Hungarian Vowel Harmony

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Hungarian Vowel Harmony i- Hungarian Vowel Harmony Anna Dunbar-Hester Senior Thesis, Linguistiscs and Languages Bryn Mawr and Swarthmore Colleges December 5,2002 In this paper I will be discussing the Hungarian Vowel Harmony System. First I will give a brief discussion of the Hungarian Suffix System, then introduce the Vowel Harmony System. Next I will explain the field research that I have done, after which I will make conclusions about the rules of harmony in Hungarian and formalize these rules through Generative Phonology and Optimality Theory. 1. The Hungarian Suffixation System. 1. The Hungarian Vowels Hungarian has an inventory of 14 vowels. Following precedent set by Robert Vago (1974), I will use native orthography throughout this paper for ease of interpreting data. The vowel system is as follows: FRONT BACK UNROUNDED ROUNDED UNROUNDED ROUNDED Short Long Short Long (Short) Long Short Long . HIGH i r 0 u u U MID e 6 0 a 6 LOW e a a Because the vowel system is so complex, I will also include here a feature analysis to help break down the above information: Native PhonetiC Back Round High Low Long Orthography Representation i i - - + - - i i: -- + - + e E - - - + - e e - - - - + a a + + - + - a ::> + -- + + 0 0 + + - - - 6 0: + + - - + 0 00 - + - - - 0 00: - + - - + u u + + + - - U u: + + + - + li y - + + - - U y: - + + - + 2. Suffixation Hungarian employs a large system of suffixes, which are heavily used to fonn words, each suffix having a single morphological function. The following examples are taken from Siptar and Torkenczy (2000) because they are great at demonstrating the wide range of suffix use available in Hungarian. a. barat -sag -os -abb -an friend -ship adj. compo adv. 'in a more friendly manner' b. haz -as -u -ando -k -nak house adj. verb participle pI. dat. 'for those intending to get married' C. te -het -seg -es -ebb -ek-et do -able -ness adj. compo pI. acc. 'the more talented ones' (ace.) 3. The Case System A number of common suffixes belong to the case group, which I have included here because they are necessary for understanding Vowel Hannony. This list is derived from two sources, one of Siptar and Torkenczy (2000) and the second from Torkenczy (1997), in attempts to provide the most complete list. Case Marker A~proximate Meaning Nominative 0 (subiect) Accusative -t (object) Dative -nak/-nek to for Instrumental -vall-vel with Illative -ba/-be Into Sublative -ra/-re onto Affative -hozl -hezl -hOz to Inessive -banI-ben in Superessive -onl -enl -on on Adessive -nal/-nel at Elative -bOl/-ool out of Delative -rol/-rol of about from top of Ablative -toll-toI from Causal/Final -ert for Translative -vaJ-ve (turn) into Essive/Formal kent kePR, ulLi.i1 similar to something Terminative -IQ up to Associative ~stul/-esti.il/-asti.il together with Temporal -kor at Distributive -onta/ -ente/ -ante repeatedly, every Note that some vowels sometimes have alternates and some do not. The vowel choices fall into the following sets: 1) ale 2) o/ela 3) ale 4) 6/0 5) e 6) i 7) 0 8) ufO. II. Hungarian Vowel Harmony. 1. Vowel Harmony Types Polgardi (1998) says "Harmony is a process whereby some segmental feature associates to all segments of a certain type in a specific domain." Hungarian contains two kinds of vowel harmony, one applying to roundness and the second to backness. Roundness Harmony in Hungarian is quite simple and therefore many linguists do not spend too much time studying it or debating its driving forces. My research is also not going to involve Roundness Harmony. The second type of Vowel Harmony employed in Hungarian is Backness Harmony, which is much more complicated than Roundness Harmony and therefore more controversial. I will do my best to present the basic arguments regarding Backness Harmony, and present the case that is most satisfying and complete in my opinion, that of Miklos Tarkenczy and Peter Siptar, in their book entitled The Phonology of Hungarian. 2. Outline of Backness Harmony Now I will give a basic outline of Backness Harmony to aide the understanding of upcoming issues. Backness Harmony works on the prinCiples of keeping vowels of one type or another (front or back) within an individual word. Hungarians pride themselves on haVing a harmonic language because it essentially makes the words prettier to listen to and easier to pronounce. Native, non-compound word roots in Hungarian are either of the front or back type. A front word root has only front vowels or front and neutral vowels only. A back word root has only back vowels. Sometimes there are mixed vowel root words though, when a word root has both neutral and back vowels, and that is where real problems begin. We should expect that