Attraction and Rejection 1 Running Head

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Attraction and Rejection 1 Running Head Attraction and Rejection 1 Running Head: ATTRACTION AND REJECTION Attraction and Rejection Eli J. Finkel Northwestern University Roy F. Baumeister Florida State University June 21, 2018 Word count: 10,575 To appear in E. J. Finkel, and R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Advanced social psychology: The state of the science. New York: Oxford University Press. Attraction and Rejection 2 Attraction and Rejection Human beings are social animals, and being social requires having relationships with others. We seek people to bond with, and we try to avoid rejection. But humans are more than just social—we are also cultural. Indeed, although other creatures, like ants and bees, are more social than humans, no other species is more cultural, nor even nearly as cultural, as we are (Baumeister, 2005). How do we, as social animals, leverage the opportunities afforded by our culture to help us meet our needs? We evolve and fine-tune sophisticated psychological machinery, including mind- reading skills and self-awareness, to determine with whom we would like to pursue a relationship (attraction) and to reduce the odds of rejection from our primary social groups (rejection). This chapter considers both of these social-psychological phenomena, starting with attraction. Attraction Historical Perspective Although theorizing on interpersonal attraction—the tendency to evaluate positively, and seek closeness with, a target person to whom we are not (yet) close—is millennia old, only a smattering of empirical investigations existed before the 1960s. Notable among them were studies on assortative mating (Harris, 1912), popularity (Moreno, 1934), power (Waller, 1938), mate preferences (Hill, 1945), sexuality (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948), and physical proximity (Festinger, Schachter, & Back, 1950). Such studies set the stage for the development of an organized subdiscipline of interpersonal attraction within social psychology. This subdiscipline coalesced in the 1960s and 1970s. Theodore Newcomb (1961) and Donn Byrne (1961) launched it with landmark publications investigating whether similarity predicts attraction. Shortly thereafter, scholars studied a broad range of attraction-relevant topics, including the effects of a target’s physical attractiveness (Walster, Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottman, 1966; Attraction and Rejection 3 Huston, 1973), the effects of a perceiver’s physiological arousal (Berscheid & Walster, 1974; Dutton & Aron, 1974), whether a target reciprocates liking from a perceiver (Walster, Walster, Piliavin, & Schmidt, 1973), whether a target who is “too perfect” is less likeable than an otherwise-identical target who has benign imperfections (Aronson, Willerman, & Floyd, 1966), and whether perceivers tend to be more attracted to a target who grows to like them over time than to a target who has liked them from the beginning (Aronson & Linder, 1965). Indeed, the empirical yield of attraction research was substantial enough to warrant a book, Interpersonal Attraction, which Ellen Berscheid and Elaine Walster published in 1969 and revised in 1978. Meanwhile, in the United States and other Western nations, the countercultural revolution was shattering long-held assumptions about gender roles and relationship dynamics, yielding skyrocketing divorce rates and widespread hand-wringing about the breakdown of traditional family life (Finkel, 2017). As a response, researchers across the social sciences redoubled their efforts to understand the contours of marriage and other long-term relationships, unearthing alarming findings regarding important topics like the rate of physical aggression within marriage (Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). Such developments altered social psychologists’ research priorities, strengthening their desire to understand what makes established relationships satisfying versus dissatisfying, stable versus unstable (Gable, this volume). As a result, starting in the 1980s, “the field of interpersonal attraction, as an organized literature, largely faded into the background, supplanted but not replaced by a field called ‘close relationships’” (Graziano & Bruce, 2008, p. 272; also see Berscheid, 1985; Reis, 2007). In the late 1980s and 1990s, a new, more evolutionarily grounded, approach to understanding interpersonal attraction emerged and gained prominence (Buss, 1989), filling the void left when the field of attraction research largely abdicated its perch within social psychology to close relationships research. The evolutionary approach leverages Darwin’s principles of natural and Attraction and Rejection 4 sexual selection (Maner & Kenrick, this volume) to derive novel hypotheses regarding when and how individuals experience attraction to a given target. In the twenty-first century, the broader field of interpersonal attraction has enjoyed a renaissance, surging alongside the still-robust field of evolutionary psychology. A major impetus behind this resurgence was the advent of technological and methodological developments in dating practices and social networking in the real world, including online dating, speed-dating, and social networking (Boyd & Ellison, 2008; Finkel & Eastwick, 2008; Finkel, Eastwick, Karney, Reis, & Sprecher, 2012). These technological developments have converged with the growth of evolutionary models to influence the types of attraction social psychologists study. In particular, although scholars have long investigated both romantic and nonromantic forms of attraction, the influence of online dating, speed-dating, and evolutionary perspectives have tilted the field increasingly toward romantic attraction in recent decades. The present chapter reflects that shift. Attraction Research