Actualité des Universaux musicaux

Topics in Universals in Music

Actualité des Universaux musicaux

Topics in Universals in Music

Sous la direction de : Jean-Luc Leroy

Copyright © 2012 Éditions des archives contemporaines

Tous droits de traduction, de reproduction et d’adaptation réservés pour tous pays. Toute reproduction ou représentation intégrale ou partielle, par quelque procédé que ce soit (électronique, mécanique, photocopie, enregistrement, quelque système de stockage et de récupération d’information) des pages publiées dans le présent ouvrage faite sans autorisation écrite de l’éditeur, est interdite.

Éditions des archives contemporaines 41, rue Barrault 75013 Paris (France)

www.archivescontemporaines.com

ISBN : 9782813000613

Avertissement : Les textes publiés dans ce volume n’engagent que la responsabilité de leurs auteurs. Pour faciliter la lecture, la mise en pages a été harmonisée, mais la spécificité de chacun, dans le système des titres, le choix de transcriptions et des abréviations, l’emploi de majuscules, la présentation des références bibliographiques, etc. a été le plus souvent conservée.

6. Musical universals: Perspectives from infancy

Sandra E. TREHUB

The search for musical universals continues to be inspired by the pervasive belief in language universals and their innate basis (e.g. Pinker, 1994). However, there are increasing challenges to the notion of language universals and to Universal Grammar, in particular. Evans and Levinson (2009) contend that misconceptions about cross- linguistic similarity have arisen from the consideration of a relatively small set of English-like languages. They point out that linguistic typologists have documented important differences in sounds, meaning, and syntax in the thousands of extant languages worldwide. Accordingly, they argue that cognitive scientists should be attempting to account for language diversity rather than similarity. Other scholars, including Christensen and Chater (2008), are challenging prevailing notions about the domain-specific biological factors that are presumed to underlie language structure and acquisition. They conceive of language as a complex cultural product that has emerged in response to human social needs. Syal and Finlay (2011) also consider social motivation as critical for the evolution and acquisition of language. In the case of music, there is general acknowledgement of a universal capacity or disposition for music but much less attention to the diverse ways of expressing that musicality across cultures (Blacking, 1995). Even the boundary between musical and non-musical behavior (i.e. what is music and what is not) differs across cultures (Nettl, 2005). In any case, the musical universals tentatively identified to date, including octave equivalence, five to seven notes per octave, unequal-step scales, temporal regularity, and repetition (Dowling & Harwood, 1984; Sloboda, 1985), are very basic as well as being derived primarily from relatively recent forms of music. Moreover, these universals focus largely on musical products rather than on behaviors, processes, or functions.

1. Musical universals in developmental perspective If there is a universal disposition for music, some signs of that disposition should be evident in early childhood. In fact, pre-verbal infants are surprisingly capable music listeners (Trehub & Hannon, 2006). For example, they detect subtle differences in musical pitch and timing (Trehub & Hannon, 2009) as well as global pitch and rhythm patterns (Hannon & Trehub, 2005a; Trehub, Thorpe, & Morrongiello, 1987a; Trehub & Thorpe, 1989). Moreover, they exhibit long-term memory for music heard regularly 6. Musical universals: Perspectives from infancy 6

(Saffran, Loman, & Robertson, 2000; Trainor, Wu, & Tsang, 2004), and they retain more detail from vocal than from instrumental renditions (Volkova, Trehub, & Schellenberg, 2006). By their first birthday, if not before, infants show sensitivity to culture-specific regularities in the music around them (Hannon & Trehub, 2005b; Soley & Hannon, 2010). Universal disposition for musical parenting Throughout the world music plays an important role in cultural rituals (Merker, 2009), fostering communal identity or goals, and regulating emotion or arousal (Blacking, 1995; Trehub, Hannon, & Schachner, 2010). A cross-cultural disposition for musical parenting is also evident. Caregivers everywhere soothe or amuse their infants with a musical repertoire consisting of lullabies and play songs (Trehub & Trainor, 1998). Lullabies are readily recognizable as such. For example, when naïve listeners hear pairs of foreign lullabies and non-lullabies matched on culture of origin and tempo, they easily identify the lullabies (Trehub, Unyk, & Trainor, 1993a), perhaps on the basis of their simplicity or repetitiveness (Unyk, Trehub, Trainor, & Schellenberg, 1992). What is remarkable is that listeners are equally successful at identifying lullabies from familiar musical cultures (i.e. Western European tradition) as from unfamiliar cultures (Trehub et al., 1993a). Although play songs also have a distinctive form, they are especially notable for their distinctive performances in infant caregiving contexts (Nakata & Trehub, 2011; Trehub et al., 1997). When naïve listeners hear paired samples of the same song by the same singer, one performed for an infant, the other performed informally with no audience, they readily identify the infant-directed version (Trainor, 1996; Trehub et al., 1997), even when the language and culture are unfamiliar (Trehub, Unyk, & Trainor, 1993b). Listeners seem to base their judgments primarily on the singer’s vocal tone rather than on the measurable differences in pitch level (higher) and tempo (slower) for infant-directed versions (Trainor, Clark, Huntley, & Adams, 1997; Trehub et al., 1997). Performances for infants also have more timing regularity and greater expressive variations in dynamics than typical informal performances of the same songs (Nakata & Trehub, 2011). Mothers do not seem to have conscious didactic goals when they sing to pre-verbal infants, but they intuitively highlight the structure of the music, especially its timing and pitch contours. Musical elements are also prominent in mothers’ speech to pre-verbal infants. For example, maternal speech involves a greatly expanded pitch range, exaggerated pitch contours, and considerable repetition (Fernald, 1991). There are similar pitch contours in mothers’ speech across cultures (Fernald et al., 1989), but the pitch intervals are individually distinctive (Bergeson & Trehub, 2007). These signature tunes may facilitate maternal voice recognition. Similarly, despite global similarities in some aspects of maternal singing, it is individually distinctive. In fact, maternal performances of the same song on different occasions are virtually identical in pitch level and tempo provided the infant’s mood is comparable (Bergeson & Trehub, 2002). As would be expected, mothers fine-tune 6. Musical universals: Perspectives from infancy 7 their performances to infants’ mood or state of arousal. Maternal affect or arousal also affects the nature of these performances. For example, maternal singing is more expressive when mother and infant can see one another than when they are separated by an opaque curtain (Trehub, Plantinga, & Russo, 2011, March). Heightened expressiveness in the face-to-face context cannot be attributed to mothers’ dependence on infant feedback because video feedback from the infant fails to close the expressiveness gap. Although spoken and sung interactions with infants are typically multimodal, featuring gestures, touch, and movement, researchers’ attention has focused largely on the acoustic features. What has escaped attention is that the visible components of maternal speech and singing differ substantially. For example, mothers smile and move considerably more when they sing than when they talk to infants (Plantinga, Trehub, & Russo, 2011, June). In fact, singing mothers commonly move in time with the music and smile almost continuously. 1.2. Universal responsiveness to musical parenting Infants are highly receptive to singing in the maternal style, as they are to speech in the maternal style (Cooper & Aslin, 1994; Fernald, 1985). For example, infants listen significantly longer to infant-directed singing than to non-infant-directed singing (Trainor, 1996), even as newborns (Masataka, 1999). When presented with audio- visual renditions of maternal speech and singing, infants are substantially more engaged by the singing than by the speech (Nakata & Trehub, 2004). Live maternal singing also modulates infant arousal, as reflected in changes in cortisol concentrations in their saliva (Shenfield, Trehub, & Nakata, 2003). What is the essence of maternal singing for the infant audience? The simple, repetitive structure of lullabies and play songs is likely to make some contribution. However, expressive voice quality, facial gestures, touch, and movement seem to make a much greater contribution. The various vocal and non-vocal features are combined to yield performances that are irresistible to infants.

2. Implications The available evidence is consistent with a human disposition for music listening and learning (Trehub et al., 2010), which may capitalize on our capacity for vocal and bodily imitation (Merker, 2009) and on our intensely social nature (Christensen & Chater, 2008; Syal & Finlay, 2011). The cross-species disposition to care for infants seems to include, in the human case, musical care. Although mothers are highly selective in the music they provide for infants, they seem more focused on their performances. In that light, it is interesting that the search for musical universals, with some notable exceptions (e.g. Lomax, 1976b), has focused largely on musical form 6. Musical universals: Perspectives from infancy 8 rather than on style and context. A renewed focus on music from the oral tradition, as performed in context, might yield fruitful perspectives on musical universals.1

1 Acknowledgements − Preparation of this paper was assisted by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.

Abstracts / Résumés

6. Sandra E. TREHUB Musical universals: Perspectives from infancy The search for musical universals has been inspired, to a considerable extent, by the apparent presence of language universals and the related presumption that some aspects of linguistic knowledge are innate. To date, however, scholars of music have failed to provide a convincing set of musical universals. It may comfort such musical scholars to know that the consensus on language universals is rapidly evaporating. For example, Evans and Levin (2009) argue that the 6000–8000 extant languages differ radically in their sound composition, meanings, and syntax, so the challenge of cognitive science is to account for linguistic diversity. Others have raised questions about the domain-specific biological endowment that is thought to underlie language structure (Christensen & Chater, 2008) and the acquisition of language (Chater, Reali, & Christensen, 2009). Instead of considering languages as being shaped by the human brain, these authors contend that languages are shaped and re-shaped by language users and learners. Similar considerations undoubtedly apply to musical systems across cultures. Nevertheless, there are important differences, which are elaborated in the paper. According to Merker (2009), music is a critically important human ritual, which has its biological basis in our capacity and motivation for vocal and bodily mimesis. Although mimesis is not a uniquely human skill, our skill level greatly outstrips that of any other species. Developmental and cross-cultural similarities and differences in the form and function of musical rituals are considered. Les universaux musicaux : perspectives à partir de la petite enfance La recherche d’universaux musicaux s’est inspirée, dans une large mesure, de l’apparente présence d’universaux du langage et de la présomption qui lui est liée que des aspects de la connaissance linguistique sont innés. À ce jour cependant, les spécialistes de la musique n’ont pas réussi à fournir un ensemble d’universaux musicaux convaincants. De tels spécialistes pourraient être réconfortés de savoir que le consensus sur les universaux du langage s’est rapidement évaporé. Par exemple, Evans et Levin (2009) soutiennent que les 6000 à 8000 langues existantes diffèrent radicalement dans leur composition sonore, leurs significations et leur syntaxe, de telle sorte que le défi des sciences cognitives est de rendre compte de la diversité linguistique. D’autres ont soulevé les questions du fondement biologique susceptible Abstracts / Résumés10 de sous-tendre la structure du langage (Christensen & Chater, 2008) et de l’acquisition du langage (Chater, Reali, & Christensen, 2009). Au lieu de considérer les langues comme formées par le cerveau humain, ces auteurs soutiennent que les langues sont formées et re-formées par les usagers et les apprenants. De telles considérations s’appliquent sans aucun doute aux systèmes musicaux à travers les cultures. Néanmoins, d’importantes différences existent, qui sont détaillées dans la contribution. Selon Merker (2009), la musique est d’une importance critique pour le rituel humain, qui a son fondement biologique dans notre capacité et notre motivation pour la mimesis vocale et corporelle. Bien que la mimesis ne soit pas une compétence uniquement humaine, notre niveau de compétence dépasse largement ceux des autres espèces. Les similitudes développementales et transculturelles et les différences dans la forme et la fonction des rituels musicaux sont examinées.