back and neutral vowels yield a back root (isn't that what neutral means?), but this is not the case. 3. Neutral Vowels Neutral vowels are generally considered to be e, ~ i, and f, although there is debate over e. Neutral vowels are sometimes referred to as "transparent" vowels, because they appear to be Invisible In the harmonizing process. Sometimes when multiple neutral vowels occur In succession they do become players in the harmonization, and that is when they are no longer transparent, but rather opaque. (This occurs when they are the last two vowels in a word root.) I will continue to use 'neutral' and not 'transparent,' but later on there will be more discussion of the opaque instances, as they are a major issue. There also seem to be degrees of neutrality, as Ringen and Kontra (1989) suggest and Torkenczy and Siptar (2000) expand upon, where high front unrounded vowels are considered the most neutral a I), the mid front unrounded vowel is less neutral (e), arid the low front unrounded vowel is the least neutral (e). The motivations for classifying different degrees of neutrality will become more apparent when we examine mixed word roots later on, but the basic justifications are as follows. The high unrounded vowels land {never alternate harmonically, because there are no Hungarian back counterparts to these vowels, which makes land (the most harmonic. In suffixes, ealways alternates with either aor O{6, and eeither has no alternate or alternates with a. So, eis less harmonic than iand ( because It sometimes alternates, and e Is the least harmonic because it always alternates. As further evidence that e is the least harmonic, there are no back harmonic neutral vowel roots that contain e, and mixed vowel roots that contain e in the last syllable are the most unstable with respect to selecting a I suffix vowel (Kenesei, Vago, Fenyvesi, 1998). 4. Simple Applications To demonstrate vowel harmony, I am going to provide a few simple examples. Notice in the case chart in 1.3 that some cases have alternate forms, i.e. -val/vel. The correct form is chosen based on whether the vowels in the root of the word make the root front harmonic or back harmonic. If a word contains front harmonic vowels (0, 0, 0, (I), it is considered a front-harmonic root and governs a front- vowel suffix. Note that if a word has a prefix, this does not effect the harmony of the root. An internal word boundary is present in this case. Note also that a word with only neutral vowels acts as front- harmonic. I posit that this occurs because neutral vowels are technically front vowels, even though they are not front-harmonic. A group of exception words of this type will be addressed later. Front Harmonv Back Harmonv gyumolcs 7 gyumolcsvel with the fruit kutya 7 kutyaval with the dog Liveg 7 uvegbOlout of the bottle lampa 7 lampabOl out of the lamp no 7 nonel at the woman nap 7 napnal at the day Peter 7 Peternek for Peter falu 7 falunak for the village kenyer 7 kenyerben in the bread paradicsom7 paradicsomban in the tomato 5. Complications As I stated before, sometimes neutral vowels behave in an opaque manner. Torkenczy and Siptar (2000) have developed a model for laying out exactly the possible behaviors of mixed stems are, which r will produce an expanded version of shortly. I am In favor of this analysis because of its abUity to describe all the complex data in one concise chart, which gives us insight Into the system of when :'::'. different suffixes are chosen. I will now summarize their analysis of mixed root behavior. A mixed root is categorized by the last two vowels in the stem. (Remember that to be a mixed root the root must contain back and neutral vowels.) First, we have a category of roots that have a back vowel in their penultimate vowel position and a neutral vowel in the last vowel position. Cases exist where, regardless of which neutral vowel is chosen, the only vowel that is chosen for the suffix is back. If the neutral vowel is e or ehowever, there are stems that govern either front or back vowels in the suffix, but either is fine. These are called vacillating endings. Some roots only take a front vowel suffix, and this is referred to as a disharmonic suffix. This only occurs when e is in the neutral vowel position. The second category of roots has a neutral vowel in the penultimate vowel position and another neutral vowel in the final vowel pOSition, giving two neutral vowels in a row. These have long been known to cause problems, for example, analizis-nek, november-nek. In these roots, the vowels apparently do not act fully neutral. There are some cases in which they seem to gain opaque status and create the power for governing a front vowel suffix, and other cases in which the stems are again vacillating.
