Genomic Imprinting Mediates Sexual Experience-Dependent Olfactory Learning in Male Mice

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Genomic Imprinting Mediates Sexual Experience-Dependent Olfactory Learning in Male Mice Genomic imprinting mediates sexual experience-dependent olfactory learning in male mice William T. Swaney*†‡, James P. Curley*, Frances A. Champagne*†, and Eric B. Keverne* *Sub-Department of Animal Behaviour, University of Cambridge, High Street, Madingley, Cambridge CB3 8AA, United Kingdom; and †Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027 Edited by Donald W. Pfaff, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY, and approved February 21, 2007 (received for review October 25, 2006) Mammalian imprinted genes are generally thought to have mother results in a complementary phenotype affecting pup evolved as a result of conflict between parents; however, recent growth, pup suckling/maternal milk letdown, and pup thermoreg- knockout studies suggest that coadaptation between mother and ulation/maternal nest-building (11), showing that these interdepen- offspring may have been a significant factor. We present evidence dent behaviors are coregulated by the same imprinted locus. Peg3 that the same imprinted gene that regulates mammalian maternal is strongly expressed in both the developing embryo and the care and offspring development also regulates male sexual behav- hypothalamus (9, 12), which controls maternal care in the mouse; ior and olfaction. We have shown that the behavior of male mice in light of these findings, it has been proposed that coadaptation carrying a knockout of the imprinted gene Peg3 does not change between mother and offspring may also have been a factor in the with sexual experience and that the mice are consequently unable evolution of imprinting in mammals. to improve their copulatory abilities or olfactory interest in female For a paternally expressed gene to be transmitted down the odor cues after mating experience. Forebrain activation, as indexed patriline and become fixed in the population, it would be antici- pated to also have adaptive effects on male behavior, enhancing by female odor-induced c-Fos protein induction, fails to increase male survival or reproductive success, and so we examined three with sexual experience, providing a neural basis for the behavioral different aspects of male behavior: sexual behavior, olfaction, and deficits that the male mice display. The behavioral and neural sexual experience-dependent changes in behavior in Peg3 knockout effects of the Peg3 knockout show that this imprinted gene has (KO) animals. Olfaction is the most important sensory faculty in evolved to regulate multiple and varied aspects of reproduction, mice, and female chemosignals detected by the main and accessory from male sexual behavior to female maternal care, and the olfactory systems play a vital role in triggering sexual behavior in development of offspring. Moreover, sexual experience-driven male mice, as studies involving ablation of the olfactory systems (13) behavioral changes may represent an adaptive response that or KO of olfactory genes (14, 15) have shown. As well as regulating enables males to increase their reproductive potential over their male sexual behavior, the hypothalamus receives input from the lifespan, and the effects we have found suggest that the evolu- accessory olfactory system (16), and so the behavioral and neuronal tion of genomic imprinting has been influenced by coadaptation responses of Peg3-KO males to female odors were also investigated between males and females as well as between females and to determine how the mutation affects reproduction-directed ol- offspring. faction and male sexual behavior. Sexual experience has been shown to have important effects on accessory olfactory system ͉ coadaptation ͉ mammalian brain ͉ reward ͉ male behavior in different rodent species, modulating interest in sexual behavior female semiochemicals (17, 18), mating behavior (19, 20), and increasing male fertility (21). Such experience-dependent changes in behavior are likely to be adaptive, enabling males to enhance he expression of certain mammalian autosomal genes in a their sexual behavior throughout life, and so we tested both virgin Tparent-of-origin fashion was first demonstrated in the 1980s and and sexually experienced males to determine whether sexual expe- termed ‘‘genomic imprinting’’ (1); since then, close to 100 mater- rience plays a role in sexual behavior in male mice and whether it nally and paternally expressed imprinted genes