Social Exclusion Causes People to Spend and Consume Strategically in the Service of Affiliation
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Social Exclusion Causes People to Spend and Consume Strategically in the Service of Affiliation NICOLE L. MEAD ROY F. BAUMEISTER TYLER F. STILLMAN CATHERINE D. RAWN KATHLEEN D. VOHS When people’s deeply ingrained need for social connection is thwarted by social exclusion, profound psychological consequences ensue. Despite the fact that social connections and consumption are central facets of daily life, little empirical attention has been devoted to understanding how belongingness threats affect consumer behavior. In four experiments, we tested the hypothesis that social exclusion causes people to spend and consume strategically in the service of affiliation. Relative to controls, excluded participants were more likely to buy a product sym- bolic of group membership (but not practical or self-gift items), tailor their spending preferences to the preferences of an interaction partner, spend money on an un- appealing food item favored by a peer, and report being willing to try an illegal drug, but only when doing so boosted their chances of commencing social con- nections. Overall, results suggest that socially excluded people sacrifice personal and financial well-being for the sake of social well-being. he desire for social relationships is one of the most fun- consequences for people’s psychological and physiological T damental and universal of all human needs (Baumeister functioning (Buckley, Winkel, and Leary 2004; DeWall and and Leary 1995). Social exclusion, a painful yet common part Baumeister 2006; Maner et al. 2007; Twenge et al. 2001; of life, thwarts this ingrained motivation and has striking Williams 2001). For example, threat of exclusion stimulates brain regions designed to detect and regulate pain (Eisen- Nicole L. Mead ([email protected]) is TIBER postdoctoral research fel- berger, Lieberman, and Williams 2003), impairs self-regula- low, Department of Marketing, Tilburg University, Tilburg 5000LE. Roy tion (Baumeister et al. 2005), hampers logical reasoning (Bau- F. Baumeister ([email protected]) is Eppes Eminent Professor of meister, Twenge, and Nuss 2002), and distorts time perception Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306. Tyler F. Still- (Twenge, Catanese, and Baumeister 2002). Because of its man ([email protected]) is visiting professor, School of Business, pervasiveness and substantial implications for physical and Southern Utah University, Cedar City, UT 84720. Catherine D. Rawn psychological well-being, social exclusion has garnered much ([email protected]) is instructor, Department of Psychology, Univer- sity of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4. Kathleen D. Vohs attention from researchers across the social sciences. Yet, ([email protected]) is McKnight Presidential Fellow and associate professor, Marketing Department, Carlson School of Management, University of Min- and the JCR review team for their helpful input. The authors thank Destiny nesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455. Address correspondence to Nicole Mead. Bahl, Laura Husband, Katherine Lanzillo, Brooke Lerner, Elizabeth Mer- This work benefited from the support of a doctoral fellowship from the vis, Anthony Moore, and Joshua Warweg for their assistance with data Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC; collection. NM), grants MH-65559 and 1RL1AA017541 from the National Institutes John Deighton served as editor and Rebecca Ratner served as associate of Health (RB), and support from SSHRC, Canada Research Chair Council, editor for this article. and the McKnight Land-Grant Professorship and Presidential Funds (KV). The authors wish to thank Ron Faber, Tilburg University’s Consumer Club, Electronically published September 9, 2010 000 ᭧ 2010 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc. ● Vol. 37 ● February 2011 All rights reserved. 0093-5301/2011/3705-0011$10.00. DOI: 10.1086/656667 000 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH within the realm of consumer behavior, relatively little work toward an ingroup member after suffering rejection (Lakin has investigated the impact of social connection threats. Given et al. 2008). Ostracized individuals are more likely than the centrality of social relationships and consumption in daily nonostracized individuals to conform to the opinions of oth- life, this is an important imbalance to redress. ers (Williams, Cheung, and Choi 2000), although it is un- In the present investigation, we tested the hypothesis that clear whether this stems from increased passivity or desire socially excluded people spend and consume strategically for acceptance. Taken together, previous research suggests in the service of affiliation. Theoretically, this means that that the need to belong conforms to the