References

Abrahams, R. (Ed.) (1969). Jump-rope Rhymes: A dictionary. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Addessi, A. R. (2000). Claude Debussy et Manuel de Falla. Un caso di influenza stilistica. Bologna, Italia: CLUEB. Agawu, K. (1991). Playing with signs: A semiotic interpretation of classic music. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Aitken, K. J. & Trevarthen, C. (2003). L’organisation soi/autrui dans le développement psychologique humain. La psychiatrie de l’enfant, XLVI(2), 471-520. Altenmüller, E. O. (2003). How Many Music Centres Are in the Brain? In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The Cognitive (p. 346-353). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Aristotle (1985). Metaphysics (M. Furth, trans.). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing. Arleo, A. (1988). Formulettes d’élimination recueillies à Saint-Nazaire, en Brière et dans la Presqu’île guérandaise. Nantes, France : Centre d’Études Métriques-Université de Nantes. Arleo, A. (2001). The Saga of Susie: The Dynamics of an International Handclapping Game. In J. C. Bishop & M. Curtis (Eds.), Play for Today in the Primary School Playground: Life, learning and creativity (p. 115-132). Buckingham, UK: Open University Press. Arleo, A. (2006). Do children’s rhymes reveal universal metrical patterns? In P. Hunt (Ed.), Children’s Literature: Critical Concepts in Literary and Cultural Studies, Vol. IV (p. 39-56). London, UK: Routledge. Arleo, A. (2009). Pif Paf Poof: Ablaut reduplication in children’s counting-out rhymes. In J. L. Aroui & A. Arleo (Eds.), Towards a Typology of Poetic Forms: From linguistic structure to metrics and beyond (p. 307-323). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Arleo, A. (forthcoming). Concepts darwiniens et l’étude de la tradition orale des enfants: métaphore ou modèle ? Actes du colloque international Charles Darwin’s Legacy in European Cultures, Université de Nantes, France, 3-4 avril 2009. Arleo, A. & Flament, B. (1990). Approche rythmique et mélodique de la réalisation de la séquence finale dans 7 comptines françaises à la lumière d’une analyse mingographique. Nantes, France : Centre d’Études Métriques-Université de Nantes. Arleo, A. & Mettouchi A. (2010). « J’ai perdu ma fille » : étude d’un jeu chanté par des écolières berbères. Dans A. Arleo et J. Delalande (dir.), Cultures enfantines : universalité et diversité (p. 107-120). Rennes, France : Presses Universitaires de Rennes. Arom, S. (1999). African Polyphony and Polyrhythm: Musical structure and methodology (M. Thom et al., trans.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. References12

Arom, S. (2000). Prolegomena to a . In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 27-29). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Auner, J. (2003). A Schoenberg Reader: Documents of a Life. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Ayari, M. & McAdams, S. (2003). Aural Analysis of Arabic Improvised Instrumental Music (Taqsm). , 21(2), 159-216. Ayotte, J., Peretz, I., & Hyde, K. (2002). Congenital : A group study of adults afflicted with a music-specific disorder. Brain, 125, 238-251. Bachelard, G. (1936). La dialectique de la durée. Paris, France : Boivin & Cie. Bachmann, M.-L. (1984). La rythmique Jaques-Dalcroze : une éducation par la musique et pour la musique. Neuchâtel, Suisse : La Baconnière. Bailey, B. A. & Davidson, J. W. (2002). Adaptive characteristics of group singing: Perceptions from members of a choir for homeless men. Musicae Scientiae, VI(2), 221-252. Bakagiannis, S. & Tarrant, M. (2006). Can music bring people together? Effects of shared musical preference on intergroup bias in adolescents. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 129-136. Balkwill, L.-L. (1997). Perception of emotion in music: A cross-cultural investigation. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, York University, Toronto, Canada. Balkwill, L.-L. & Thompson, W. (1999). A cross-cultural investigation of the perception of emotion in music: Psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Perception, 17(1), 43-64. Balpe, J.-P. (2006). Quelques concepts de l’art numérique. Site du groupe « Transitoire Observable » . Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Baroni, M. (1993). Linguaggio musicale e valori sociali. In M. Imberty, M. Baroni, & G. Porzionato (Eds.), Memoria musicale e valori sociali. Metodi d’indagine e aspetti educativi (p. 33-71). Milano, Italia: Ricordi. Baroni, M. (2010). GTTM and post-tonal music. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 5, 69-93. Baroni, M. & Callegari, L. (Eds.) (1984). Musical grammars and computer analysis. Firenze, Italia: Olschki. Baroni, M. & Dalmonte, R. (2009). Per una grammatica dello stile: considerazioni sulle arie da camera del Seicento italiano. Rivista di Analisi e Teoria musicale, 15(2), 127-162. Baroni, M., Dalmonte, R., & Jacoboni, C. (1999). Le regole della musica. Indagine sui meccanismi della comunicazione. Torino, Italia: Edizioni di Torino. (English trans.: A computer-aided inquiry on music communication. The rules of music. Lewiston, NY: The Edwin Mellen Press, 2003; French trans.: Les règles de la musique. Étude sur les mécanismes de la communication. Sampzon, France : Delatour, 2007.) Baroni, M. & Jacoboni, C. (1978). Proposal for a grammar of melody. Montréal, Canada: Les Presses de l’Université de Montréal. Bartlett, J. (1984). Cognition of complex events: Visual scenes and music. In W. R. Crozier & A. J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive Processes in the Perception of Art (p. 225-251). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland. Bastien, C. (1997). Les connaissances de l’enfant à l’adulte. Paris, France : Armand Colin. Beck, R. J., Cesario, T. C., Yousefi, A., & Enamoto, H. (2000). Choral singing, performance perception, and immune system changes in salivary immunoglobulin A and cortisol. Music Perception, 18(1), 87-106. References13

Becker, J. (2001). Anthropological perspectives on . In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research (p. 135-160). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of Culture. New York, NY: Houghton Miflin Company. Bennet-Clarke, H. (2002). Le chant sexuel des cigales. Pour la Science, Hors-série 34, 10-13. Bent, I. & Drabkin, W. (1987). Analysis. London, UK: The Macmillan Press. (A. Cœurdevey et J. Tabouret, trad. : L’Analyse musicale. Histoire et Méthodes. Nice, France : Éditons Main d’Œuvre, 1998.) Bentley, A. (1983). Musikalische Begabung bei Kindern und ihre Messbarkeit (R. Jakoby, trans.). Berlin, Deutschland: Verlag Moritz Diesterweg. (Original published in 1966.) Bergeson, T. R. & Trehub, S. E. (2002). and tempo in mothers’ songs to infants. Psychological Science, 13(1), 72-75. Bergeson, T. R. & Trehub, S. E. (2007). Signature tunes in mothers’ speech to infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 648-654. Bergson, H. (1898). Essai sur les données immédiates de la conscience. Paris, France : Félix Alcan. Berlin, B. & Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Berry, W. (1986). Form in Music (2d ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bhattacharya, J. & Petsche, H. (2001). Musicians and the gamma band: A secret affair? NeuroReport, 12(2), 371-374. Bispham, J. (2009-2010). Music’s “design features”: Musical motivation, musical pulse, and musical pitch. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 41-61. Blacking, J. (1973). How Musical is man? Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. Blacking, J. (1975). Le sens musical. Paris, France : Éditions de Minuit (Original book: How musical is man?, 1973.) Blacking, J. (1977). Les Universaux en musique, peut-on les entendre ? The World of Music, XIX, 23-29. Blacking, J. (1984). What languages do musical language describe? In M. Baroni & L. Callegari, Musical grammars and computer analysis (p. 363-370). Firenze, Italia: Olschki. Blacking, J. (1995). Music, Culture, and Experience. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Blondel, M. & Miller, C. (2009). Symmetry and children’s poetry in sign languages. In J.-L. Aroui & A. Arleo (Eds.), Towards a Typology of Poetic Forms: From linguistic structure to metrics and beyond (p. 143-163). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Blood, A. J. & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11818-11823. Bodner, E. & Gilboa, A. (2009). On the power of music to affect intergroup relations. Musicae Scientiae, XIII(1), 85-112. Bonini-Baraldi, F. (2010). L’émotion en partage (musiciens de Transylvanie). Thèse de Doctorat, Université de Paris 10-La Défense, Nanterre, France. Boulez, P. & Cage, J. (1990). Correspondance et documents (J.-J. Nattiez, éd.). Winterthur, Suisse : Amadeus. Bourdieu, P. (1990). The Logic of the Practice (R. Nice, trans.). Cambridge, UK: Polity Press. References14

Brailoiu, C. (1956/1984). Children’s rhythms. In L. Lloyd (Ed. and trans.), Problems of (p. 206-238). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (Problèmes d’ethnomusicologie [textes réunis et présentés par G. Rouget]. Genève : Minkoff, 1973.) Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Bréhier, E. (1967). Histoire de la philosophie, Tome I : L’Antiquité et le Moyen Âge, Fascicule 3 : Moyen Âge et Renaissance (7e éd.). Paris, France : P.U.F. Brown, D. (1991). Human Universals. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Brown, S. (2000). The “Musilanguage” Model of Music Evolution. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 271-300). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Brown, S. (2007). Contagious heterophony: A new theory about the origins of music. Musicae Scientiae, 11(1), 3-26. Brown, S., Merker, B., & Wallin, N. L. (2000). An Introduction to Evolutionary . In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 3-24). The MIT Press. Brown, S. & Volgsten, U. (Eds.) (2005). Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music. New York, NY: Berghahn Books. Buchet, E. (1966/1995). Beethoven : légendes et vérité. Paris, France : Buchet/Chastel. Burling, R. (1966). The metrics of children’s verse: A cross-linguistic study. American Anthropologist, 68, 1418-1441. Bystrina, I. (1983). Kodes und Kodewandel. Zeitschrift für Semiotik, 5, 1-22. Canguilhem, G. (1965). La connaissance de la vie. Paris, France : Vrin, 2009. Cariani, P. (1989). On the design of devices with emergent semantic functions. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, State University of New York, New York, NY. Carterette, E. C. & Kendall, R. A. (1999). Comparative music perception and cognition. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (p. 725-791). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Chailley, J. (1954-1955). Formation et transformation du langage musical, 1 : Intervalles et échelles. Paris, France : Les Cours de Sorbonne. Chailley, J. (1985). Éléments de philologie musicale. Paris, France : Alphonse Leduc. Changeux, J.-P. (1983). L’Homme neuronal. Paris, France : Fayard. Chauvin-Payan, C. (2010). Universaux spatiaux et gestuels dans les jeux de tape-mains. Dans A. Arleo et J. Delalande (dir.), Cultures enfantines : universalité et diversité (p. 137-158). Rennes, France : Presses Universitaires de Rennes. Chávez, C. (1961). Musical Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cheng, A. (1997). La pensée chinoise. Paris, France : Éditions du Seuil. Cheng, E., Ockelford, A., & Welch, G. (2009). Researching and developing music provision in special schools in England for children and young people with complex needs. Australian Journal of , 2, 27-48. Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Christensen, M. H. & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 489-509. Clarke, E. C. (2005). Ways of Listening: An Ecological Approach to the Perception of Musical Meaning. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. References15

Clayton, M. (2009). The social and personal functions of music in cross-cultural perspective. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of (p. 35- 44). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Clayton, M., Herbert, T., & Middleton, R. (2003). The cultural study of music: A critical introduction. New York, NY: Routledge. Cohen, A. J., Thorpe, L. A., & Trehub, S. E. (1987). Infants’ Perception of Musical Relations in Short Transposed Tone Sequences. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 33-47. Cohen, J. E. (1962). Information theory and music. Behavioral Science, 7(2), 137-163. Cook, N. (2001). Theorizing musical meaning. Music Theory Spectrum, 23(2), 170-195. Cooper, R. P. & Aslin, R. A. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65, 1663-1677. Cope, D. (1996). Experiments in musical intelligence. Madison, WI: A-R Editions. Cornulier (de), B. (2009). Minimal chronometric forms: On the durational metrics of 2-2- stroke groups. In J.-L. Aroui & A. Arleo (Eds.), Towards a Typology of Poetic Forms: From linguistic structure to metrics and beyond (p. 123-141). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Corpataux, F. (2010). Jeux chantés sur trois continents : approche comparative. Dans A. Arleo et J. Delalande (dir.), Cultures enfantines : universalité et diversité (p. 97-106). Rennes, France : Presses Universitaires de Rennes. Costa-Giomi, E. (2010, March). Look at who’s singing!: ‘Infants’ and ‘adults’ perception of timbre-melody associations. Paper presented at the International Conference in Infant Studies, Baltimore, MD. Costa-Giomi, E. (2011a, Feb). Infants recognize their mothers’ singing voice. Paper presented at the Texas Music Educators Association, San Antonio, TX. Costa-Giomi, E. (2011b, Feb.). Infant discriminate female voices in speech but not music. Paper presented at the Texas Music Educators Association, San Antonio, TX. Costa-Giomi, E., Cohen, L., Solan, D., & Brock, A. (under review). Infant discrimination and categorization of timbre and melody. Costa-Giomi, E. & Davila, Y. (2010, July). Infant discrimination of female voices. Paper presented at the International Society for Music Education, Research Commission Meeting, Changchun, China. Cox, A. (2001). The Mimetic Hypothesis and Embodied Musical Meaning. Musicae Scientiae, 5(2), p. 195-212. Cross, I. (2006). The Origins of Music: Some Stipulations on Theory. Music perception, 24(1), 79-81. Cross, I. (2009-2010). The evolutionary nature of musical meaning. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 179-197. (A. Tognetti, trad. : La nature évolutionnaire de la signification musicale. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 157- 169]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Cross, I. & Morley, I. (2008). The evolution of music: Theories, definitions and the nature of the evidence. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality (p. 61-82). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Crowder, R. (1985). Perception of the major/minor distinction: III. Hedonic, musical, and affective discriminations. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23(4), 314-316. References16