have been identified has similar effects in Peg3-KO mutants and wild-type males. (2). Over the last 20 years, the essential roles that imprinted genes We assessed the neural basis of these behaviors by using c-Fos play in development, particularly that of the brain, have become expression as a marker of brain activation in response to female clear (3, 4), suggesting that imprinted genes might have enabled the urinary chemosignals in wild-type (wt) sexually experienced (wt- evolutionary expansion of the mammalian brain (5). Most im- SE) males, wt virgin (wt-V) males, Peg3-KO sexually experienced printed genes are strongly expressed in the placenta (6) and are (p3-SE) males, and Peg3-KO virgin (p3-V) males. involved in regulating fetal development (7). These developmental roles, in particular those of Igf2, Igf2r, Igf2AS, and H19, have been Results cited as evidence to support the conflict theory for the evolution of Peg3 KO Males’ Interest in Female Urine Is Unaffected by Sexual genomic imprinting (8). This theory posits that imprinted genes Experience. Adult male C57BL/6J (B6) mice were tested on their evolved with placentation in mammals as a result of parental ability to discriminate between male and female urine and their conflict over offspring investment. The fetus/placenta is a geneti- cally half-paternal structure that provides the father with an op- Author contributions: W.T.S., J.P.C., and E.B.K. designed research; W.T.S. performed re- portunity to manipulate the mother’s investment in his offspring to search; W.T.S. analyzed data; and W.T.S., J.P.C., F.A.C., and E.B.K. wrote the paper. the possible detriment of her potential future offspring. Thus, The authors declare no conflict of interest. paternally expressed genes are predicted to enhance offspring growth, whereas maternally expressed genes are predicted to resist This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. this as a result of an evolutionary ‘‘arms race’’ over levels of Abbreviations: AOB, accessory olfactory bulb; B6, C57BL/6J; BNSTp, posterior part of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; KO, knockout; MeAmgA, anterior medial amygdala; investment in offspring. However, work by our group on the MeAmgPD, posterior–dorsal medial amygdala; MPOA, medial preoptic area; NAc, nucleus paternally expressed gene Peg3 has shown that as well as regulating accumbens; p3-SE, sexually experienced Peg3-KO; p3-V, virgin Peg3-KO; VMH, ventrome- pup development, this gene also regulates mothers’ maternal be- dial hypothalamus; wt, wild type; wt-SE, sexually experienced wt; wt-V, virgin wt. havior (9), a finding that is not predicted by the conflict theory (10). ‡To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. Targeted deletion of Peg3 in either pups or independently in the © 2007 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA 6084–6089 ͉ PNAS ͉ April 3, 2007 ͉ vol. 104 ͉ no. 14 www.pnas.org͞cgi͞doi͞10.1073͞pnas.0609471104 Downloaded by guest on September 24, 2021 Fig. 2. Sexual behavior of wt-SE (n ϭ 16), p3-SE (n ϭ 10), wt-V (n ϭ 14), and p3-V (n ϭ 10) males in 1-h sexual behavior tests. (a) Mean latencies to mount females. wt-SE males mounted females faster than all other groups. (b and c) Frequency of mounting (b) and intromission (c) during tests. wt-SE males mounted and intromitted more frequently than all other groups. Compari- sons are with wt-SE males (** indicate P Ͻ 0.01; *** indicate P Ͻ 0.001; error bars indicate SEM). Fig. 1. Mean time spent sniffing water, male urine, and female urine by greater preference for female urine than wt-V males (P ϭ 0.004), ϭ ϭ ϭ ϭ wt-SE (n 7), p3-SE (n 7), wt-V (n 6), and p3-V (n 7) males in olfactory p3-SE males (P ϭ 0.040), and p3-V males (P ϭ 0.009), but no other tests. (a and b) Habituation–dishabituation tests. All groups sniffed the first urine stimulus more than the final water stimulus, and wt-SE, wt-V, and p3-V group differences were found. males increased their olfactory investigations when the urine types were switched. (c) Urine preference tests. Significance is represented by an asterisk Sexual Behavior Is Enhanced by Sexual Experience in wt Males but Not for comparison of interest in male and female urine and by a dagger for in Peg3-KO Males. To assess whether the Peg3-KO had effects on comparison with the preference of wt-SE males. All groups spent longer male mating behavior similar to those seen on male olfactory sniffing female urine than male urine, but wt-SE males had a significantly behavior, we tested the sexual behavior of wt-SE, wt-V, p3-SE, and greater preference for female