broad pattern found excluded people will treat money and consumption as means among many motivations: when thwarted, people look for to an end, namely, the goal of affiliation, rather than as ends new ways to satisfy the need. Although previous research themselves. Our hypothesis was derived from research sug- supports the theory that threats to belongingness heighten gesting that people’s desire for social connection increases the motivation for social acceptance, it has relatively less when their need to belong has been threatened by exclusion to say about the strategies that excluded individuals use to or rejection (Lakin, Chartrand, and Arkin 2008; Maner et foster affiliation. In the current work, we propose that con- al. 2007). sumption and spending are important tools that excluded Of particular interest was the question of whether thwarted people use to help them on their quest for new social re- belongingness causes people to sacrifice money and personally lationships. favorable consumption for the sake of social inclusion. That is, socially excluded people might overspend, subvert personal Spending and Consumption as Affiliation Tools tastes and desires, or engage in risky consumption (e.g., use illicit drugs) insofar as doing so is perceived as socially lucra- Decades of research indicate that consumers use the tive. We examine boundary conditions implied by the logic symbolic nature of consumption as a way to communicate underlying our hypothesis to show that such consumption is information about themselves to others (Ball and Tasaki strategically designed to facilitate social connection. The pre- 1992; Belk 1988; Berger and Heath 2007; Escalas and sent investigation therefore sought not only to theoretically and Bettman 2003, 2005; Levy 1959; Muniz and O’Guinn empirically delineate how social exclusion affects consumption 2001; Ratner, Kahn, and Kahneman 1999; Richins 1994; decisions but also to illuminate important factors that determine Shavitt and Nelson 1999; for a review see Belk, Bahn, and when people will make personally harmful consumption de- Mayer [1982]). Such communication attempts are partic- cisions. ularly prevalent when people want to make a good im- pression on others or facilitate social interaction (Argo, White, and Dahl 2006; Berger and Rand 2008; Griskevi- CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK cius et al. 2007; Ratner and Kahn 2002; White and Dahl Evidence That Social Exclusion Heightens the 2006, 2007). For example, men avoided choosing a “la- Desire to Affiliate dies’ cut” steak when they made their steak selection in public, arguably because they wanted to present a manly Humans have an innate drive to be a part of social rela- self-image (White and Dahl 2006). Hence, self-presenta- tionships because a social group afforded survival and safety tional motives guide consumption decisions, and people throughout evolutionary history (Baumeister and Leary 1995; sometimes use consumption as a way to communicate spe- Buss and Kenrick 1998). In support of the assertion that social cific information about themselves to others. connections are a need, not just a desire, Baumeister and Our hypothesis that excluded people strategically con- Leary (1995) reviewed decades of research and concluded sume in the service of affiliation aligns with research fo- that people suffer psychologically and physically when they cusing on the communicative value of consumption, yet lack sufficient social ties. the manner in which people will consume to foster affil- Given the negative side effects associated with belong- iation is unclear. Previous research suggests two socially ingness deficits, it is perhaps not surprising that people have profitable routes. First, a sizable literature indicates that psychological mechanisms in place that help ensure that the motivation to make a good impression causes people their need to belong is being met. For example, people to consume in the hopes of differentiating themselves from continually monitor their level of inclusion (Leary et al. others, such as by signaling status or uniqueness (Ariely 1995) and automatically allocate attention to social op- and Levav 2000; Berger and Heath 2007; Griskevicius et portunities in the environment when inclusion drops below al. 2007; Ratner and Kahn 2002; Ratner et al. 1999; Snyder adesirablelevel(DeWall,Maner,andRouby2009;Gard- and Fromkin 1980). Hence, one prediction is that excluded ner, Pickett, and Brewer 2000). people try to boost their social appeal by spending in ways Accumulating research suggests that exclusion heightens that will make them seem different from others. people’s desire to form new social connections. Excluded Asecondpredictionisthatthedesireforassimilationover- people are cautiously eager to work and play with others, whelms the desire for differentiation when level of inclusion and they tend to view new sources