Dale, N. & Salt, A. (2007). Early support developmental journal for children with visual impairment: The case for a new developmental framework for early intervention. Child: Care, Health and Development, 33(6), 684-690. Damasio, A. (2000). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. London, UK: Vintage. Damasio, A. (2010). L’autre moi-même. Paris, France : Odile Jacob. Da Silva Neves, R. (1999). Psychologie cognitive. Paris, France : Armand Colin. Davidson, P. A. (2004). Turbulence: An Introduction for Scientists and Engineers. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Davidson, R. (1994). On emotion, mood, and related affective constructs. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions (p. 51-55). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Davies, J. B. (1978). The psychology of music. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. New York, NE: Oxford University Press. DeCasper, A. & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176. Delalande, F. (1996). Les Unités Sémiotiques Temporelles : Problématique et essai de définition. Dans L. M. Marseille (dir.), Les Unités Sémiotiques Temporelles : éléments nouveaux d’analyse musicale (p. 16-25). Marseille, France : Laboratoire Musique et Informatique de Marseille. Delavenne, A. (2006). Vers une approche musicale de l’organisation temporelle des interactions vocales précoces. Mémoire de fin d’études, Université Paris X, Nanterre, France. Delavenne, A., Gratier, M., Devouche, E., & Apter, G. (2010). Temps et phrasé fragmenté des interactions entre des mères présentant un trouble de la personnalité et leurs nourrissons à trois mois. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig (dir.), Musique et Évolution (p. 141-153). Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga. Deliège, C. (1984). Les fondements de la musique tonale. Paris, France : J.-C. Lattès. Deliège, C. (2003). Cinquante ans de modernité musicale : de Darmstadt à l’IRCAM. Contribution historiographique à une musicologie critique. Sprimont, Belgique : Mardaga. Deliège, I. (1987). Grouping conditions in listening to music: An approach to Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s grouping preferences rules. Music Perception, 4(4), 325-360. Deliège, I., Vitouch, O., & Ladinig, O. (dir.) (2010). Musique et Évolution. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga. Demeuldre, M. (dir.) (2004). Sentiments doux-amers dans les musiques du monde. Paris, France : L’Harmattan. DeNora, T. (2000). Music in Everyday Life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DeNora, T. (2001). Quand la musique de fond entre en action. Terrain, 37, 75-88. Desain, P. & Honing, H. (2003). The formation of rhythmic categories and metric priming. Music Perception, 32(3), 341-365. Descola, P. (2005). Par-delà nature et culture. Paris, France : Gallimard. Deutsch, D. (1982). Organizational Processes in Music. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, Mind and Brain. The Neuropsychology of Music (p. 119-135). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Devillers, C. & Tintant, H. (1996). Questions sur la théorie de l’évolution. Paris, France : P.U.F. References17

DeWitt, L. & Samuel, A. (1999). The role of knowledge-based expectations in music perception: Evidence from musical restoration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(2), 123-144. Di Francesco, M. (1998). L’io e i suoi sé. Identità personale e scienza della mente. Milano, Italia: Raffaello Cortina Editore. Dimatati, M. (2009). Exploring absolute pitch in children and young people with congenital visual impairment. MSc thesis, Goldsmiths’ College, London, United Kingdom. Dimatati, M., Ockelford, A., Heaton, P., Pring, L., & Downing, J. (2010). Absolute pitch in children with congenital visual impairment: A preliminary study. Poster presented at ICMPC 11, Seattle, WA. Dissanayake, E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 389-410). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Dissanayake, E. (2006). Ritual and Ritualization: Musical Means of Conveying and Shaping Emotion in Humans and Other Animals. In S. Brown & U. Volgsten (Eds.), Music and manipulation (p. 31-56). New York, NY: Berghahn Books. Dissanayake, E. (2008). If music is the food of love, what about survival and reproductive success? Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 169-192. Dobzhansky, T. (1956). What is an adaptive trait? The American Naturalist, 90(855), 337-347. Dogana, F. (1983). Suono e senso. Fondamenti teorici ed empirici del simbolismo fonetico. Milano, Italia: Angeli. Dowling, W. J. (1978). Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 85(4), 341-354. Dowling, W. J. (1989). Simplicité et complexité en musique et en cognition. Dans S. McAdams et I. Deliège (dir.), La musique et les sciences cognitives (P. Drianne, trad.) (p. 351-360). Bruxelles, Belgique : Mardaga. Dowling, W. J. & Harwood, D. L. (1986). Music cognition. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Drake, C. (1998). Psychological Processes Involved in the Temporal Organization of Complex Auditory Sequences: Universal and Acquired Processes. Music Perception, 16(1), 11-26. Drake, C. & Bertrand, D. (2003). The quest for universals in temporal processing in music. In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music (p. 21-31). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Drake, C. & Palmer, C. (1993). Accent structures in music performance. Music Perception, 10(3), 343-378. Dreyfus, H. (1991). Being-in-the-World: A Commentary on Heidegger’s Being and Time, Division I. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Dufter, A. & Noel Aziz Hanna, P. (2009). Natural versification in French and German counting- out rhymes. In J.-L. Aroui & A. Arleo (Eds.), Towards a Typology of Poetic Forms: From linguistic structure to metrics and beyond (p. 101-121). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Dupont, F. (1991). Homère et Dallas. Paris, France : Hachette. Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. During, J. (1994). Quelque chose se passe. Lagrasse, France : Verdier. Duve (de), C. (2002). À l’écoute du vivant. Paris, France : Odile Jacob. References18

Ebcioglu, K. (1986). An expert system for harmonization of chorales in the style of J. S. Bach. Ph. D. Dissertation, Department of Computer Science and Engineering, University at Buffalo, New York, NY. Edelman, G. M. (1989). Neural Darwinism: The theory of group selection. New York, NY: Basic Books. Eliot, T. S. (1920/1997). The Sacred Wood: Essays on Poetry and Criticism. London, UK: Faber and Faber. Eliot, T. S. (1933). The Use of Poetry and the Use of Criticism. London, UK: Faber and Faber. Ellis, A. J. (1885). On the musical scales of various nations. Journal of the Society of Arts, 33, 485- 527. Erickson, R. (trans.) (1995). Musica enchiriadis and Scolica enchiriadis. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Ericsson, K. A. & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American psychologist, 49(8), 725-747. Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological review, 100(3), 363-406. Etoile (de l’), S. (2009). Processes of : Clinical and scientific rationales and models. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (p. 493-502). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Evans, N. & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429-492. Fassbender, C. (1995). La sensibilité auditive du nourrisson aux paramètres acoustiques du langage et de la musique. Dans I. Deliège et J. A. Sloboda, Naissance et développement du sens musical (p. 63-99). Paris, France : P.U.F. (Infant’s auditory sensitivity towards acoustic parameters of speech and music. In I. Deliège & J. A. Sloboda [Eds.], Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence [p. 56-87]. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 1996.) Fauconnier, G. (1985/1994). Mental Spaces: Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities. New York, NY: Basic Books. Fay, W. (1973). On the echolalia of the blind and of the autistic child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 38(4), 478-489. Feld, S. (1981). ‘Flow like a Waterfall’: The Metaphors of Kaluli Musical Theory. Yearbook for Traditional Music, 13, 22-47. Feld, S. (1982). Sound and Sentiment. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Feldman, M. & Laland, K. (1996). Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 11, 453-457. Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 181-195. Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: Is the melody the message? Child Development, 60, 1497-1510. Fernald, A. (1991). Prosody in speech to children: Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Annals of Child Development, 8, 43-80. References19

Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papoušek, M., Boysson-Bardies (de), B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477-501. Feuerstein, R. & Rand, Y. (1991/1995). Préface. Dans B. Dias, De l’évaluation psychométrique à l’évaluation du potentiel d’apprentissage. Lucerne, Suisse : SZH-SPC. Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition, 100, 173-215. Fónagy, I. (1977). Le statut de la phonostylistique. Phonetica, 34, 1-18. François, P. (2004). Professionnels et amateurs. Dans J.-J. Nattiez (dir.), Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 2 : Les savoirs musicaux (p. 585-607). Arles, France : Actes sud. Frith, S. (1987). Toward an aesthetic of popular music. In R. Leppert & S. McClary (Eds.), Music and society (p. 133-149). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Frith, S. (1996). Rhythm: Race, Sex and the Body. In Performing Rites. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Frith, S. (1996). Rhythm: Time, Sex and the Mind. In Performing Rites. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Fritz, T., Jentschke, S., Gosselin, N., Sammler, D., Peretz, I., & Koelsch, S. (2009). Universal Recognition of Three Basic Emotions in Music. Current Biology, 19, 1-4. Fucks, W. (1962). Mathematical analysis of the formal structure of music. Institute of Radio Engineers Transactions on Information Theory, 8, 225-228. Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotions in strong experiences with music. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research (p. 431-449). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Gabrielsson, A. & Lindström, E. (2001). The influence of musical structure on emotional expression. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research (p. 223-248). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Gabrielsson, A. & Lindström, E. (2009). The role of structure in the musical expression of emotions. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion (p. 367- 400). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Gallagher, S. & Zahavi, D. (2008). The Phenomenological Mind: An Introduction to Philosophy of Mind and Cognitive Science. New York, NY: Routledge. Gaver, W. W. (1993). What in the world do we hear? An ecological approach to auditory event perception. Ecological Psychology, 5(1), 1-29. Geertz, C. (1983). Local knowledge: Further essays in interpretative anthropology (Chap. 1: Blurred genres: The refiguration of social thought, p. 19-35). New York, NY: Basic Books. Geissmann, T. (2000). Gibbon Songs and Human Music from an Evolutionary Perspective. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 103-123). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Giannattasio, F. (1992). Il concetto di musica. Roma, Italia: NIS. Gibbs, R. W. (1999). Intentions in the Experience of Meaning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gibbs, R. W. (2005). Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. London, UK: Allen & Unwin. References20