Methods Social-psychological studies of close relationships typically involve the recruitment of research participants who are already involved in a close relationship—frequently a serious romantic relationship—and the studies address topics like how satisfying the relationship is and how likely it is to dissolve during a longitudinal follow-up period. Social-psychological studies of attraction, in contrast, typically involve the recruitment of research participants who report on a stranger, and the studies are about their attraction to him or her. Most such studies assess participants’ attraction to targets they have never met, or have just met for the first time. Some ask participants to report on a hypothetical partner, as when they report how important it is that a potential partner has a good sense of humor. Some ask participants to report on a specific target (or more than one), perhaps by showing them a photograph or profile of him or her. Although such studies enjoy high levels of experimental Attraction and Rejection 5 control, the extent to which the attraction-related processes they identify generalize to situations in which we actually meet a potential partner is often unknown. To address such concerns about generalizability while maintaining strong experimental control, scholars sometimes invest the resources required to introduce strangers to each other in a psychology laboratory or in another structured context. They might, for example, have participants interact with a research confederate. One influential method that seeks to maximize both external and internal validity is speed-dating (Finkel & Eastwick, 2008). In speed-dating studies, participants volunteer for an actual speed-dating event in which they meet around a dozen preferred-sex partners for interactions of, say, four minutes in duration. The value of speed-dating methods has come into especially sharp focus as concerns about replicability within social psychology have grown, because speed-dating events provide an efficient means of collecting high-impact data characterized by strong statistical power. For example, one study included eight heterosexual speed-dating events conducted over a three-day period, running 187 participants through a procedure that yielded more than 1,100 video-recorded speed-dates and 2,200 interaction-record questionnaires. Such data enable researchers to answer questions that are unanswerable with most methods, such as the extent to which David’s attraction to Kyoko is due to something about David (he’s attracted to all the women), something about Kyoko (all the men are attracted to her), or something about their unique chemistry (even after control for David’s attraction to the women in general and the men’s general attraction to Kyoko, he is uniquely attraction to her) (Joel, Eastwick, & Finkel, 2017; see Kenny and La Voie, 1984). When evaluating research methods, the most important consideration is the extent to which the methods are aligned with the research question. If a researcher wants to know what qualities people think they want in a hypothetical partner, simply asking participants what they want is sufficient. But if the researcher wants to know whether people exhibit greater attraction to partners Attraction and Rejection 6 who possess more of a given quality, the researcher is better served by observing how much attraction participants experience toward a real-life potential partner who possesses more versus less of that quality. Theory and Research on Attraction Recent methodological advances have emerged alongside notable, and desperately needed, theoretical advances. According to the original, 2010 version of this chapter, research on attraction is “a theoretical morass. Dozens of theories have guided research, and scholars have devoted little effort toward linking these far-flung theories into
Recommended publications
  • Social Psychology: a Very Short Introduction 1St Edition Pdf, Epub, Ebook
    SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY: A VERY SHORT INTRODUCTION 1ST EDITION PDF, EPUB, EBOOK Richard J Crisp | 9780198715511 | | | | | Social Psychology: A Very Short Introduction 1st edition PDF Book Shakespeare's Tragedies. Table of Contents. What scientific grounding do they have for their approach? Topics include persuasion, social influence, group influence, and prejudice. Video Transcript:. David Miller. For the avoidance of doubt, this paragraph does not form part of the public licenses. The Treaty of Versailles. More considerations for licensors. Philosophical Method. American Cultural History. All rights reserved. Jewish History. Condition: Used: Like New. Public users are able to search the site and view the abstracts and keywords for each book and chapter without a subscription. The Laws of Thermodynamics. Modern Drama. Ashley Jackson. Thinking and Reasoning. Contains some markings such as highlighting and writing. Artificial Intelligence. The Reagan Revolution. May Learn how and when to remove this template message. Steven W. Please subscribe or login to access full text content. Philosophy of Biology. Book is in Used-Good condition. Help Learn to edit Community portal Recent changes Upload file. Comparative Literature. Condition: Good. Publisher Washington State University. Offer from the Licensor — Licensed Material. Horror [ disambiguation needed ]. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. Receive an instructor-signed certificate with the institution's logo to verify your achievement and increase your job prospects. Kenneth Falconer. Except for the limited purpose of indicating that material is shared under a Creative Commons public license or as otherwise permitted by the Creative Commons policies published at creativecommons. What gets into our minds? British Politics. Subject Psychology. Social Psychology: A Very Short Introduction 1st edition Writer Roy Baumeister Professor.