urine than all other groups. (* and † indicate P Ͻ p3-V males. Males were paired with receptive virgin B6 females; 0.05; **, ##, and †† indicate P Ͻ 0.01; *** and ### indicate P Ͻ 0.001; and error their sexual behavior over the following hour was recorded and bars indicate SEM.) analyzed. Significantly more wt-SE males than p3-SE males mounted (P ϭ 0.018), intromitted (P Ͻ 0.001), and ejaculated (P ϭ preference for female urine over male urine in olfactory tests in 0.001) during testing; however, no other group differences in the which they could physically contact the urine and so use both the proportion of males performing sexual behavior were found. La- accessory and main olfactory systems. Habituation–dishabituation tency data were analyzed for males that performed each behavior tests were used to measure the ability of subject males to discrim- measured, but because of the very low incidence of intromission and ejaculation among p3-SE males, it was only possible to analyze inate between urine and water and between male and female urine.
Recommended publications
  • Chapter 51 Animal Behavior
    Chapter 51 Animal Behavior Lecture Outline Overview: Shall We Dance? • Red-crowned cranes (Grus japonensis) gather in groups to dance, prance, stretch, bow, and leap. They grab bits of plants, sticks, and feathers with their bills and toss them into the air. • How does a crane decide that it is time to dance? In fact, why does it dance at all? • Animal behavior is based on physiological systems and processes. • An individual behavior is an action carried out by the muscular or hormonal system under the control of the nervous system in response to a stimulus. • Behavior contributes to homeostasis; an animal must acquire nutrients for digestion and find a partner for sexual reproduction. • All of animal physiology contributes to behavior, while animal behavior influences all of physiology. • Being essential for survival and reproduction, animal behavior is subject to substantial selective pressure during evolution. • Behavioral selection also acts on anatomy because body form and appearance contribute directly to the recognition and communication that underlie many behaviors. Concept 51.1: A discrete sensory input is the stimulus for a wide range of animal behaviors. • An animal’s behavior is the sum of its responses to external and internal stimuli. Classical ethology presaged an evolutionary approach to behavioral biology. • In the mid-20th century, pioneering behavioral biologists developed the discipline of ethology, the scientific study of how animals behave in their natural environments. • Niko Tinbergen, of the Netherlands, suggested four questions that must be answered to fully understand any behavior. 1. What stimulus elicits the behavior, and what physiological mechanisms mediate the response? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Instinct Differs from Learned Behavior in That Instinctive Behavior A
    18 Student: ___________________________________________________________________________ 1. Instinct differs from learned behavior in that instinctive behavior A. does not change; learned behavior does. B. is acquired; learned behavior is genetic. C. is adaptive; learned behavior is not. D. All of these answers are true. 2. Assuming that animals have the same feelings as we do is A. ecology. B. anthropomorphism. C. psychology. D. natural history. 3. The most complex type of learning that also uses previous experience to solve a problem is A. imprinting. B. conditioned response. C. insight. D. instinctive. 4. Social behavior occurs in groups, but not all groups display social behavior. Groups that display social behavior often have A. some individuals doing one job and others in the group doing different jobs. B. long periods of contact between parents and offspring. C. elaborate methods of communicating among individuals in the group. D. All of these answers are true. 5. Which of the following is used by some animals in finding their way (navigation)? A. Sense of smell. B. Position of the sun. C. Detection of electromagnetism. D. All of these answers are true, since each is used by some animal. 6. A response an animal develops to a stimulus as a result of pleasant experiences occurring each time the stimulus is received is a kind of learning called A. insight. B. instrumental learning. C. habituation. D. classical conditioning. 7. Dominance hierarchy is a form of A. social behavior. B. territorial behavior. C. imprinting. D. insight learning. 8. Learning is most common in animals that A. are large. B. have large brains.