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gjerdingen, R. (1988). Classic Turn of Phrase: Music and the Psychology of Convention. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Gobes, S. & Bolhuis, J. (2007). Birdsong memory: A neural dissociation between song recognition and production. Current Biology, 17(9), 789-793. Goddard, C. (2002). The search for the shared semantic core of all languages. In C. Goddard & A. Wierzbicka (Eds.), Meaning and Universal Grammar: Theory and Empirical Findings, Vol. I (p. 5-40). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2009). Darwinian Populations and Natural Selection. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Goldschmidt, W. (1959). Man’s Way: A Preface to the Understanding of Human Society. New York, NY: Holt. Gordon, E. E. (1989). Manual for the advanced measures of music audiation. Chicago, IL: GIA Publications. Gould, S. J. (1996). Full House: The Spread of Excellence from Plato to Darwin. New York, NY: Harmony Books. Gould, S. J. (2002). The Structure of Evolutionary Theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (M. Blanc, trad. : La structure de la théorie de l’évolution. Paris, France : Gallimard, 2006.) Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 205, 581-598. Gourlay, K. A. (1984). The Non-Universality of Music and the Universality of Non-Music. The World of Music, XXVI, 25-36. Gratier, M. (1999-2000). Expressions of belonging: The effect of acculturation on the rhythm and harmony of mother-infant vocal interaction. Musicae Scientiae, Spécial issue, 93-119. Gratier, M. (2001). Rythmes et appartenances culturelles. Étude acoustique des échanges vocaux entre mères et bébés autochtones et migrants. Thèse de Doctorat, Université René Descartes, Paris, France. Gratier, M. (2003). Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: Cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cognitive Development, 18, 533-554. Gratier, M. & Apter-Danon, G. (2008). The musicality of belonging: Repetition and variation in mother-infant interaction. In C. Trevarthen & S. Malloch (Eds.), Communicative musicality (p. 301-327). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Greenberg, H. (Ed.) (1963). Universals of language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Grewe, O., Altenmüller, E., Nagel, F., & Kopiez, R. (2009-2010). Individual emotional reactions towards music: Evolutionary-based universals? Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 261-287. (M. Gratier, trad. : Réactions individuelles émotionnelles à la musique : des universels issus de l’évolution ? Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 207-225]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Grigorenko, E. L. (2003). Expertise and mental disabilities: Bridging the unbridgeable? In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (p. 156-185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gundlach, R. (1935). Factors determining the characterization of musical phrases. American Journal of Psychology, 47(4), 624-644. References21

Hagen, E. H. & Hammerstein, P. (2009-2010). Did Neanderthals and other early humans sing ? Seeking the biological roots of music in the territorial advertisements of primates, lions, hyenas, and wolves. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 291-320. (S. Renard, trad. : L’Homme de Néandertal et les premiers Hommes chantaient-ils ? Peut-on trouver les racines biologiques de la musique dans les avertissements territoriaux des primates, des lions, des hyènes et des loups ? Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 229-249]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the perception of auditory events. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Hannon, E. E. & Trehub, S. E. (2005a). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16, 48-55. Hannon, E. E. & Trehub, S. E. (2005b). Tuning in to musical rhythms: Infants learn more readily than adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 12639-12643. Hanslick, E. (1865). Vom Musikalisch-Schönen: ein Beitrag zur Revision der Aesthetik der Tonkunst. Leipzig, Deutschland: Weigel. Hanslick, E. (1893/2004). Du beau dans la musique : essai de réforme de l’esthétique musicale (C. Bannelier, trad.). Paris, France : Phénix. Hargreaves, D. J. & North, A. C. (Eds.) (1997). The social psychology of music. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Hargreaves, D. J. & North, A. C. (1999). The function of music in everyday life: Redefining the social in music psychology. Psychology of Music, 27, 71-83. Harris, Z. S. (1951). Methods in Structural Linguistics. Chicago, IL: Chicago University. Harwood, D. L. (1976). Universals in Music: A Perspective from Cognitive Psychology. Ethnomusicology, 20(3), 521-533. Hauser, M. D. (2000). The Sound and the Fury: Primate Vocalizations as Reflections of Emotion and Thought. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 77-102). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Hauser, M., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The Faculty of Language: What Is, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298, 1570-1579. Hayes, B. & McEachern, M. (1998). Quatrain form in English verse. Language, 74(3), 473-507. Heaton, P. (2009). Assessing musical skills in autistic children who are not savants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1552), 1,443-1,447. Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1998). Autism and pitch-processing: A precursor for savant musical ability? Music Perception, 15(3), 291-305. Heaton, P., Williams, K., Cummins, O., & Happé, F. (2008). Autism and pitch-processing splinter skills: A group and subgroup analysis. Autism, 12(2), 203-219. Helmlinger, A. (2008). Les steelbands de Trinidad et Tobago: ethnomusicologie cognitive d’une mémoire d’orchestre. Intellectica, 48(1), 81-101. Hevner, K. (1936). Experimental studies of the elements of expression in music. American Journal of Psychology, 48(2), 246-268. Heyduk, R. G. (1975). Rated Preference For Musical Compositions as it Relates to Complexity and Exposure Frequency. Perception & Psychophysics, 17(1), 84-91. Higgins, K. M. (2006). The Cognitive and Appreciative Import of Musical Universals. Revue Internationale de Philosophie, 238, 486-503. References22

Hirtler, E. (1995). Die Musik als scientia mathematica von der Spatäntike bis zum Barock. Frankfurt am Main, Deutschland: Lang. Hjelmslev, L. (1943/1961). Prolegomena to a Theory of Language (F. J. Whitfield, Trans.). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. Horton, T. (2001). The compositionality of tonal structures: A generative approach to the notion of musical meaning. Musicae Scientiae, 5(2), 131-160. Houston, D. & Jusczyk, P. (2000). The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation by infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26, 1570-1582. Houston, D. & Jusczyk, P. (2003). Infants’ long-term memory for the sound patterns of words and voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 1143-1154. Howe, M. J. A. & Davidson, J. W. (2003). The early progress of able young musicians. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (p. 186-212). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Hucbald (1978). De harmonica institutione. In Hucbald, Guido, and John on Music: Three Medieval Treatises (W. Babb, trans.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Hughes, D. W. (1989). Grammars of non Western music: A selective survey. In P. Howell, R. West, & I. Cross (Eds.), Representing musical structure (p. 327-362). London, UK: Academic Press. Huron, D. (2006). Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Hurt-Thaut, C. (2009). Clinical practice in music therapy. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (p. 503-514). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Husserl, E. (1893-1917/1991). On the Phenomenology of the Consciousness of Internal Time (J. B. Brough, trans.). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Hyde, K. L., Zatorre, R. J., Griffiths, T. D., Lerch, J. P., & Peretz, I. (2006). The brain morphometry of congenital amusic individuals. Brain, 129, 2562-2570. Ibsen al Faruqi, L. (1987). The Cantillation of the Qur’an. Asian Music, 19(1), 1-25. Ilari, B. & Polka, L. (2006). Infants’ preferences and long-term memory for complex music. International Journal of Music Education, 24, 7-20. Imberty, M. (1981). Les écritures du temps. Sémantique psychologique de la musique, Tome 2. Paris, France : Bordas. Imberty, M. (2003). Langage, musique et cognition : quelques remarques sur l’évolution nécessaire des problématiques psychologiques des vingt dernières années. Circuit : Musiques Contemporaines, 13(2), 93-110. Imberty, M. (2005). La musique creuse le temps. De Wagner à Boulez: Musique, psychologie, psychanalyse. Paris, France : L’Harmattan. Imberty, M. & Gratier, M. (dir.) (2007). Temps, geste et musicalité. Paris, France : L’Harmattan. Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Jackendoff, R. & Lerdahl, F. (1977). Toward a formal theory of tonal music. Journal of Music Theory, 21(1), 111-171. Jackendoff, R. & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what’s special about it? Cognition, 100(1), 33-72. References23

Jairazbhoy, N. A. (1971/1995). The rgs of North Indian Music: Their structure and evolution. Bombay, India: Popular Prakashan Pvt. Ltd. Jakobson, R. (1976). Six leçons sur le son et le sens. Paris, France : Éditions de Minuit. Jan, S. (2007). The memetics of music: A neo-Darwinian view of musical structure and culture. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate. Janata, P. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying auditory image formation in music. In R. I. Godøy & H. Jørgensen (Eds.), Musical imagery (p. 27-42). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. Jaques-Dalcroze, E. (1907). Le rythme, la musique et l’éducation. Lausanne, Suisse : Foetisch. Jeannerod, M. (2002). La nature de l’esprit. Paris, France : Odile Jacob. Jeffries, T. (1974). Relationship of interval frequency count to ratings of melodic intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(5), 903-905. Johnson, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Johnson, M. (2007). The Meaning of the Body: The Aesthetics of Human Understanding. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Johnson-Laird, P. & Oatley, K. (1992). Basic emotions, rationality, and folk theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6(1), 201-223. Joliat, F. (2000). La formation musicale des futurs enseignants. Revue Musicale Suisse, 5, 17-20. Joliat, F. (2003, août). Preliminary aptitude test for discrimination of acoustico-gestual synchronisation between two clips (PATDAGS). Poster presented at the International conference Music and gesture, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom. Joliat, F. (2008). L’effet de l’aptitude musicale dans la détection de la désynchronisation audiovisuelle : l’œil musical ou le syndrome de Ludwig van Beethoven. Thèse de doctorat, Université de Fribourg, Fribourg, Suisse. Joliat, F. (2010a). La mesure de l’aptitude musicale : ses théories, ses outils et ses résultats. Médecine des Arts, 67, 30-43. Joliat, F. (2010b). L’effet de l’aptitude musicale dans la détection de la désynchronisation audiovisuelle : l’œil musical ou le syndrome de Ludwig van Beethoven. Recherche en Éducation Musicale, Numéro spécial, 28, 29-48. Jusczyk, P., Pisoni, D., & Mullennix, J. (1992). Some consequences of stimulus variability on speech processing by 2-month-old infants. Cognition, 43, 253-291. Juslin, P. N. (1997). Perceived emotional expression in synthesized performances of a short melody: Capturing the listener’s judgement policy. Musicae Scientiae, 1(1), 225-256. Juslin, P. N., Friberg, A., & Bresin, R. (2001-2002). Toward a computational model of expression in music performance: The GERM model. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 63-122. Juslin, P. N. & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.) (2001). Music and Emotion: Theory and Research. New York, NY: University Press. Juslin, P. N. & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.) (2009). Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Juslin, P. N. & Timmers, R. (2009). Expression and communication of emotion in music performance. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion (p. 453-489). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. References24

Kaemmer, J. E. (1993). Music in human life: Anthropological perspectives on music. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Keil, C. (1987). Participatory Discrepancies and the Power of Music. Cultural Anthropology, 2(3), 275-283. Kim, J. (1995). Mind-Body Problem. In T. Honderich (Ed.), Oxford Companion to Philosophy. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Kippen, J. & Bel, B. (1992). Modeling music with grammars: Formal language representation in the Bol processor. In A. Marsden & A. Pople (Eds.), Computer representations and models in music (p. 207-238). London, UK: Academic Press. Kivy, P. (1984). Sound and semblance: Reflections on musical representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Konvalinka, I., Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Follow you, follow me: Continuous mutual prediction and adaptation in joint tapping. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, i-First, 1-11. Kramer, L. (1991). Musical narratology: A theoretical outline. Indiana Theory Review, 12, 141-162. Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(4), 336-352. Krumhansl, C. L. & Agres, K. R. (2008). Musical Expectancy: The Influence of Musical Structure on Emotional Response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 584-585. Kuhl, P. K. (1979). The perception of speech in early infancy. In N. J. Lass (Ed.), Speech and Language: Advances in Basic Research and Practice. New York, NY: Academy Press. Kuhl, P. K. (1983). Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 263-285. Kuhl, P. K. & Miller, J. D. (1982). Discrimination of auditory target dimensions in the presence or absence of variation in a second dimension by infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 279-292. Kupiec, J.-J. (2003). Les espèces existent-elles ? Sciences et Avenir, Hors-série, 134, 34-39. Labussière, A. (1976). Rythme enfantin, pentatonismes et giusto syllabique dans les créations mélodiques des enfants. Revue de Musicologie, LXII(1), 25-84. Labussière, A. (1983). Réflexion sur le rôle de l’éducation musicale dans l’enseignement secondaire contemporain. Civilisations, 8, 77-84. Labussière, A. (1994). Un parcours obligé de l’analyse mélodique : l’écoute analytique de productions vocales d’enfants d’âge scolaire. Dans Production et expressivité musicale chez l’enfant (p. 15-25). Paris, France : Université Paris Sorbonne-Paris IV. Labussière, A. (1994-1995). Le style musical enfantin, concepts et méthodes d’analyse. Musique et style, méthodes et concepts (p. 115-124). Paris, France : Université Paris Sorbonne-Paris IV. Labussière, A. (1997). Réflexion sur la modalité à partir de productions vocales d’enfants d’âge scolaire. Musurgia, IV(3), 81-118. Labussière, A. (2007a). Pérennité du sens modal dans l’organisation du chant traditionnel à voix nue. Dans M. Favrot, L’analyse mélodique (p. 29-44). Paris, France : Université Paris Sorbonne-Paris IV. Labussière, A. (2007b). « Chantez-moi ce texte », le modèle linguistique français face au modèle mélodique dans l’improvisation vocale sur un texte imposé : interférences, ruptures, References25