    [Show full text]
  • Does Social Connection Turn Good Deeds Into Good Feelings? on the Value of Putting the 'Social' in Prosocial Spending
    Does Social Connection Turn Good Deeds into Good Feelings? On the Value of Putting the 'Social' in Prosocial Spending The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Aknin, Lara B., Elizabeth W. Dunn, Gillian M. Sandstrom, and Michael I. Norton. "Does Social Connection Turn Good Deeds into Good Feelings? On the Value of Putting the 'Social' in Prosocial Spending." International Journal of Happiness and Development 1, no. 2 (2013): 155–171. Published Version http://www.inderscience.com/info/inarticle.php?artid=55643 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:11148070 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Putting the “Social” in Prosocial Spending 1 Does Social Connection Turn Good Deeds into Good Feelings?: On the Value of Putting the "Social" in Prosocial Spending Lara B. Aknina, Elizabeth W. Dunnb, Gillian M. Sandstromb, & Michael I. Nortonc a Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, 8888 University Drive, Burnaby, British Columbia, V5A 1S6, email: [email protected] b Psychology Department, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T 1Z4, emails: [email protected], [email protected] c Marketing Unit, Harvard Business School, Soldiers Field Road, Boston, Massachusetts, USA 02163, email: [email protected] Putting the “Social” in Prosocial Spending 2 Abstract When are the emotional benefits of generous behavior most likely to emerge? In three studies, we demonstrate that the hedonic benefits of generous spending are most likely when spending promotes positive social connection.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Acceptance and Rejection: the Sweet and the Bitter
    Current Directions in Psychological Science Social Acceptance and Rejection: 20(4) 256 –260 © The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: The Sweet and the Bitter sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721411417545 http://cdps.sagepub.com C. Nathan DeWall1 and Brad J. Bushman2 1University of Kentucky and 2The Ohio State University and VU University, Amsterdam Abstract People have a fundamental need for positive and lasting relationships. In this article, we provide an overview of social psychological research on the topic of social acceptance and rejection. After defining these terms, we describe the need to belong and how it enabled early humans to fulfill their survival and reproductive goals. Next, we review research on the effects of social rejection on emotional, cognitive, behavioral, and biological responses. We also describe research on the neural correlates of social rejection. We offer a theoretical account to explain when and why social rejection produces desirable and undesirable outcomes. We then review evidence regarding how people cope with the pain of social rejection. We conclude by identifying factors associated with heightened and diminished responses to social rejection. Keywords social rejection, social exclusion, social acceptance, need to belong Deep down even the most hardened criminal is starving identify factors associated with heightened and diminished for the same thing that motivates the innocent baby: responses to social rejection. Love and acceptance. — Lily Fairchilde What Are Social Acceptance Hardened criminals may seem worlds apart from innocent and Social Rejection? babies. Yet, as the Fairchilde quote suggests, there is reason to Social acceptance means that other people signal that they believe that most people share a similar craving for social wish to include you in their groups and relationships (Leary, acceptance.
    [Show full text]
  • The Prevalence and Nature of Unrequited Love
    SGOXXX10.1177/2158244013492160SAGE OpenBringle et al. 492160research-article2013 Article SAGE Open April-June 2013: 1 –15 The Prevalence and Nature © The Author(s) 2013 DOI: 10.1177/2158244013492160 of Unrequited Love sgo.sagepub.com Robert G. Bringle1,2, Terri Winnick2,3 and Robert J. Rydell4 Abstract Unrequited love (UL) is unreciprocated love that causes yearning for more complete love. Five types of UL are delineated and conceptualized on a continuum from lower to greater levels of interdependence: crush on someone unavailable, crush on someone nearby, pursuing a love object, longing for a past lover, and an unequal love relationship. Study 1a found all types of UL relationships to be less emotionally intense than equal love and 4 times more frequent than equal love during a 2-year period. Study 1b found little evidence for limerent qualities of UL. Study 2 found all types of UL to be less intense than equal love on passion, sacrifice, dependency, commitment, and practical love, but more intense than equal love on turmoil. These results suggest that UL is not a good simulation of true romantic love, but an inferior approximation of that ideal. Keywords unrequited love, interdependence, love, friendship Although virtually all aspire to consummate romantic love, Baumeister et al., 1993). Interdependence encompasses the path toward achieving the ideal love relationship is lit- influence, behavioral control, and the frequency, diversity, tered with relationships that are incomplete approximations. and length of interaction (Berscheid, Snyder, & Omoto, Many of these are discarded, whereas some relationships are 2004). Berscheid and Ammazzalorso (2001) posited that maintained in spite of their imperfections.