Recommended publications
  • Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops
    Title Page Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops by Eva Bacas University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2019 Committee Page UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES This thesis was presented by Eva Bacas It was defended on November 8, 2019 and approved by Alana DeLoge, Quechua Instructor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Melinda Fricke, Assistant Professor, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh Gillian Gallagher, Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics, New York University Thesis Advisor/Dissertation Director: Claude Mauk, Senior Lecturer, Department of Linguistics, University of Pittsburgh ii Copyright © by Eva Bacas 2019 iii Abstract Sociophonetic Variation in Bolivian Quechua Uvular Stops Eva Bacas, BPhil University of Pittsburgh, 2019 Quechua is an indigenous language of the Andes region of South America. In Cochabamba, Bolivia, Quechua and Spanish have been in contact for over 500 years. In this thesis, I explore sociolinguistic variation among bilingual speakers of Cochabamba Quechua (CQ) and Spanish by investigating the relationship between the production of the voiceless uvular stop /q/ and speakers’ sociolinguistic backgrounds. I conducted a speech production study and sociolinguistic interview with seven bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers. I analyzed manner of articulation and place of articulation variation. Results indicate that manner of articulation varies primarily due to phonological factors, and place of articulation varies according to sociolinguistic factors. This reveals that among bilingual CQ-Spanish speakers, production of voiceless uvular stop /q/ does vary sociolinguistically.
    [Show full text]
  • LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin
    LT3212 Phonetics Assignment 4 Mavis, Wong Chak Yin Essay Title: The sound system of Japanese This essay aims to introduce the sound system of Japanese, including the inventories of consonants, vowels, and diphthongs. The phonological variations of the sound segments in different phonetic environments are also included. For the illustration, word examples are given and they are presented in the following format: [IPA] (Romaji: “meaning”). Consonants In Japanese, there are 14 core consonants, and some of them have a lot of allophonic variations. The various types of consonants classified with respect to their manner of articulation are presented as follows. Stop Japanese has six oral stops or plosives, /p b t d k g/, which are classified into three place categories, bilabial, alveolar, and velar, as listed below. In each place category, there is a pair of plosives with the contrast in voicing. /p/ = a voiceless bilabial plosive [p]: [ippai] (ippai: “A cup of”) /b/ = a voiced bilabial plosive [b]: [baɴ] (ban: “Night”) /t/ = a voiceless alveolar plosive [t]: [oto̞ ːto̞ ] (ototo: “Brother”) /d/ = a voiced alveolar plosive [d]: [to̞ mo̞ datɕi] (tomodachi: “Friend”) /k/ = a voiceless velar plosive [k]: [kaiɰa] (kaiwa: “Conversation”) /g/ = a voiced velar plosive [g]: [ɡakɯβsai] (gakusai: “Student”) Phonetically, Japanese also has a glottal stop [ʔ] which is commonly produced to separate the neighboring vowels occurring in different syllables. This phonological phenomenon is known as ‘glottal stop insertion’. The glottal stop may be realized as a pause, which is used to indicate the beginning or the end of an utterance. For instance, the word “Japanese money” is actually pronounced as [ʔe̞ ɴ], instead of [je̞ ɴ], and the pronunciation of “¥15” is [dʑɯβːɡo̞ ʔe̞ ɴ].
    [Show full text]
  • The Sound Patterns of Camuno: Description and Explanation in Evolutionary Phonology
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects 6-2014 The Sound Patterns Of Camuno: Description And Explanation In Evolutionary Phonology Michela Cresci Graduate Center, City University of New York How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/191 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] THE SOUND PATTERNS OF CAMUNO: DESCRIPTION AND EXPLANATION IN EVOLUTIONARY PHONOLOGY by MICHELA CRESCI A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, The City Universtiy of New York 2014 i 2014 MICHELA CRESCI All rights reserved ii This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in satisfaction of the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. JULIETTE BLEVINS ____________________ __________________________________ Date Chair of Examining Committee GITA MARTOHARDJONO ____________________ ___________________________________ Date Executive Officer KATHLEEN CURRIE HALL DOUGLAS H. WHALEN GIOVANNI BONFADINI Supervisory Committee THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK iii Abstract THE SOUND PATTERNS OF CAMUNO: DESCRIPTION AND EXPLANATION IN EVOLUTIONARY PHONOLOGY By Michela Cresci Advisor: Professor Juliette Blevins This dissertation presents a linguistic study of the sound patterns of Camuno framed within Evolutionary Phonology (Blevins, 2004, 2006, to appear). Camuno is a variety of Eastern Lombard, a Romance language of northern Italy, spoken in Valcamonica. Camuno is not a local variety of Italian, but a sister of Italian, a local divergent development of the Latin originally spoken in Italy (Maiden & Perry, 1997, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. UMI University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information C om pany 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Order Number 9401204 Phonetics and phonology of Nantong Chinese Ac, Benjamin Xiaoping, Ph.D. The Ohio State University, 1993 Copyri^t ©1993 by Ao, Benjamin Xiaoping.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetics and Phonology Seminar Introduction to Linguistics, Andrew