    [Show full text]
  • The Oedipus Effect Transcript
    The Oedipus Effect Transcript Date: Tuesday, 18 February 2014 - 6:00PM Location: Museum of London 18 February 2014 The Oedipus Effect Professor Glenn D Wilson Sophocles’ great tragedy Oedipus Rex tells the story of a man who, according to the Oracle, is fated to kill his father and marry his mother. Horrified by this prospect he moves far away from home, has a “road rage” confrontation with a stranger and kills him (later to find out it was his father) and goes on to marry a woman who turns out to be his biological mother. He had not realised that the parents from whom he tried to distance himself were actually foster parents. Many other writers have seen profound significance in this story. In 1851, Wagner wrote that “today we need only expound faithfully on the myth of Oedipus and we in it win an intelligible picture of the whole history of mankind”. Uncontainable incestuous desires appeared as a theme in several of his operas. Half a century later, Sigmund Freud made the Oedipus complex the centrepiece of his psychoanalytic theory. Little boys, he concluded, went through a phase of desiring intercourse with their mother and fearing castration by their jealous father. Little girls were thought to desire their father, envying his penis and feeling hostile toward their mother (the Electra complex). Homosexuality (called “inversion”) was said to be caused by a “failure to resolve the Oedipus complex”. Freud became progressively grandiose in his views about the importance of his Oedipus theory. In Totem and Taboo (1918), he wrote that “the beginnings of religion, morals, society and art converge in the Oedipus complex” (Schey, 2013).
    [Show full text]
  • Learned Behavior
    Learning and Adaptation Learning - a process by which long-lasting changes in behavior are acquired as a result of experience. Learned Behavior Successive Approximations (aka Shaping) Learning as an Adaptive Mechanism 7 Types of Learning As a coping mechanism for an ever changing world. 1. Habituation Young predators must learn how to hunt, where to hunt, 2. Imprinting and what to hunt. 3. Associative Learning Water and food resources may change for an animal 4. Social Learning (aka observational learning) 5. Spatial Learning 6. Cognitive Map Learning 7. Insightful Learning (aka problem solving) Habituation - A Type of Learning Imprinting - A Type of Learning A decrease or disappearance of a built-in, natural response to Learning that is irreversible and limited to a sensitive time a stimulus that occurs when the animal repeatedly period in an animal’s life: often results in a strong bond encounters the stimulus. between offspring and parents. Don’t waste your time with stimuli that don’t matter. (We will discuss this one more in the near future.) Associative Learning - A Type of Learning Classical Conditioning - Another example Territorial defense in male blue gourami fish intruder = ‘real’ stimulus Unconditioned fins erect and battle ready = response red light = ‘arbitrary’ stimulus Conditioned fins erect and battle ready = response How would you do this experiment? Classical Conditioning - another example Classical Conditioning - another example Territorial defense in male blue gourami fish Territorial defense in male blue gourami
    [Show full text]
  • Imprinting: Toward a Multilevel Theory Working Paper
    Imprinting: Toward A Multilevel Theory Christopher Marquis András Tilcsik Working Paper 13-061 January 9, 2013 Copyright © 2013 by Christopher Marquis and András Tilcsik Working papers are in draft form. This working paper is distributed for purposes of comment and discussion only. It may not be reproduced without permission of the copyright holder. Copies of working papers are available from the author. Imprinting: Toward A Multilevel Theory Christopher Marquis András Tilcsik Harvard University University of Toronto 333 Morgan Hall 105 St. George Street Boston, MA 02163 Toronto, ON, M5S 3E6, Canada [email protected] [email protected] January 9, 2013 Forthcoming in Academy of Management Annals Acknowledgments. We thank Julie Battilana, Royston Greenwood, Victoria Johnson, Mike Lounsbury, Bill McEvily, George Shinkle, Sameer Srivastava, and Art Stinchcombe for helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper. Imprinting: Toward A Multilevel Theory Abstract The concept of imprinting has attracted considerable interest in numerous fields—including organizational ecology, institutional theory, network analysis, and career research—and has been applied at several levels of analysis, from the industry to the individual. This article offers a critical review of this rich yet disparate literature and guides research toward a multilevel theory of imprinting. We start with a definition that captures the general features of imprinting across levels of analysis but is precise enough to remain distinct from seemingly similar concepts, such as path dependence and cohort effects. We then provide a framework to order and unite the splintered field of imprinting research at different levels of analysis. In doing so, we identify economic, technological, institutional, and individual influences that lead to imprints at the level of (a) organizational collectives, (b) single organizations, (c) organizational building blocks, and (d) individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • Article Describes and Defines a Phenomenon That Is Termed Genetic Sexual Attraction
    GENETIC SEXUAL ATTRACTION Leslie Pate Mackinnon, L.C.S.W. www.lesliepatemackinnonlcsw.com This article describes and defines a phenomenon that is termed genetic sexual attraction. The incidence of genetic sexual attraction was first reported following an uptick in the number of reunions between adopted individuals and their blood relatives. The earliest reports of adopted individuals searching began to emerge in the 1950’s. The organic and natural curiosity for adoptees to search, began to show up in the literature and search groups began to form across the country. In the 1970’s searching picked up pace dramatically. This timing coincided with the return of adult adoptees who were placed from the Baby Scoop Era. relaxation of adoption laws which gave adoptees easier access to the records. Today, DNA has provided both the interest and the ease of adoption searches. It’s almost the norm to search and fewer people are passing up on the opportunity to connect. This in turn led to an increase in reunions which is a meeting between adopted individuals and their biological relatives. With the increase, reports of Genetic Sexual Attraction, or GSA, as it is commonly referred to, began to randomly emerge in both the U.S. and around the world. Most individuals experiencing the intense emotions of reunion, describe a strong feeling of 'genetic attraction'. This genetic attraction is defined as a fascination with the newly found relative, as well as with the many similarities that they see mirrored in one another. Often those in the early stages of a reunion spend inordinate amounts of time in contact with the other person.
    [Show full text]
  • A Stimulus Control Analysis of Imprinting in a Human-Reared Pigeon
    A STIMULUS CONTROL ANALYSIS OF IMPRINTING IN A HUMAN-REARED PIGEON Christopher A. Varnon, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS August 2011 APPROVED: Jesús Rosales-Ruiz, Major Professor Jonathan Pinkston, Committee Member Manish Vaidya, Committee Member Richard Smith, Chair of the Department of Behavior Analysis Thomas Evenson, Dean of the College of Public Affairs and Community Service James D. Meernik, Acting Dean of the Toulouse Graduate School Varnon, Christopher A., A stimulus control analysis of imprinting in a human- reared pigeon. Doctor of Philosophy (Behavioral Analysis), August 2011, 81 pp., 4 tables, 23 figures, references, 52 titles. Events that occur early in the life of birds greatly influence social and sexual preferences throughout the course of life. Traditionally, this is explained by a learning process known as imprinting. Young birds are thought to imprint to early stimuli, causing the development of permanent preferences for those stimuli. In the present study, imprinting is examined with respect to behaviors of an adult human-reared pigeon in several conditions. The subject was either presented with no stimulus, a conspecific stimulus, a novel stimulus, a human stimulus, or the human and novel stimuli simultaneously. Several phases within these conditions were employed to pinpoint the variables that produced the most social and sexual behavior. The results showed that while some conditions produced unclear behavior, other conditions produced very clear indications of sexual preference for humans and fear of conspecifics. The results suggest that the concept of imprinting may not be needed to explain the sexual preference of the subject, and that operant contingencies may play a large role in sexual behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding in Buffalo and Other Farm Animals