conciliations. Dans B. Bossis, M.-N. Masson et J.-P. Olive, Le modèle vocal, la musique, la voix, la langue (p. 211-228). Rennes, France : Presses Universitaires de Rennes. Labussière, A. (2007c). Geste et structure dans le chant traditionnel à voix nue. Dans J.-J. Nattiez (dir.), Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 5 : L’unité de la musique (p. 980-1028). Arles, France : Actes sud. Labussière, A. (2008). Voix de Turquie. Une étude stylistique de chants traditionnels à voix nue. Musurgia, XV(4), 5-36. Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York, NY: Basic Books. Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. 1. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Larson, S. (2004). Musical Forces and Melodic Expectations: Comparing Computer Models with Experimental Results. Music Perception, 21, 457-498. Lartillot, O. (2004a). Fondements d’un système d’analyse musicale suivant une modélisation cognitiviste de l’écoute. Thèse de doctorat, Université Paris VI, Paris, France. Lartillot, O. (2004b). A musical pattern discovery system founded on a modeling of listening strategies. Computer Music Journal, 28(3), 53-67. Lartillot, O. (2009). Taxonomic categorisation of motivic patterns. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 4(B), 25-46. Lartillot, O. (2010). Reflexions towards a generative theory of musical parallelism. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 5, 195-229. Lartillot, O. & Ayari, M. (2009). Segmentation of Tunisian modal improvisation: Comparing listeners’ responses with computational predictions. Journal of New Music Research, 38(2), 117-127. Lartillot, O. & Toiviainen, P. (2007a). A Matlab Toolbox for Musical Feature Extraction from Audio. Paper presented at the International Conference on Digital Audio Effects, Bordeaux, France. Lartillot, O. & Toiviainen, P. (2007b). Motivic matching strategies for automated pattern extraction. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 4(A), 281-314. Lea, R., Rapp, D., Elfenbein, A., Mitchel, A., & Romine, R. (2008). Sweet silent thought: Alliteration and resonance in poetry comprehension. Psychological Science, 19(7), 709-716. Lebedeva, G. & Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Sing that tune: Infants’ perception of melody and lyrics and the facilitation of phonetic recognition in songs. Infant Behaviour and Development, 33(4), 419-430. Lecanuet, J.-P. (1995). L’expérience auditive prénatale. Dans I. Deliège et J. A. Sloboda, Naissance et développement du sens musical (p. 7-38). Paris, France : P.U.F. (Prenatal auditory experience. In I. Deliège & J. A. Sloboda [Eds.], Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence [p. 3-34]. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 1996.) Leech, G. L. (1974). Semantics. Middlesex, UK: Penguin Books. References26

Lefebvre, H. (2004). Rhythmanalysis: Space, time and everyday life (S. Elden and G. Moore, trans.). London, UK: Continuum. (Original book: Éléments de rythmanalyse : introduction à la connaissance des rythmes. Paris, France : Éditions Syllepse, 1992.) Lefkoff, G. (Ed.) (1967). Papers from the West Virginia University Conference on Computer Applications in Music. Morgantown, WV: West Virginia University Library. Lehmann, C., Welker, L., & Schiefenhövel, W. (2009-2010). Towards an Ethology of Song: A categorization of musical behaviour. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 321-338. (S. Renard, trad. : Vers une éthologie du chant : une catégorisation du comportement musical. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 267- 277]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Lehmann, A. C. & Gruber, H. (2009). Music. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman, The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (p. 457-470). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Leman, M. (2008). and mediation technology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Leman, M. (2010). An embodied approach to music semantics. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 5, 43-67. Lerdahl, F. (1988). Cognitive constraints on compositional systems. In J. A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative processes in music (p. 231-259). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Lerdahl, F. (1992). Cognitive constraints on compositional systems. Contemporary Music Review, 6(1), 97-121. Lerdahl, F. (2001). Tonal pitch space. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Lerdahl, F. & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Leroy, J.-L. (2003). Vers une épistémologie des savoirs musicaux. Paris, France : L’Harmattan. Leroy, J.-L. (2005). Le vivant et le musical. Paris, France : L’Harmattan. Leroy, J.-L. (2009a). A general model for the dynamic functioning of musical systems. Musicae Scientiae, 13(1), 3-34. Leroy, J.-L. (2009b). Les styles musicaux comme médiateurs culturels dans l’élaboration du sens musical − 1. Le concept de style musical. Journal de Recherche en Éducation Musicale (JREM), 8, 7-50. Leroy, J.-L. (2010). Principes d’organisation des hauteurs discrètes dans les systèmes musicaux. Musurgia, XVII(1), 17-47. Leroy, J.-L. (2012). Un paradigme pour les sciences de l’éducation musicale : de la musique objet à la musique fonction. Université de Paris-Sorbonne/Observatoire Musical Français (Document de recherche O.M.F., série Didactique de la musique, n° 33). Leroy, J.-L. (forthcoming). Outline for a general theory of music. Paris, France : Éditions des Archives Contemporaines. Leroy, Y. (2002). L’univers sonore animal. Pour la Science, Hors-série, 34, 6-9. Lestel, D. (2009). Les origines animales de la culture. Paris, France : Flammarion. Lévi-Strauss, C. (1963). Structural Anthropology (C. Jacobson and B. G. Schoepf, trans.). New York, NY: Doubleday Anchor Books. (Original book published in 1958.) Levitin, D. J. & Tirovolas, A. K. (2009). Current advances in the cognitive neuroscience of music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 211-231. References27

Leydier, K. (2007, juin). Le rôle des berceuses dans le développement psychique du nourrisson. Communication présentée à l’Université de Franche-Comté, France. Liberman, M. Y. (1975). The intonational system of English. Bloomington, In: Indiana University Linguistics Club. Lidov, D. & Gabura, J. (1973). A Melody Writing Algorithm Using a Formal Language Model. Computer Studies in the Humanities, 4(3-4), 138-148. Lipscomb, S. D. (2005). The perception of audio-visual composites: Accent structure alignment of simple stimuli. Selected reports in ethnomusicology, 12, 37-67. Livingstone, S. L. & Thompson, W. F. (2009-2010). The emergence of music from the Theory of Mind. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 83-115. (S. Renard, trad. : Émergence de la musique et théorie de l’esprit. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 47-63]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Lomax, A. (1976a). Cantometrics: A method in musical anthropology. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Lomax, A. (1976b). Cantometrics: An approach to the anthropology of music. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Lord, A. B. (1960). The singer of Tales. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lortat-Jacob, B. (1996). Canti di passione. Castelsardo, Sardegna: Lucca, LIM. Lortat-Jacob, B. (2003). L’oreille Jazz : essai d’ethnomusicologie. Circuit, XIV(1), 43-51. Lortat-Jacob, B. (2004). Ce que chanter veut dire. Étude de pragmatique. L’Homme, 171-172, 83-101. Lortat-Jacob, B. (2010). Quand les affects confondent parole et musique – Sardaigne et Albanie. Dans C. Calame, F. Dupont, B. Lortat-Jacob et M. Manca (dir.), La voix actée. Pour une nouvelle ethnopoétique (p. 245-261). Paris, France : Kimé. Lösche, I. (1990). Sensorimotor and action development in autistic children from infancy to early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(5), 749-761. Lumsden, C. & Wilson, E. (1981). Genes, Mind and Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lynch, M. P., Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Urbano, R. C. (1990). Innateness, Experience, and Music Perception. Psychological Science, 1, 272-276. Määttänen, P. (1993). Action and Experience: A Naturalistic Approach to Cognition. Helsinki, Sweden: Academia Scientiarum Fennica. Mâche, F.-B. (1983). Musique, mythe, nature ou Les dauphins d’Arion. Paris, France : Klincksieck. Mâche, F.-B. (2000). The necessity of and problems with a universal musicology. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 473-479). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Mâche, F.-B. (2001). Musique au singulier. Paris, France : Odile Jacob. Malloch, S. (1999-2000). Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 29-57. Margulis, E. H. (2003). : A Discussion and Model. Ph. D. Dissertation, Columbia University, New York, NY. Margulis, E. H. & Levine W. H. (2004). Melodic Expectation: A Priming Study. In S. D. Lipscomb, R. Ashley, R. O. Gjerdingen, & P. Webster (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th References28

International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (p. 364-366). Evanston, IL: Casual Productions. Marler, P. (2000). Origins of Music and Speech: Insights from Animals. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 31-48). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Marler, P. (2004). Les origines animales de la musique. Dans J.-J. Nattiez (dir.), Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 2 : Les savoirs musicaux (p. 495-505). Arles, France : Actes sud. Marsden, A. & Pople, A. (Eds.) (1992). Computer representations and models in music. London, UK: Academic Press. Marsh, K. (2008). The Musical Playground: Global tradition and change in children’s songs and games. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Martin, D.-C. (2001). « Chanter l’amour ». Musique, fierté et pouvoir. Terrain, 37, 89-104. Martin, P. J. (2006). Music, Identity, and Social Control. In S. Brown & U. Volgsten (Eds.), Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music (p. 57-73). New York, NY: Berghahn Books. Masataka, N. (1999). Preference for infant-directed singing in 2-day-old hearing infants of deaf parents. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1001-1005. Massin, B. & Massin, J. (1990). Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart. Paris, France : Fayard. Mayer, R. E. (2003). What causes individual differences in cognitive performance? In R. J. Sternberg & L. Grigorenko (Eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (p. 263-273). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Maynard Smith, J. (2001). La construction du vivant : gènes, embryons et évolution. Paris, France : Éditions Cassini. Mayr, E. (1983). How to carry out the adaptationist program? The American Naturalist, 121(3), 324-334. McAdams, S. & Battier, M. (2005). Creation and perception of a contemporary musical work: The Angel of the Death by Roger Reynolds. Paris, France: IRCAM/Centre Pompidou. McAdams, S. & Bertoncini, J. (1997). Organization and discrimination of repeating sound sequences by newborn infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 2945-2953. McAdams, S. & Bregman, A. (1979). Hearing Musical Streams. Computer Music Journal, 3(4), 26-43. McDermott, J. H. (2009). What can experiments reveal about the origins of music? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 164-169. McDermott, J. & Hauser, M. D. (2005). The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution. Music Perception, 23(1), 29-59. McDermott, J. & Hauser, M. D. (2006). Thoughts on an Empirical Approach to the Evolutionary Origins of Music. Music Perception, 24(1), 111-116. McDonald, R., Hargreaves, D. J., & Miell, D. (Eds.) (2002). Musical identities. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. McDonald, R., Hargreaves, D. J., & Miell, D. (2009). Musical identities. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (p. 462-470). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. References29

McDonald, R., Miell, D., & Wilson, G. (2005). Talking about music: A vehicle for identity development. In D. Miell, R. MacDonald, & D. J. Hargreaves (Eds.), Musical communication (p. 321-338). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. McEvoy, R., Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1988). The functions of immediate echolalia in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(4), 657-668. McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping together in time: Dance and drill in human history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. McPherson, S. (1915). Form in Music. London, UK: Joseph Williams Ltd. Meeùs, N. (1997). Mode et système, conceptions ancienne et moderne de la modalité. Musurgia, IV(3). Mehler, J., Bertoncini, J., Barrière, M., & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D. (1978). Infant recognition of mother’s voice. Perception, 7, 491-497. Mehler, J., Bertoncini, J., Barriere, M., & Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D. (1984). Recognition of the mother’s voice by newborns. Cuadernos de Psicología, 6, 61-70. Merker, B. (2006). The Uneven Interface Between Culture and Biology in Human Music. Music Perception, 24(1), 95-98. Merker, B. (2009). Ritual foundations of human uniqueness. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality (p. 45-59). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1942). La Structure du Comportement. Paris, France : Presses Universitaires de France. (A. Fisher, trans.: The Structure of Behaviour. London, UK: Methuen, 1965.) Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris, France : Gallimard. (C. Smith, trans.: Phenomenology of Perception. London, UK: Routledge, 2002.) Merleau-Ponty, M. (1964). The Primacy of Perception: And Other Essays on Phenomenology, Psychology, the Philosophy of Art, History and Politics (E. Edie, Ed.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Merriam, A. P. (1964). The anthropology of music. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L. B. (1967). Music, the Arts, and Ideas. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L. B. (1973). Explaining Music. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. Meyer, L. B. (1989). Style and Music Theory, History and Ideology. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. Meyer, L. B. (1998). Un univers d’universaux. Philosophie, 59(4), 23-48. Meyer, L. B. (2001). Music and emotion: Distinctions and uncertainties. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research (p. 341-360). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Michaels, C. & Carello, C. (1981). Direct Perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Miell, D., McDonald, R., & Hargreaves, D. J. (Eds.) (2005). Musical Communication. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Miereanu, C. & Hascher, X. (Eds.) (1998). Les Universaux en musique. Actes du 4e Congrès international sur la signification musicale. Paris, France : Publications de la Sorbonne. Mikiten, T. (1996). A method for research in music medicine. In R. R. Pratt & R. Spintge (Eds.), Musicmedicine, Vol. 2 (p. 14-23). Saint Louis, MO: MMB Music. Miller, O. & Ockelford, A. (2005). Visual Needs. London, UK: Continuum. References30

