity on a living animal. They believed that if the mission was unsuccessful, then the loss of a dog's life wouldn't be as bad as a lost human. On November 3rd 1957, Sputnik 2 was launched into space with Laika. She had gone from being a stray dog in Moscow to being an animal astronaut pioneer.
    [Show full text]
  • The History of the Animal Care Program at NASA Johnson Space
    The Animal Care Program • Johnson Space Center is committed to the ethical and humane care and use of animals and each person must share responsibility in meeting that commitment. • Our role in the care and use of animals carries the responsibility to effectively meet the needs of the animals in our charge while actively seeking and employing methods that embody respect for the life of animals. - JSC Animal Care & Use Handbook, JSC29259 STS-3 Insect Flight Motion Study December 1959 Wallops Island, VA Rhesus Monkey “SAM” MDS – Mice Drawer System • Study bone loss – long duration • ACP provided training on animal handling and euthanasia • 100- 150 days housing Actual Animals in Space… Before humans actually went into space, one of the prevailing theories of the perils of space flight was that humans might not be able to survive long periods of weightlessness. American and Russian scientists utilized animals - mainly monkeys, chimps and dogs - in order to test each country's ability to launch a living animal into space and bring it back alive and unharmed. Dog: Laika in Sputnik II Scientists want to know how the body reacts to microgravity. Many experiments can be conducted on humans while they work aboard the Space Shuttle or International Space Station (ISS), but many others interfere with daily activities; that's where the animals come into play. And while scientists may not really care how a rat reacts to space conditions, animal data can Chimpanzee: Enose transfer to human models and help prevent or Mercury Atlas 5 solve physical issues people face today. Animals go into space to help conduct scientific research only when absolutely necessary.
    [Show full text]