    animals Review Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding in Buffalo and Other Farm Animals Agustín Orihuela 1,* , Daniel Mota-Rojas 2 , Ana Strappini 3 , Francesco Serrapica 4 , Ada Braghieri 5 , Patricia Mora-Medina 6 and Fabio Napolitano 5 1 Facultad de Ciencias Agropecuarias, Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos, Cuernavaca 62209, Morelos, Mexico 2 Neurophysiology, Behavior and Animal Welfare Assessment, DPAA, Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana, (UAM), Mexico City 04960, Mexico; [email protected] 3 Faculty of Veterinary Sciences, Animal Science Institute, Universidad Austral de Chile, Valdivia 5090000, Chile; [email protected] 4 Dipartimento di Agraria, Universitàdi Napoli Federico II, Via Università100, 80055 Portici, Italy; [email protected] 5 Scuola di Scienze Agrarie, Forestali, Alimentari ed Ambientali, Università degli Studi della Basilicata, 85100 Potenza, Italy; [email protected] (A.B.); [email protected] (F.N.) 6 Livestock Science Department, Facultad de Estudios Superiores Cuautitlán, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México (UNAM), Mexico City 54714, Mexico; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] Simple Summary: The present paper reviews the importance of bonding for the survival and well- being in the cow–calf relationship. The review focuses on buffaloes and information from other Citation: Orihuela, A.; Mota-Rojas, species is used for comparison or to find more general patterns in the absence of specific sources. D.; Strappini, A.; Serrapica, F.; Differences between several farm species are also described, focusing on the role played by the Braghieri, A.; Mora-Medina, P.; sensory stimuli during the sensitive period after birth. How bonding can be classified according to Napolitano, F. Neurophysiological the predominant senses used by different species, the importance of learning (i.e., imprinting) in the Mechanisms of Cow–Calf Bonding in development of mother–young relationship, and the neurobiological mechanisms involved are also Buffalo and Other Farm Animals.
    [Show full text]
  • Princeton/Stanford Working Papers in Classics
    Princeton/Stanford Working Papers in Classics Evolutionary psychology and the historian Version 1.0 April 2013 Walter Scheidel Stanford University Abstract: New possibilities have been opened up for historians by a new wave of engagement with biology, or more particularly with human biology, for the study of human history, environmental history, health history, and the co-evolutionary history of humans and other species. This paper critically explores the uses and limits of evolutionary psychology for the study of history by focusing on the particularly intensely discussed phenomenon of incest avoidance. © Walter Scheidel. [email protected] “He who is the son says this to his mother: ‘Let us give (ourselves) in bodily intercourse so that we shall not have fear of Hell, and the sins we have committed will go from the account, and at the Cinwad Bridge [of posthumous judgment] we will be (pure in) heart, and a beautiful and seemly place will be ours, and we shall please Ohrmazd and cause harm to Ahriman.’ The mother says this to (her) son if she speaks righteously: ‘I give (myself) to you (in) bodily intercourse,’ even as that son said. The father says this to (his) daughter, the brother says this to his sister, (so) the sister says this to her brother.” 1 In the former Zoroastrian tradition represented in this text from around 900 CE, sexual relations between members of the nuclear family were not only to be encouraged but meant to result in progeny: “And there are those children who are born of a father and his daughter. Light flashes and glows and can be seen in course of time; and very fortunate and pleased is someone who has a child of his child (…).