Mills, A. (1993). Visual handicap. In D. Bishop & K. Mogford (Eds.), Language Development in Exceptional Circumstances (p. 150-164). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Mills, M. & Melhuish, E. (1974). Recognition of mother’s voice in early infancy. Nature, 252, 123-124. Mithen, S. (2005). The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London, UK: Weidenfeld and Nicholson. Mithen, S. (2009). The Music Instinct: The Evolutionary Basis of Musicality. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169 (p. 3-12). Boston, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Moles, A. (1968). Information Theory and Esthetic Perception. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. Molino, J. (1975). Fait musical et sémiologie de la musique. Musique en Jeu, 17, 37-62. Molino, J. (2000). Towards an evolutionary theory of music and language. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 165-176). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Molino, J. & Nattiez, J.-J. (2005). Tipologie e universali. In J.-J. Nattiez, M. Bent, R. Dalmonte, & M. Baroni (Eds.), Enciclopedia della Musica, Vol. 5: L’unità della musica (p. 331-366). Torino, Italia: Einaudi. Molino, J. & Nattiez, J.-J. (2007). Typologie et Universaux. Dans J.-J. Nattiez (dir.), Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 5 : L’unité de la musique (p. 337-396). Arles, France : Actes sud. Monod, J. (1970). Le hasard et la nécessité. Paris, France : Éditions du Seuil. Morin, E. (1973). Le paradigme perdu : la nature humaine. Paris, France : Éditions du Seuil. Morrongiello, B. & Roes, C. (2000). Children’s memory for new songs: Integration or independent storage of words and tunes? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50(1), 25-38. Motte-Haber (de la), H. (1994). Principales théories scientifiques en psychologie de la musique : les paradigmes. Dans A. Zenatti (dir.), Psychologie de la musique (p. 27-53). Paris, France : PUF. Murdock, G. P. (1945). The Common Denominator of Cultures. In R. Linton (Ed.), The Science of Man in the World Crisis (p. 123-142). New York, NY: Columbia University Press. Nakata, T. & Trehub, S. E. (2004). Infants’ responsiveness to maternal speech and singing. Infant Behavior & Development, 27(4), 455-464. Nakata, T. & Trehub, S. E. (2011). Expressive timing and dynamics in infant-directed and non- infant-directed singing. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind & Brain, 21(1-2), 45-53. Narmour, E. (1990). The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-Realization Model. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Narmour, E. (1992). The Analysis and Cognition of Melodic Complexity: The Implication-Realization Model. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Nattiez, J.-J. (1973). Trois modèles linguistiques pour l’analyse musicale. Musique en jeu, 10, 3-11. Nattiez, J.-J. (1975). Fondements d’une sémiologie de la musique. Paris, France : Union Générale d’Éditions. Nattiez, J.-J. (1987). Musicologie générale et sémiologie. Paris, France : Christian Bourgois. Nattiez, J.-J. (1990). Music and discourse: Towards a semiology of music (C. Abbate, Trans.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Original book published in 1987.) References31

Nattiez, J.-J. (2002). Musica e significato. In J.-J. Nattiez, M. Bent, R. Dalmonte, & M. Baroni (Eds.), Enciclopedia della Musica, Vol. 2: Il sapere musicale (p. 206-238). Torino, Italia: Einaudi. (La signification comme paramètre musical. Dans J.-J. Nattiez [dir.], Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 2 : Les savoirs musicaux [p. 256-289]. Arles, France : Actes sud, 2004.) Nattiez, J.-J. (2003). Musiche tradizionali e significati. In J.-J. Nattiez, M. Bent, R. Dalmonte, & M. Baroni (Eds.), Enciclopedia della Musica, Vol. 3: Musica e culture (p. 841-861). Torino, Italia: Einaudi. Nattiez, J.-J. (2004). Ethnomusicologie. Dans J.-J. Nattiez (dir.), Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 2 : Les savoirs musicaux (p. 721-739). Arles, France : Actes sud. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. New York, NY: Freeman. Nemoto, I., Fujimaki, T., & Wang, L. (2010). fMRI measurement of brain activities to major and minor chords and cadence sequences. Conference Proceedings – IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 1, 5,640-5,643. Nettl, B. (1983). The Study of Ethnomusicology: Twenty-nine Issues and Concepts. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. Nettl, B. (2000). An Ethnomusicologist Contemplates Universals in Musical Sound and Musical Cultures. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 463-472). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Nettl, B. (2005). The Study of Ethnomusicology: Thirty-One Issues and Concepts. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press. Nielzén, S. & Cesarec, Z. (1982). Emotional experience of music as a function of musical structure. Psychology of Music, 10(1), 7-17. North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & Hargreaves, J. J. (2004). Uses of Music in Everyday Life. Music Perception, 22(1), 41-77. Ockelford, A. (1988). Some observations concerning the musical education of blind children and those with additional handicaps. Paper presented at the 32nd Conference of the Society for Research in Psychology of Music and Music Education (now SEMPRE), University of Reading, United Kingdom. Ockelford, A. (1991). The role of repetition in perceived musical structures. In P. Howell, R. West, & I. Cross (Eds.), Representing Musical Structure (p. 129-160). London, UK: Academic Press. Ockelford, A. (1999). The Cognition of Order in Music: A Metacognitive Study. London, UK: Roehampton Institute. Ockelford, A. (2005). Repetition in Music: Theoretical and Metatheoretical Perspectives. London, UK: Ashgate. Ockelford, A. (2006). Implication and expectation in music: A zygonic model. Psychology of Music, 34(1), 81-142. Ockelford, A. (2008). Music for Children and Young People with Complex Needs. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Ockelford, A. (2009a). Zygonic Theory: Introduction, Scope, Prospects. Zeitschrift der Gesellschaft für Musiktheorie, 6(2), 91-172. Ockelford, A. (2009b). Similarity relations between groups of notes: Music-theoretical and music-psychology perspectives. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 4(B), 47-93. Ockelford, A. (2010a). Understanding autism. Libretto, 3, 11. References32

Ockelford, A. (2010b). Music – window on the autistic mind. In K. Heid & I. Joly (Eds.), What’s Special? Opera, Dance and Music Education for and with People with Special Needs across Europe (p. 48-51). Brussels, Belgium: Reseo. Ockelford, A. (2011). Another exceptional musical memory: Evidence from a savant of how atonal music is processed in cognition. In I. Deliège & J. Davidson (Eds.), Music and the Mind: A Book to Honour the Achievements of . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Ockelford, A., Gott, S., Risdon, J., & Zimmermann, S. (2011). Focus on Music, 3: Exploring the Musical Interests and Abilities of Blind and Partially-Sighted Children with Leber Congenital Amaurosis. London, UK: Institute of Education. Ockelford, A. & Matawa, C. (2009). Focus on Music, 2: Exploring the Musical Interests and Abilities of Blind and Partially-Sighted Children with Retinopathy of Prematurity. London, UK: Institute of Education. Ockelford, A., Pring, L., Welch, G., & Treffert, D. (2006). Focus on Music: Exploring the Musical Interests and Abilities of Blind and Partially-Sighted Children with Septo-Optic Dysplasia. London, UK: Institute of Education. Ockelford, A. & Sergeant, D. (under review). Musical expectancy in atonal contexts: Musicians perception of ‘antistructure’. Ockelford, A., Welch, G., Jewell-Gore, L., Cheng, E., Vogiatzoglou, A., & Himonides, E. (2011). Sounds of Intent, Phase 2: Approaches to the quantification of music- developmental data pertaining to children with complex needs. European Journal of Special Needs Education. Opie, I. & Opie, P. (1985). The Singing Game. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Paillard, J. (1994). La conscience. Dans M. Richelle, J. Requin et M. Robert (dir.), Traité de psychologie expérimentale, Vol. 2 (p. 639-684). Paris, France : P.U.F. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Panksepp, J. (2009-2010). The emotional antecedents to the evolution of music and language. In O. Vitouch & O. Ladinig (Eds.), Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 229-259. (M. Gratier, trad. : Les antécédents émotionnels de l’évolution de la musique et du langage. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 189- 206]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Panksepp, J. (2010). See Panksepp, J. (2009-2010). Panksepp, J. & Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: The neuro-affective foundations of music appreciation. Behavioral Processes, 60, 133-155. Papoušek, H. (1995). Musicalité et petite enfance. Origines biologiques et culturelles de la précocité. Dans I. Deliège et J. A. Sloboda (dir.), Naissance et développement du sens musical (p. 41-62). Paris, France : PUF. (Musicality in infancy research: Biological and cultural origins of early musicality. In I. Deliège & J. A. Sloboda [Eds.], Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence [p. 37-55]. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.) Papoušek, M. (1995). Le comportement parental intuitif, source cachée de la stimulation musicale dans la petite enfance. Dans I. Deliège et J. A. Sloboda (dir.), Naissance et développement du sens musical (p. 101-130). Paris, France : PUF. (Intuitive parenting: A hidden source of musical stimulation in infancy. In I. Deliège & J. A. Sloboda [Eds.], Musical Beginnings: Origins and Development of Musical Competence [p. 88-112]. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.) References33

Patel, A. D. (2006). Musical Rhythm, Linguistic Rhythm, and Human Evolution. Music Perception, 24, 99-104. Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, Language, and the Brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Patel, A. D. (2010). Music, biological evolution, and the brain. In M. Bailar (Ed.), Emerging Disciplines (p. 91-144). Houston, TX: Rice University Press. Patnaik, P. (1997). Rasa in Aesthetics. New Delhi, Delhi: DK Printworld. Pattee, H. (1995). Artificial life needs a real epistemology. In F. Moran, A. Moreno, J. Merelo, & P. Chacon (Eds.), Advances in Artifical Life: Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence (p. 23-38). Berlin, Deutschland: Springer. Payne, K. (2000). The Progressively Changing Songs of Humpback Whales: A Window on the Creative Process in a Wild Animal. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 135-150). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Payne. R. & Sorensen, M. (2005). The Cuckoos: Cuculidae. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Peretz, I. (2002). La musica e il cervello. In J.-J. Nattiez, M. Bent, R. Dalmonte, & M. Baroni (Eds.), Enciclopedia della Musica, Vol. 2: Il sapere musicale (p. 241-279). Torino, Italia : Einaudi. (Le cerveau musical. Dans J.-J. Nattiez [dir.], Musiques : une encyclopédie pour le XXIe siècle, Vol. 2 : Les savoirs musicaux [p. 293-320]. Arles, France : Actes sud, 2004.) Peretz, I. (2003). Brain specialization for music: New evidence from congenital amusia. In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music (p. 193-203). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Peretz, I. (2006). The nature of music from a biological perspective. Cognition, 100, 1-32. Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., & Bouchard, B. (1998). Music and emotion: Perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damage. Cognition, 68(2), 111-141. Peretz, I. & Zatorre, R. (Eds.) (2003). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Petrini, K., Dahl, S., Rocchesso, D., Waadeland, C. H., Avanzini, F., Puce, A., & Frank, E. P. (2009). Multisensory integration of drumming actions: Musical expertise affects perceived audiovisual asynchrony. Experimental brain research, 198(2-3), 339-352. Phan, M., Pytte, C., & Vicario, D. (2006). Early auditory experience generates long-lasting memories that may subserve vocal learning in songbirds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(4), 1,088-1,093. Phillips-Silver, J., Toiviainen, P., Gosselin, N., Piché, O., Nozaradan, S., Palmer, C., & Peretz, I. (2011). Born to dance but beat deaf: A new form of congenital amusia. Neuropsychologia, 49, 961-969. Piaget, J. (1967). Biologie et connaissance. Paris, France : Gallimard. Piaget, J. (1987). Introduction. Dans J. Piaget, P. Mounoud et J.-P. Bronckart (dir.), Psychologie (p. 307-316). Paris, France : Gallimard. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York, NY: William Morrow. Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate. New York, NY: Viking Press. Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural Language and Natural Selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707-784. Plantinga, J., Trehub, S. E., & Russo, F. A. (2011, June). Multimodal aspects of maternal speech and singing. Paper presented at Neurosciences and Music IV, Edinburgh, Scotland. References34