    Phonetics and Phonology Phonetics and Phonology Voicing: In voiced sounds, the vocal cords (=vocal folds, Stimmbände) are pulled together Seminar Introduction to Linguistics, Andrew McIntyre and vibrate, unlike in voiceless sounds. Compare zoo/sue, ban/pan. Tests for voicing: 1 Phonetics vs. phonology Put hand on larynx. You feel more vibrations with voiced consonants. Phonetics deals with three main areas: Say [fvfvfv] continuously with ears blocked. [v] echoes inside your head, unlike [f]. Articulatory phonetics: speech organs & how they move to produce particular sounds. Acoustic phonetics: what happens in the air between speaker & hearer; measurable 4.2 Description of English consonants (organised by manners of articulation) using devices such as a sonograph, which analyses frequencies. The accompanying handout gives indications of the positions of the speech organs Auditory phonetics: how sounds are perceived by the ear, how the brain interprets the referred to below, and the IPA description of all sounds in English and other languages. information coming from the ear. Phonology: study of how particular sounds are used (in particular languages, in languages 4.2.1 Plosives generally) to distinguish between words. Study of how sounds form systems in (particular) Plosive (Verschlusslaut): complete closure somewhere in vocal tract, then air released. languages. Examples of phonological observations: (2) Bilabial (both lips are the active articulators): [p,b] in pie, bye The underlined sound sequence in German Strumpf can occur in the middle of words (3) Alveolar (passive articulator is the alveolar ridge (=gum ridge)): [t,d] in to, do in English (ashtray) but not at the beginning or end. (4) Velar (back of tongue approaches soft palate (velum)): [k,g] in cat, go In pan and span the p-sound is pronounced slightly differently.
    [Show full text]
  • The State of California Vowels
    Background Methods The CVS: (1) the lowering of the front lax vowels Cory Holland University of California, Davis Data Collection: Because the goal of this study was to collect data from as wide a (2) the fronting of TRAP before nasals Shifting or Shifted? The state of California vowels geographical distribution as possible, data were collected using several recruitment methods: (1) (3) the fronting of GOOSE and FOOT and the centralization of GOAT in person from a diverse pool of undergraduate and graduate students and department staff at (4) the merger of back vowels before /l/ the university (2) by email from academic, professional and social networks. Those participants (5) the merger of LOT and THOUGHT and the backing of the resulting vowel not contacted in person were instructed to record the reading passage in their home in .wav (6) fronting of STRUT format, if possible, and to make the recording in a place with minimal background noise, and F2 (Hz) F2 (Hz) 2400 2200 2000 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 600 2200 2100 2000 1900 1800 1700 1600 1500 1400 1300 1200 1100 read the passage with natural feeling speed and intonation. (1) Front lax vowel lowering: Reports are somewhat mixed, but recent reports have KIT and DRESS 300 350 1. Retracting of front lax vowels 3. Fronting of back vowels 4. Merger of back vowels before /l/ Reading Passages: A total of 6872 tokens from “The Boy who cried wolf” (Deterding, 2006) lowering, when not pre-/n/ to the point that KIT is even in height with FACE.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 ILLUSTRATION of the IPA Kazakh Adam G. Mccollum University Of
    1 ILLUSTRATION OF THE IPA Kazakh Adam G. McCollum University of California San Diego [email protected] Si Chen Hong Kong Polytechnic University [email protected] Kazakh (ISO 639-3, kaz) is a Kipchak (Northwestern) Turkic language with approximately ten million speakers (Muhamedowa 2015). While the majority of Kazakh speakers live in the Republic of Kazakhstan, significant Kazakh-speaking populations exist throughout Central Asia. Kazakh spoken in Kazakhstan is described as having three or four dialects, but many researchers agree that differences between dialects are small and largely lexical (Kara 2002; Grenoble 2003; Muhamedowa 2015; see Amanzholov 1959 for more on Kazakh dialects). <Insert Figure 1 about here> Sound files were recorded from one Kazakh speaker in San Diego, CA. The analysis derives largely from sound files recorded in San Diego, California, but is also informed by significant fieldwork in Kazakhstan. The consultant is a female in her early thirties from the Zhambul region of southeastern Kazakhstan. She has lived in the U.S. for five years, and speaks Kazakh, Russian, Turkish, and English. Excluding the data collected for the analysis of stress, as well as The North Wind and the Sun passage, all words were produced in isolation. 2 Consonants Bilabial Dental/Alveolar Postalveolar Palatal Velar Uvular Plosive p b t̪ d̪ k ɡ q Nasal m n̪ ŋ Trill r Fricative s̪ z̪ ʃ ʒ χ ʁ Approximant w j Lateral l̪ Approximant The Kazakh consonantal inventory consists of the twenty contrastive sounds listed above (Balakayev 1962:40). Note that many (and possibly all) of these consonants alternate for the backness and roundness of flanking vowels, making it difficult in some cases to determine whether an alternation is between two phonemes or between allophones of a single phoneme.