    [Show full text]
  • An Experimental Test of the Westermarck Effect: Sex Differences in Inbreeding Avoidance
    Behavioral The official journal of the ISBE Ecology International Society for Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/art028 Advance Access publication 18 April 2013 Original Article An experimental test of the Westermarck effect: sex differences in inbreeding avoidance Urszula M. Marcinkowska,a Fhionna R. Moore,b and Markus J. Rantalaa Downloaded from aUniversity of Turku, Department of Biology, Sect Ecol, Turku 20014, Finland and bUniversity of Dundee, School of Psychology, Dundee DD1 4HN, Scotland UK In order to avoid inbreeding, humans and other animals develop a strong sexual aversion to individuals with whom they have lived closely in http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ infancy and early childhood (usually biological siblings), a phenomenon called the “Westermarck effect” or negative sexual imprinting. The mechanisms underlying this phenomenon, however, remain unclear. For example, it is not known whether negative imprinting is based only on actual sexual aversion to brothers and sisters or also on generalizing the traits of their siblings to nonkin. If imprinting is more general, one could predict that people would avoid mating with all individuals that resemble their other-sex siblings. In our study, women rated morphed other-sex faces that resemble their siblings as significantly lower in sexual attractiveness than morphed faces on average, and the opposite effect was found in men—similarity to sibling increased perceived attractiveness. Interestingly, self-similarity did not influence the preferences of either men or women. These sex differences are consistent with parental investment theory, as females bear greater costs associated with inbreeding depression, perhaps explaining their deeper aversion toward engagement in sexual activities with male individuals who bear cues to related- ness.
    [Show full text]
  • Imprinting and Kin Recognition Imprinting
    Imprinting and kin recognition Imprinting Konrad Lorenz Filial imprinting Critical period Experimental approaches Sensitive period Hours after hatching precocial altricial Filial imprinting Multiple cues strengthen imprinting response Filial imprinting Timing of critical period Locomotion-fear dichotomy Tolerance of novel objects Locomotor performance Hours after hatching Sexual imprinting Process of learning characteristics of future mates Preferred humans ring dove Reared by hand Reared with conspecifics Preferred doves Sexual imprinting Recognition of appropriate mates acquisition stage consolidation stage hatching sexual maturity time Sexual imprinting Sexual imprinting as way to strike balance between species recognition and inbreeding Male Japanese quail raised with females preferred… < < …slightly similar over identical over dissimilar females. Sexual imprinting as evolutionary force Mate assortatively when placed together Assortative mating due to sexual imprinting Sexual imprinting as evolutionary force Pairs of benthic (top) and limnetic (bottom) of sticklebacks Divergent selection Parental care exclusively by male Sexual imprinting as evolutionary force Heterospecific Conspecific father father Heterospecific No odor father Tested female mate preferences as adults Sexual imprinting as evolutionary force Prefer benthics conspecific limnetics male Prefer heterospecific male Sexual imprinting as evolutionary force benthics limnetics heterospecific conspecific biological Strength of imprinting correlated with father’s parental care
    [Show full text]
  • Stability and Individual Variability of Social Attachment in Imprinting
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.04.025072; this version posted April 5, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Stability and individual variability of social attachment in imprinting Bastien S. Lemaire1*, Daniele Rucco1, Mathilde Josserand1,2, Giorgio Vallortigara1, Elisabetta Versace3,1,4* 1 Center for Mind and Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Italy 2 Ecole Normale Supérieure Lyon, France 3School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, United Kingdom 4Alan Turing Institute, United Kingdom Author for correspondence:* [email protected]; [email protected] Abstract Filial imprinting has become a model for understanding memory, learning and social behaviour in neonate animals. This fast attachment mechanism allows the young of precocial bird species to learn the characteristics of conspicuous visual stimuli and display affiliative response to them. Although more prolonged exposure to an object produces a stronger preference for it afterwards, this relation is not linear. Chicks can even prefer to approach novel rather than familiar objects at some stages of imprinting. The time course and stability of imprinting has just started to be investigated. To date, little is known about how filial preferences develop across time, due to the challenges in assessing individual performance. This study aimed to investigate filial preferences for familiar and novel imprinting objects over time. We have used an automated setup to track the behaviour of chicks continuously for subsequent days.
    [Show full text]