Pomerantz, J. R. (1986). Visual form perception: An overview. In E. C. Schwab & H. C. Nusbaum (Eds.), Visual perception, Vol. 2: Pattern recognition by humans and machines (p. 1- 30). New York, NY: Academic Press. Port, R. & Leary, A. (2005). Against formal phonology. Language, 81(4), 927-964. Pring, L. & Ockelford, A. (2005). Children with septo-optic dysplasia – musical interests, abilities and provision: The results of a parental survey. The British Journal of Visual Impairment, 23(2), 58-66. Rapp, D. & Samuel, A. (2002). A reason to rhyme: Phonological and semantic influences on lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 28(3), 564-571. Ratner, L. G. (1980). Classic music: Expression, form, and style. New York, NY: Schirmer. Rauschecker, J. P. (2003). Functional Organization and Plasticity of Auditory Cortex. In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music (p. 357-365). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Reti, R. (1951). The thematic process in music. New York, NY: Macmillan. Reuchlin, M. (1990). L’aptitude. Encylopaedia universalis, Vol. 2 (p. 681-685). Paris, France : Encyclopaedia universalis. Révész, G. (1953). Introduction to the psychology of music (G. I. C. de Courcy, trans.). London, UK: Longsmans, Green and Co. (Original published in 1946.) Rey, A. (dir.) (2001). Le Grand Robert de la langue française, Vol. 6. Paris, France : Dictionnaires Le Robert. Reybrouck, M. (1997). Gestalt Concepts and Music: Limitations and Possibilities. In M. Leman (Ed.), Music, Gestalt and Computing: Studies in Cognitive and (p. 57-69). Berlin-Heidelberg, Deutschland: Springer Verlag. Reybrouck, M. (2001a). Biological roots of musical epistemology: Functional Cycles, Umwelt, and enactive listening. Semiotica, 134(1-4), 599-633. Reybrouck, M. (2001b). Musical imagery between sensory processing and ideomotor simulation. In R. I. Godøy & H. Jørgensen (Eds.), Musical imagery (p. 117-135). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. Reybrouck, M. (2004). Music Cognition, Semiotics and the Experience of Time: Ontosemantical and Epistemological Claims. Journal of New Music Research, 33(4), 411- 428. Reybrouck, M. (2005a). A Biosemiotic and Ecological Approach to Music Cognition: Event Perception between Auditory Listening and Cognitive Economy. Axiomathes: An International Journal in Ontology and Cognitive Systems, 15(2), 229-266. Reybrouck, M. (2005b). Body, mind and music: between experiential cognition and cognitive economy. TRANS-Transcultural Music Review, 9, . Reybrouck, M. (2006). Musical creativity between symbolic modelling and perceptual constraints: The role of adaptive behaviour and epistemic autonomy. In I. Deliège & G. Wiggins (Eds.), Musical Creativity: Multidisciplinary Research in Theory and Practice (p. 42-59). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Reynolds, R. (2004). Compositional strategies in The Angel of Death, for piano, chamber orchestra, and computer-processed sound. Music Perception, 22(2), 173-205. References35

Rimland, B. & Edelson, S. M. (1995). A pilot study of auditory integration training in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 61-70. Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192. Rizzolati, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Les neurones miroirs. Paris, France : Odile Jacob. Roads, C. (1984). An overview of music representations. In M. Baroni & L. Callegari (Eds.), Musical grammars and computer analysis. Firenze, Italia: Olschki. Robb, L. (1999-2000). Emotional musicality in mother-infant vocal affect, and an acoustic study of postnatal depression. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 123-154. Robinson, D. (1995). Charlot : entre rire et larmes. Paris, France : Gallimard. Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Rosner, B. & Meyer, L. B. (1986). The perceptual roles of melodic process, contour and form. Music Perception, 4(1), 1-40. Rothenberg, D. (2001). Music in nature. Alternatives Journal, 27(2), 30-31. Roud, S. (2010). The Lore of the Playground: One hundred years of children’s games, rhymes and traditions. London, UK: Random House Books. Rousseau, J.-J. (1768). Dictionnaire de musique. New York, NY: Georg Olms/Johnsons Reprint Corporation. Rubin, D. (1995). Memory in Oral Traditions: The Cognitive Psychology of Epic, Ballads and Counting- Out Rhymes. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Ruwet, N. (1967). Musicologie et linguistique. Revue internationale des sciences sociales, 19(1), 85-93. Ruwet, N. (1987). Methods of analysis in musicology (M. Everist, Trans.). , 6(1- 2), 3-36. (Méthodes d’analyse en musicologie, Revue belge de Musicologie, 20[1-4], 65-90.) Saarikallio, S. H. (2008). Music in mood regulation: Initial scale development. Musicae Scientiae, XII(2), 291-307. Sachs, C. (1953). Rhythm and Tempo: A study in music history. London, UK: Dent. Sacks, O. (2007). : Tales of Music and the Brain. New York, NY: Vintage Books. Saffran, J. R., Johnson, E. K., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1999). Statistical learning by 8- month-old infants. Science, 274, 27-52. Saffran, J. R., Loman, M. M., & Robertson, R. R. W. (2000). Infant memory for musical experiences. Cognition, 77, 15-23. Sallis, J. (2006). Transfigurements: On the True Sense of Art. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Saussure (de), F. (1983). Course in general linguistics (R. Harris, Trans.). London, UK: Duckworth. (Original book published in 1916.) Schaeffer, P. (1966). Traité des objets musicaux. Paris, France : Éditions du Seuil. Schafer, M. (1979). The Tuning of the World. New York, NY: Knopf. Schellenberg, E. G. & Trehub, S. E. (1996). Natural Musical Intervals: Evidence from Infant Listeners. Psychological Science, 7, 272-277. Schellenberg, E. G. & Trehub, S. E. (1999). Culture-general and culture-specific factors in the discrimination of melodies. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 107-127. References36

Schenker, H. (1906). Harmonielehre (Neue musikalische Theorien und Phantasien, Band 1). Stuttgart, Deutschland: Cotta. Schenker, H. (1935). Der freie Satz (Neue musikalische Theorien und Phantasien, Band 3). Vienna, Austria: Universal Edition. Scherer, K. R. (1989). Vocal correlates of emotional arousal and affective disturbance. In H. Wagner & A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of social psychophysiology. New York, NY: Wiley. Scherer, K. R. (1991). Emotion expression in speech and music. In J. Sundberg, L. Nord, & R. Carlson (Eds.), Music, Language, Speech and Brain (p. 146-156). London, UK: Macmillan. Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. (2001). Emotion inferences from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(1), 76-92. Scherer, K. R. & Oshinsky, J. (1977). Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 1(4), 336-346. Scherer, K. R. & Zentner, K. (2001). Emotional effects of music: Production rules. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research (p. 361-392). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Schoenberg, A. (1967). Fundamentals of Musical Composition. London, UK: Faber and Faber. Schoenberg, A. (1975). Style and Idea: Selected Writings of Arnold Schoenberg (L. Stein, Ed., L. Black, trans.). New York, NY: St. Martins Press; London, UK: Faber & Faber. Schön, D., Moreno, S., Boyer, M., Kolinsky, R., Besson, M., & Peretz, I. (2006). Why do mothers sing to their children? Paper presented at the International Conference in Music Perception and Cognition, Bologna, Italia. Schubert, E. (2009-2010). The fundamental function of music. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 63-81. (S. Renard, trad. : Les fonctions fondamentales de la musique. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 35-46]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Scruton, R. (1977). The . Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Scruton, R. (2009). Understanding Music: Philosophy and Interpretation. London, UK: Continuum. Seashore, C. E. (1967). Psychology of music. New York, NY: Dover. (Original published in 1938.) Segré, G. (2001). La voix d’Elvis… Terrain, 37, 61-74. Selfridge-Field, E. (2007). Social dimensions of melodic identity, cognition, and association. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 4(A), 77-94. Selincourt (de), B. (1920/1956). Music and duration. Music and Letters, 1, 286-293. (In S. Langer [Ed.], Reflections on Art [p. 152-160]. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.) Serafine, M., Crowder, R., & Repp, B. (1984). Integration of melody and text in memory for songs. Cognition, 16(3), 285-303. Sève, B. (2002). L’Altération Musicale ou ce que la musique apprend au Philosophe. Paris, France : Éditions du Seuil. Shaffer, H. L. (1989). Cognition et affect dans l’interprétation musicale. In S. McAdams & I. Deliège (dir.), La musique et les sciences cognitives (p. 537-550). Paris, France : Mardaga. Shaw, R., Flascher, O., & Mace, W. (1996). Dimensions of Event Perception. In W. Prinz & B. Bridgeman (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Action, Vol. 1: Perception (p. 345-395). London, UK: Academic Press. References37

Shaw, R. & Pittenger, J. (1978). Perceiving change. In H. L. Pick & E. Salzman (Eds.), Modes of Perceiving and Processing Information (p. 187-204). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Shenfield, T., Trehub, S. E., & Nakata, T. (2003). Maternal singing modulates infant arousal. Psychology of Music, 31, 365-375. Shepard, R. N. (1981). Psychophysical complementarity. In M. Kuboby & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (Chap. 10). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Shuter-Dyson, R. & Gabriel, C. (1981). The psychology of musical ability. London, UK: Methuen. Slater, P. (2000). Birdsong Repertoires: Their Origins and Use. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 49-63). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Sloboda, J. A. (1985). The musical mind: The cognitive psychology of music. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Sloboda, J. A. (1992). Etudes empiriques de la réponse émotionnelle à la musique. Analyse Musicale, 26, 14-21. Sloboda, J. A. (1999). Everyday uses of music listening: A preliminary study. In S. W. Yi (Ed.), Music, mind and science (p. 354-369). Seoul, South Korea: Western Music Research Institute. Sloboda, J. A. (2001). Do pshychologists have anything useful to say about composition? In Analyse et création musicales (p. 69-78). Paris, France : L’Harmattan. Sloboda, J. A. (2005). Exploring the musical mind: Cognition, emotion, ability, function. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press. Sloboda, J., Lamont, A., & Greasley, A. (2009). Choosing to hear music: Motivation process and effect. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (p. 431-440). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Sloboda, J. A., O’Neill, S. A., & Ivaldi, A. (2001). Functions of music in everyday life: An exploratory study using the Experience Sampling Method. Musicae Scientiae, V(1), 9-29. Smith, J. F. (1979). The Experiencing of Musical Sound: Prelude to a Phenomenology of Music. New York, NY: Gordon and Breach. Snyder, A. W., Mulcahy, E. L., Taylor, J., Mitchell, D. J., Sachdev, P., & Gandevia, S. C. (2003). Savant-like skills exposed in normal people by suppressing the left fronto-temporal lobe. Journal of integrative neuroscience, 2(2), 149-158. Soley, G. & Hannon, E. E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: A cross-cultural comparison. Developmental Psychology, 46, 286-292. Sparshott, F. (1994). Music and feeling. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 52(1), 23-35. Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Spintge, R. & Droh, R. (Eds.) (1992). Music Medicine. Saint-Louis, MO: MMB Music. Steedman, M. J. (1984). A generative grammar for jazz chord sequences. Music Perception, 2(1), 52-77. Stern, D. N. (1981). Mère-enfant. Les premières relations. Sprimont, Belgique : Mardaga. (Original book: The First Relationship: Infant and Mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1977.) Stern, D. N. (1989). Le monde interpersonnel du nourrisson. Une perspective psychanalytique et développementale (A. Lazartigues et D. Pérard, trad.). Paris, France : P.U.F. (Original book: The interpersonal world of the infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology, 1985.) References38