    [Show full text]
  • FL 004 293 Malikouti-Drachman, Angeliki
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 082 566 FL 004 293 AUTHOR Malikouti-Drachman, Angeliki; And Others TITLE Working Papers in Linguistics No. 15. INSTITUTION Ohio State Univ., Columbus. Dept, of Linguistics. PUB DATE Apr 73 NOTE 224p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$9.87 DESCRIPTORS Articulation (Speech); Bibliographies; Charts; Child Language; Cognitive Processes; Consonants; Diagrams; Intonation; *Language Learning Levels; *Linguistic Patterns; Morphology (Languages); *Oral Expression; Phonemics; Phonetics; *Phonology; Physiology; Pronunciation; Sentence Structure; Vowels; *Written Language ABSTRACT This issue is devoted to first language acquisition. It includes twelve papers concerning: the acquisition of liquids ("1" and "r"); creative errors in the written syntax of deaf children; theoretical and methodological problems in the acquisition of phonology, illustrated from Greek an? English: the basis of articulation; the philosophy of scie.ce; and vowel nasalization. (Author/DD) st' WORKING PAPERS IN LINGUISTICS NO. 15 By Angeliki Malikouti-Drachman, Gaberell Drachman, Mary Louise Edwards, Jonnie E. Geis, and Lawrence C. Schourup U S DEPARTMENT Of MELT4 EOUCTION t WELFARE NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF EOU.:ATiON THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRO DLJCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIGIN ATING IT POINTS OF VIEW OR OPINIONS STATEO 00 NOT NECESSARILY REPRE SENT OFFICIAL NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF EDUCATION POSITION OW POLICY Department of Linguistics The Ohio State University Columbus, Ohio 43210 April 1973 FILMED FROM BEST AVAILABLE COPY Int2oduction This issue of Working Papers is very largely devoted to first language acquisition. The first two papers present the full and unabridged version of ilary Edwards' master's thesis (June 1971) on the acquisition of liquids, and part of Jonnie Geis' work on creative errors in the (written) syntax of deaf children.
    [Show full text]
  • 2 I F the Production of Speech Sounds
    2 The productifion of speech sounds 2.1 Articulators above the larynx All the sounds we make when we speak are the result of muscles contracting. The muscles in the chest that we use for breathing produce the flow of air that is needed for almost all speech sounds; muscles in the larynxx produce many different modifications in the flow of air from the chest to the mouth. After passing through the larynx, the air goes through what we call the vocal tract, which ends at the mouth and nostrils; we call the part comprising the mouth the oral cavityy and the part that leads to the nostrils the nasal cavity. Here the air from the lungs escapes into the atmosphere. We have a large and complex set of muscles that can produce changes in the shape of the vocal tract, and in order to learn how the sounds of speech are produced it is necessary to become familiar with the different parts of the vocal tract. These different parts are called articulators, and the study of them is called articulatory phonetics. Fig. 1 is a diagram that is used frequently in the study of phonetics. It represents the human head, seen from the side, displayed as though it had been cut in half. You will need to look at it carefully as the articulators are described, and you will find it useful to have a mirror and a good light placed so that you can look at the inside of your mouth. i) The pharynxx is a tube which begins just above the larynx.