    [Show full text]
  • Lovesickness” in Late Chos Ǒn Literature
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Reinterpreting “Lovesickness” in Late Chos ǒn Literature A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Asian Languages and Cultures by Janet Yoon-sun Lee 2014 © Copyright by Janet Yoon-sun Lee 2014 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Reinterpreting “Lovesickness” in Late Chos ǒn Literature By Janet Yoon-sun Lee Doctor of Philosophy in Asian Languages and Cultures University of California, Los Angeles, 2014 Professor Peter H. Lee, Chair My dissertation concerns the development of the literary motif of “lovesickness” (sangsa py ǒng ) in late Chos ǒn narratives. More specifically, it examines the correlation between the expression of feelings and the corporeal symptoms of lovesickness as represented in Chos ǒn romance narratives and medical texts, respectively, of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries. As the convergence of literary and medical discourse, lovesickness serves as a site to define both the psychological and physical experiences of love, implying the correlation between mind and body in the non-Western tradition. The analysis itself is re-categorized into the discussions of the feeling and the body. In the discussion of the feeling, it will be argued that the feeling of longing not only occupies an important position in literature, but also is gendered and structured in lyrics and narratives of the seventeenth century. In addressing the rubric of feelings of “longing,” this part seeks the ii theoretical grounds of how the intense experience of longing is converted to language of love and to bodily symptoms to constitute the knowledge of lovesickness. The second part concerns the representation of lovesick characters in Korean romance, particularly concerning the body politics of the Chos ŏn society.
    [Show full text]
  • Addiction NIDA Director Alall Lesllller Wallis to Illtroduce Cogllitive Scielltists to Drug Abuse Alld Addictioll Research
    i OBSERVE4:R Published by the American Psychological Society Vol. 10. No.4 July/August 1997 Art imitates life science... • Budgetary Politics ... In an editorial in The Chronicle of Higher On the Cover of a Magazine Education. APS looks at Advancement ofpsychological science is in vogue as behavioral science where science funding fits in strikes a pose on the cover of the nation's premier science journal the federal budget ............... 3 • Human CapiJallnitiative... cience magazine's June 6, 1997, cover looks a little APS and NSF sponsor unusual. Instead of the usual cancer cell stain, workshop to develop report on simulated 3-D protein molecule, or sun spots. the research opportunities ......... .4 S cover on this issue sports behavioral research! Portray­ • Health and Well-being... ing the science of behavioral genetics. the cover APS and SPSSI jointly sponsor represents a significant departure from Science's seminar at Smithsonian seemingly monotonic focus on things purely biology­ Institution ................. ... .... ..... 6 and physics-related. • Giving Away Psychology ... A time-lapse-like pose of two aging human New Observer feature beings, monozygotic twins to be specific (and a rare examines public health and occurrence it is indeed), graces the world-known tobacco farming ................. 12 magazine's facade. Not that a cover says every­ • Ninth Annual APS thing. But. sure enough. the magazine is wrapped Convention ... in behavioral science and psychologists are spilling Coverage of annual meeting out from between the issue's covers: APS Fellow Irving includes highlights of Gottesman explains in a PERSPECTIVES column some of the basics symposia, sessions, satellite --rS~E=E?S~c=m=N~C=E~O~N~P~AG~E~8 meetings, and more ...........