Stern, D. N. (1997). La constellation maternelle (A. Cupa, trad.). Paris, France : Calmann-Lévy. (Original book: The Motherhood Constellation: A unified view of parent-infant therapy, 1995.) Stern, D. N. (2003). Le moment présent en psychothérapie. Un monde dans un grain de sable (M. Garène, trad.). Paris, France : Odile Jacob. Stevens, C. (2004). Cross-cultural studies of musical pitch and time. Acoustical Science and Technology, 25(6), 433-438. Stevens, S. (1951). Mathematics, Measurement, and Psychophysics. In S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology (p. 1-49). New York, NY: Wiley-Chapman & Hall. Storino, M. T., Dalmonte, R., & Baroni, M. (2007). An empirical investigation on the perception of musical style. Music Perception, 24(5), 417-432. Stravinsky, I. (1942). Poetics of Music. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sundberg, J. & Lindblom, B. (1976). Generative theories in language and music descriptions. Cognition, 4(1), 99-122. Suzuki, M., Okamura, N., Kawachi, Y., Tashiro, M., Arao, H., Hoshishiba, T., Gyoba, J., & Yanai, K. (2008). Discrete cortical regions associated with the musical beauty of major and minor chords. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioural Neuroscience, 8(2), 131-161. Swain, J. P. (1997). Musical languages. New York, NY: Norton. Syal, S. & Finlay, B. (2011). Thinking outside the cortex: Social motivation in the evolution and development of language. Developmental Science, 14, 417-430. Tafuri, J. (2008, Sept.). Singing before speaking: Experiences in the first year of life. Keynote Paper presented at the Conference Musical Development & Learning, Roehampton University, London, United Kingdom. Tarrant, M., North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2000). English and American adolescents’reasons for listening to music. Psychology of Music, 28, 166-173. Teplov, B. M. (1966). Psychologie des aptitudes musicales (J. Deprun, trad.). Paris, France : PUF. Thayer, A.-W. (1964). The Thayer’s Live of Beethoven (E. Buchet, trad.). New York, NY: Princeton University Press. Théberge, M. (2002). La question du développement artistique chez l’enfant. Dans M. Wirthner et M. Zulauf (Eds.), À la recherche du développement musical (p. 59-92). Paris, France : L’Harmattan. Thompson, W. F. & Robitaille, B. (1992). Can composers express emotions through music? Empirical Studies of the Arts, 10(1), 79-89. Thompson, W. F. & Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Listening to Music. In R. Colwell (Ed.), MENC Handbook on Musical Cognition and Development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Thorpe, M., Ockelford, A., & Aksentijevic, A. (2011). An empirical exploration of the zygonic model of expectation in music. Psychology of Music. Todt, D. (1971). Äquivalente und konvalente gesangliche Reaktionen einer extrem regermäßig singenden Nachtigall (Luscinia megarhynchos B.). Zeitschr. fûr vergl. Physiol., 71, 262-285. Trainor, L. J. (1996). Infant preferences for infant-directed versus noninfant-directed playsongs and lullabies. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 83-92. Trainor, L. J., Clark, E. D., Huntley, A., & Adams, B. (1997). The acoustic basis of preferences for infant-directed singing. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 383-396. References39

Trainor, L. J., Wu, L., & Tsang, C. D. (2004). Long-term memory for music: Infants remember tempo and timbre. Developmental Science, 7, 289-296. Trehub, S. E. (1985). Infants’ perception of melodies: Changes in a single tone. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 213-223. Trehub, S. E. (1987). Infants’ perception of musical patterns. Perception and Psychophysics, 41, 635-641. Trehub, S. E. (2000). Human Processing Predispositions and Musical Universals. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 427-448). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Trehub, S. E. (2001). Musical predispositions in infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 1-16. Trehub, S. E. (2003a). The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 669-673. Trehub, S. E. (2003b). Musical predispositions in infancy: An update. In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music (p. 3-20). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Trehub, S. E. (2003c). Toward a Developmental Psychology of Music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 402-413. Trehub, S. E., Bull, D., & Thorpe, L. A. (1984). Infants’ perception of melodies: The role of melodic contour. Child Development, 55, 821-830. Trehub, S. E., Endman, M., & Thorpe, L. A. (1990). Infants’ perception of timbre: Classification of complex tones by spectral structure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 300-313. Trehub, S. E. & Hannon, E. E. (2006). Infant music perception: Domain-general or domain- specific mechanisms? Cognition, 100, 73-99. Trehub, S. E. & Hannon, E. E. (2009). Conventional rhythms enhance infants’ and adults’ perception of musical patterns. Cortex, 45, 110-118. Trehub, S. E., Hannon, E. E., & Schachner, A. (2010). Perspectives on music and affect in the early years. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (p. 645-668). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Trehub, S. E. & Nakata, T. (2001-2002). Emotion and music in infancy. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 37-61. Trehub, S. E., Plantinga, J., & Russo, F. A. (2011, March). Maternal singing to infants in view or out of view. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada. Trehub, S. E. & Thorpe, L. A. (1989). Infants’ perception of rhythm: Categorization of auditory sequences by temporal structure. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 217-229. Trehub, S. E., Thorpe, L. A., & Morrongiello, B. A. (1987a). Organizational processes in infants’ perception of auditory patterns. Child Development, 58, 741-749. Trehub, S. E., Thorpe, L. A., & Morrongiello, B. A. (1987b). Infants’ perception of melodies: Changes in a single tone. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 213-223. Trehub, S. E. & Trainor, L. J. (1998). Singing to infants: Lullabies and play songs. Advances in Infancy Research, 12, 43-77. Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., Kamenetsky, S. B., Hill, D. S., Trainor, L. J., Henderson, J. L., & Saraza, M. (1997). Mothers’ and fathers’ singing to infants. Developmental Psychology, 33, 500-507. References40

Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., & Trainor, L. J. (1993a). Adults identify infant-directed music across cultures. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 193-211. Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., & Trainor, L. J. (1993b). Maternal singing in cross-cultural perspective. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 285-295. Trevarthen, C. (1999-2000). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 155-215. Trevarthen, C. (2004). Learning about ourselves, from children: Why a growing human brain needs interesting companions? Annual report – Hokkaido University research and clinical center for child development, 26, 9-44. Trevarthen, C. & Gratier, M. (2005). Voix et musicalité. Nature, émotion, relations et culture. Dans M.-F. Castarède et G. Konopczynski (dir.), Au commencement était la voix (p. 105- 116). Toulouse, France : Érès. Trevarthen, C. & Malloch, S. (2009). Musicality: Communicating the Vitality and Interests of Life. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Truchot, P. (2006). Une approche Bergsonienne de la spatialité en Musique. Astérion, 4, 217- 236. Tsang, C. D. & Trainor, L. J. (2002). Spectral slope discrimination in infancy: Sensitivity to socially important timbres. Infant Behavior and Development, 25(2), 183-194. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & A. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory. New York, NY: Academic Press. Unyk, A. M. & Carlsen, J. (1987). The Influence of Expectancy on Melodic Perception. Psychomusicology, 7(1), 3-23. Unyk, A. M., Trehub, S. E., Trainor, L. J., & Schellenberg, E. G. (1992). Lullabies and simplicity: A cross-cultural perspective. Psychology of Music, 20, 15-28. Uttal, W. R. (1973). The Psychobiology of Sensory Coding. New York, NY: Harper & Row. Uttal, W. R. (1998). Toward a New Behaviorism: The Case Against Perceptual Reductionism. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Vandermersch, B. (1999). Article « sujet ». Dans H. Bloch, R. Chemana, É. Dépret, A. Gallo, P. Leconte, J.-F. Le Ny, J. Postel et M. Reuschlin, Grand Dictionnaire de la Psychologie (p. 912-913). Paris, France : Larousse-Bordas. Varèse, E. & Wen-Chung, C. (1966). Perspectives of New Music, 5(1), 11-19. Vion-Dury, J. (2008). Musique et Mémoire : Plis et replis de la pensée. Dans G. Comet, A. Lejeune et C. Maury-Rouan (dir.), Mémoire individuelle, mémoire collective et histoire (p. 47- 71). Marseille, France : Éditions Solal. Vion-Dury, J. & Besson, M. (2010). L’électroencéphalographie et la musique : apports, limites et perspectives épistémologiques. Dans L. Guirard (dir.), Cinquante ans de psychologie de la musique. L’école de Robert Francès (p. 74-107). Montauban, France : Alexitère Éditions. Vitouch, O. & Ladinig, O. (Eds.) (2009-2010). Music and Evolution. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue. Volkova, A., Trehub, S. E., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Infants’ memory for musical performances. Developmental Science, 9, 584-590. Wallace, W. (1994). Memory for music: Effect of melody on recall of text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20(6), 1,471-1,485. Wallin, N. L. (1991). Biomusicology: Neurophysiological and Evolutionary Perspectives on the Origins and Purposes of Music. New York, NY: Pendragon Press. References41

Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., & Brown, S. (Eds.) (2000). The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Wallon, H. (1942). De l’acte à la pensée. Paris, France : Flammarion. Watson, K. (1942). The nature and measurement of musical meanings. Psychological Monographs, 54(2), 1-43. Welch, G., Ockelford, A., Carter, F.-C., Zimmermann, S., & Himonides, E. (2009). Sounds of Intent: Mapping musical behaviour and development in children and young people with complex needs. Psychology of Music, 37(3), 348-370. Wermke, K. & Mende, W. (2009-2010). Musical elements in human infants’ cries: In the beginning is the melody. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 151-175. (L. Richelle, trad. : Éléments musicaux dans les cris du nourrisson : au commencement est la mélodie. Dans I. Deliège, O. Vitouch et O. Ladinig [dir.], Musique et Évolution [p. 123-139]. Wavre, Belgique : Mardaga, 2010.) Westerlund, H. (2002). Bridging Experience, Action, and Culture in Music Education. Helsinki, Sweeden: Sibelius Academy. Whaley, J., Sloboda, J., & Gabrielsson, A. (2009). Peak experiences in music. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (p. 452-461). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Whaling, C. (2000). What’s Behind a Song? The Neural Basis of Song Learning in Birds. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (p. 65-76). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Wierzbicka, A. (1996). Semantics: Primes and Universals. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Wiggins, G. A., Müllensiefen, D., & Pearce, M. T. (2010). On the non-existence of music: Why music theory is a figment of the imagination. Musicae Scientiae, Discussion Forum, 5, 231-255. Willems, E. (1936/1987). Les bases psychologiques de l’éducation musicale. Fribourg, Suisse : Pro Musica. Williams, L. R. (2009). Effect of Voice-part Training and Music Complexity on Focus of Attention to Melody or Harmony. Contributions to Music Education, 36(2), 45-57. Wilson, S., Abbott, D., Lusher, D., Gentle, E., & Jackson, G. (2010). Finding your voice: A singing lesson from functional imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 32(12), 2115-2130. Winkler, I., Háden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., & Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. PNAS, 2468. (Retrieved from .) Winograd, T. (1968). Linguistics and the computer analysis of tonal harmony. Journal of Music Theory, 12(1), 2-49. Wittmann, M. & Pöppel, E. (1999-2000). Temporal mechanisms of the brain as fundamentals of communication with special reference to music perception and performance. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue, 13-28. Zatorre, R. & Peretz, I. (Eds.) (2001). The Biological Foundations of Music. New York, NY: The New York Academy of Sciences. Zbikowski, L. M. (1998). Metaphor and music theory: Reflections from cognitive science. Music Theory Online, 4(1). Zbikowksi, L. M. (2002). Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. References42

Zuckerkandl, V. (1956). Sound and Symbol: Music and the External World. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.