    [Show full text]
  • Introductory Phonology
    9781405184120_1_pre.qxd 06/06/2008 09:47 AM Page iii Introductory Phonology Bruce Hayes A John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Publication 9781405184120_4_C04.qxd 06/06/2008 09:50 AM Page 70 4 Features 4.1 Introduction to Features: Representations Feature theory is part of a general approach in cognitive science which hypo- thesizes formal representations of mental phenomena. A representation is an abstract formal object that characterizes the essential properties of a mental entity. To begin with an example, most readers of this book are familiar with the words and music of the song “Happy Birthday to You.” The question is: what is it that they know? Or, to put it very literally, what information is embodied in their neurons that distinguishes a knower of “Happy Birthday” from a hypothetical person who is identical in every other respect but does not know the song? Much of this knowledge must be abstract. People can recognize “Happy Birth- day” when it is sung in a novel key, or by an unfamiliar voice, or using a different tempo or form of musical expression. Somehow, they can ignore (or cope in some other way with) inessential traits and attend to the essential ones. The latter include the linguistic text, the (relative) pitch sequences of the notes, the relative note dura- tions, and the musical harmonies that (often tacitly) accompany the tune. Cognitive science posits that humans possess mental representations, that is, formal mental objects depicting the structure of things we know or do. A typical claim is that we are capable of singing “Happy Birthday” because we have (during childhood) internalized a mental representation, fairly abstract in character, that embodies the structure of this song.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Published By
    The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Trans 10 fax: +31 30 253 6000 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://wwwlot.let.uu.nl/ Cover illustration by Silke Hamann ISBN 90-76864-39-X NUR 632 Copyright © 2003 Silke Hamann. All rights reserved. The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes Fonetiek en fonologie van retroflexen (met een samenvatting in het Nederlands) Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit Utrecht op gezag van de Rector Magnificus, Prof. Dr. W.H. Gispen, ingevolge het besluit van het College voor Promoties in het openbaar te verdedigen op vrijdag 6 juni 2003 des middags te 4.15 uur door Silke Renate Hamann geboren op 25 februari 1971 te Lampertheim, Duitsland Promotoren: Prof. dr. T. A. Hall (Leipzig University) Prof. dr. Wim Zonneveld (Utrecht University) Contents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Markedness of retroflexes 3 1.2 Phonetic cues and phonological features 6 1.3 Outline of the dissertation 8 Part I: Phonetics of Retroflexes 2 Articulatory variation and common properties of retroflexes 11 2.1 Phonetic terminology 12 2.2 Parameters of articulatory variation 14 2.2.1 Speaker dependency 15 2.2.2 Vowel context 16 2.2.3 Speech rate 17 2.2.4 Manner dependency 19 2.2.4.1 Plosives 19 2.2.4.2 Nasals 20 2.2.4.3 Fricatives 21 2.2.4.4 Affricates 23 2.2.4.5 Laterals 24 2.2.4.6 Rhotics 25 2.2.4.7 Retroflex vowels 26 2.2.5 Language family 27 2.2.6 Iventory size 28 2.3 Common articulatory properties of retroflexion 32 2.3.1 Apicality 33 2.3.2 Posteriority
    [Show full text]
  • A Corpus for Large-Scale Phonetic Typology
    A Corpus for Large-Scale Phonetic Typology Elizabeth SaleskyC Eleanor Chodroff7 Tiago PimentelZ Matthew WiesnerC Ryan CotterellZ;D Alan W Blackì Jason EisnerC CJohns Hopkins University 7University of York ZUniversity of Cambridge DETH Zurich¨ ìCarnegie Mellon University [email protected] [email protected] Abstract A major hurdle in data-driven research on ty- pology is having sufficient data in many lan- MCD guages to draw meaningful conclusions. We 5 6 present VoxClamantis V1.0, the first large- 7 8 scale corpus for phonetic typology, with 9 aligned segments and estimated phoneme- Figure 1: The 635 languages of our corpus geo-located level labels in 690 readings spanning 635 with mean Mel Cepstral Distortion (MCD) scores. languages, along with acoustic-phonetic mea- sures of vowels and sibilants. Access to Existing multilingual speech corpora have similar such data can greatly facilitate investigation restrictions, with data too limited for many tasks of phonetic typology at a large scale and (Engstrand and Cunningham-Andersson, 1988; across many languages. However, it is non- Ladefoged and Maddieson, 2007) or approximately trivial and computationally intensive to ob- 20 to 30 recorded languages (Ardila et al., 2020; tain such alignments for hundreds of lan- Harper, 2011; Schultz, 2002). guages, many of which have few to no re- The recently developed CMU Wilderness corpus sources presently available. We describe the (Black, 2019) constitutes an exception to this rule methodology to create our corpus, discuss with over 600 languages. This makes it the largest caveats with current methods and their impact and most typologically diverse speech corpus on the utility of this data, and illustrate pos- to date.
    [Show full text]