    [Show full text]
  • The Unrequited Love As Reflected in William Blake's “Love's Secret”
    1 THE UN5(4UITED L29( AS REF/ECTED IN WIL/IAM B/$.E‘S —L29(‘S SECRET“ Submitted by: Olivia Fergie Risthania 13020110110008 ABSTRACT The purpose of this final academic paper is to describe the love story which tells sadness in :illiam Blake‘s —Love's Secret“. The writer adopts Erich Fromm‘s theory of unrequited love from The Art of Loving. This final academic paper concerns intrinsic and extrinsic side of the poem. In the intrinsic side, the writer discusses about the existing diction and figurative language such as denotation and connotation and imagery to understand the true meaning of the poem. The extrinsic side, the writer discusses about unrequited love. The writer used library research, note-taking and internet browsing for collecting the data. The result is that love does not only bring happiness but also deep sadness for the speaker of the poem as unrequited love which is reflected in —/ove's Secret“. So, it succeeds in bringing the readers to feel what the speaker feels. Keywords: Love, Diction, Figurative Language, Unrequited Love, Deep Sadness 1. Introduction Poetry is universal because there are a lot of topics can be shown, for example: love, family, life, sadness, happiness, death, marriage and others. Those issues are the reflection of the ordinary life which the ordinary person is concerned. In all ages and in some countries, poetry has been regarded as one of important thing in human‘s existence. Love is an incredibly powerful word. This word is so easy to say, but it has a great meaning and feeling to every human being.
    [Show full text]
  • Bringing in a New Era in Character Education. INSTITUTION Stanford Univ., CA
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 476 547 EA 032 522 AUTHOR Damon, William, Ed. TITLE Bringing in a New Era in Character Education. INSTITUTION Stanford Univ., CA. Hoover Institution on War, Revolution, and Peace. ISBN ISBN-0-8179-2962-2 PUB DATE 2002 -00 -00 NOTE 211p. AVAILABLE FROM Hoover Institution Press, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305-6010 ($15). Tel: 650-723-1754; Tel: 877-466-8374 (Toll Free); Fax: 650-723-1687; e- mail:. [email protected]; Web site: http://www.hoover.org. PUB TYPE Books (010) Guides Non-Classroom (055) EDRS PRICE EDRS Price MF01/PC09 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Codes of Ethics; Consciousness Raising; Democratic Values; Educational Principles; Elementary Secondary Education; *Ethical Instruction; Ethics; Higher Education; Integrity; Moral Issues; *Moral Values; Personality Development; Social Values; Student Development; *Values Clarification; *Values Education IDENTIFIERS Character Development ABSTRACT The nine essays in this book present perspectives on what is needed in character education from kindergarten to college. Two main themes run through the volume. The first is a consensus among the authors that fundamental moral standards must be passed along to the young and that educators at all levels bear the obligation to transmit these core standards to their studehts. The second is a shared determination to discard old oppositions that have paralyzed some of the best efforts in character education over the past several decades. The essays are titled as follows: "Transmitting Moral Wisdom in an Age of the Autonomous Self" (Arthur J. Schwartz); "How Moral Education Is Finding It8 Way Back into American's Schools" (Christina Hoff Sommers); "The Science of Character Education" (Marvin W.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Connection and Compassion: Important Predictors of Health and Well-Being
    6RFLDO&RQQHFWLRQDQG&RPSDVVLRQ,PSRUWDQW3UHGLFWRUV RI+HDOWKDQG:HOO%HLQJ Emma Seppala, Timothy Rossomando, James R. Doty Social Research: An International Quarterly, Volume 80, Number 2, Summer 2013, pp. 411-430 (Article) 3XEOLVKHGE\7KH-RKQV+RSNLQV8QLYHUVLW\3UHVV DOI: 10.1353/sor.2013.0027 For additional information about this article http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/sor/summary/v080/80.2.seppala.html Access provided by Stanford University (15 Jun 2015 23:56 GMT) Emma Seppala, Timothy Rossomando, and James R. Doty Social Connection and Compassion: Important Predictors of Health and Well-Being Being unwanted, unloved, uncared for, forgotten by everybody—I think that is a much greater hunger, a much greater poverty than the person who has nothing to eat. —Mother Theresa (Costello 2008,14) Communities, which included the greatest number of the most sympathetic members, would flourish best, and rear the greatest number of offspring. —(Darwin 1871,130). Love and compassion are necessities, not luxuries. Without them, humanity cannot survive. —His Holiness the XlVth Dalai Lama (Ramaswamy 2006, 148) From the dawn of modern psychology, psychological theorists have emphasized the importance of positive human social connection for health, well-being, and survival. Both early and modern psychologists social research Vol. 80 : No. 2 : Summer 2013 411 have argued that social connection—that is, the development of posi­ tive relationships with others in the social world—is a primary psycho­ logical need and motivator essential for human development and survival (Baumeister and Leary 1995,499; Hart, Shaver, and Goldenberg 2005, 1000; Maslow 1943, 375). Indeed, several decades of research on social connection now confirm that it is linked to a substantial number of psychological and physical health benefits as well as longevity (Berkman and Syme 1979, 201-202; Cacioppo et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Steps Toward the Ripening of Relationship Science
    Personal Relationships, 14 (2007), 1–23. Printed in the United States of America. Copyright Ó 2007 IARR. 1350-4126=07 DISTINGUISHED SCHOLAR ARTICLE Steps toward the ripening of relationship science HARRY T. REIS The University of Rochester Harry T. Reis is professor of psychology in the Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology at the Uni- versity of Rochester. He first studied human relationships at the City College of New York, where he received a B.S. in 1970. Reis subsequently received a Ph.D. from New York University in 1975. A former president of the International Society for the Study of Personal Relationships (2000– 2001), he served as editor of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: Interpersonal and Group Processes (1986–1990) and is currently editor of Current Directions in Psychological Science. After 10 years as its executive offi- cer (1995–2004), the Society for Personality and Social Psychology awarded him the Distinguished Service Award (2006) and elected him as president (2007). Reis served as member or Chair of the American Psychological Associa- tion’s Board of Scientific Affairs from 2001 through 2003, is a Fellow of the American Psychological Association and of the Association for Psychological Science, and was a Ful- bright Senior Research Fellow in the Netherlands in 1991. Reis’s research, which concerns mechanisms that regulate interpersonal processes, has been funded by the National Science Foundation, the National Institute of Mental Health, and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Abstract Recent decades have seen remarkable growth in research and theorizing about relationships. E.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereotypes Can Be Learned Through Implicit Associations Or Explicit Rules
    Stereotypes Can Be Learned through Implicit Associations or Explicit Rules by Anthony Michael Pascoe Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Duke University Date: ____________________ Approved: _________________________________ Wendy Wood, Committee Co-Supervisor _________________________________ Mark Leary, Committee Co-Supervisor _____________________________ Harris Cooper ____________________________ James Bettman Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience in the Graduate School of Duke University 2011 ABSTRACT Stereotypes Can Be Learned through Implicit Associations or Explicit Rules by Anthony Michael Pascoe Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Duke University Date: ____________________ Approved: _________________________________ Wendy Wood, Committee Co-Supervisor _________________________________ Mark Leary, Committee Co-Supervisor _____________________________ Harris Cooper ____________________________ James Bettman An abstract of a dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience in the Graduate School of Duke University 2011 Copyright by Anthony Michael Pascoe 2011 Abstract Two studies examined whether stereotypes can be created using different learning paradigms and whether the resulting stereotypes will have different properties that affect their activation, suppression, and explicit knowledge. In the Pilot
    [Show full text]
  • The Art of Thinking Clearly (577H) Sources.Indd
    THE ART OF THINKING CLEARLY A NOTE ON SOURCES Hundreds of studies have been conducted on the vast majority of cognitive and behav- ioural errors. In a scholarly work, the complete reference section would easily double the pages of this book. I have focused on the most important quotes, technical refer- ences, recommendations for further reading and comments. The knowledge encompassed in this book is based on the research carried out in the fields of cognitive and social psychology over the past three decades. SURVIVORSHIP BIAS Survivorship bias in funds and stock market indices, see: Edwin J. Elton, Martin J. Gruber & Christopher R. Blake, ‘Survivorship Bias and Mutual Fund Performance’, The Review of Financial Studies, volume 9, number 4, 1996, 1097–1120. Statistically relevant results by coincidence (self-selection), see: John P. A. Ioan- nidis, ‘Why Most Published Research Findings Are False’, PLoS Med 2(8), 2005, e124. SWIMMER’S BODY ILLUSION Nassim Nicholas Taleb, The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable (New York: Random House, 2007), 109–110. ‘Ideally, the comparison should be made between people who went to Harvard and people who were admitted to Harvard but chose instead to go to Podunk State. Unfortunately, this is likely to produce samples too small for statistical analysis.’ Thomas Sowell, Economic Facts and Fallacies (New York: Basic Books, 2008), 106. David Lykken & Auke Tellegen, ‘Happiness Is a Stochastic Phenomenon’, Psycho- logical Science, Vol.7, No. 3, May 1996, 189. In his book Good to Great, Jim Collins cites the CEO of Pitney Bowes, Dave Nassef: ‘I used to be in the Marines, and the Marines get a lot of credit for building people’s values.
    [Show full text]