The Cultural Shaping of Compassion

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Cultural Shaping of Compassion Santa Clara University Scholar Commons Psychology College of Arts & Sciences 2017 The cultural shaping of compassion Birgit Koopmann-Holm Santa Clara University, [email protected] Jeanne L. Tsai Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.scu.edu/psych Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Koopmann-Holm, B. & Tsai, J. L. (2017). The cultural shaping of compassion. In E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon- Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of compassion science (pp. 273-285). New York: Oxford University Press. Copyright © 2017. Reproduced by permission of Oxford University Press. This material was originally published in The Oxford handbook of compassion science edited by E. M. Seppälä, E. Simon-Thomas, S. L. Brown, M. C. Worline, C. D. Cameron, & J. R. Doty, and has been reproduced by permission of Oxford University Press. For permission to reuse this material, please visit http://www.oup.co.uk/academic/rights/ permissions. This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Arts & Sciences at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. CHAP TER 21 The Cultural Shaping of Compassion Birgit Koopmann-Holm and Jeanne L. Tsai Abstract In this chapter, we first review the existing literature on cross-cultural studies on compassion. While cultural similarities exist, we demonstrate cultural differences in the conception, experience, and expression of compassion. Then we present our own work on the cultural shaping of compassion by introducing Affect Valuation Theory ( e.g., Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2006), our theoretical framework. We show how the desire to avoid feeling negative partly explains cultural differences in conceptualizations and expressions of compassion. Specifically, the more people want to avoid feeling negative, the more they focus on the positive (e.g., comforting memories) than the negative (e.g., the pain of someone's death) when responding to others' suffering, and the more they regard responses as helpful that focus on the positive (vs. negative). Finally, we discuss implications of our work for counseling, health care, and public service settings, as well as for interventions that aim to promote compassion. Key Words: culture, compassion, sympathy, affect valuation theory, emotion, American, German In The Descent ofMan, and Selection in Relation Simon-Thomas, 2010). However, because previous to Sex, Charles Darwin described the feeling of con­ researchers have used other terms (e.g., "sympathy," cern about another person's suffering (i.e., compas­ "empathy," "altruism") to refer to states and behav­ sion), as a basic human instinct that composed the iors that overlap with and are related to compassion, "noblest part ofour nature" (Darwin, 1871 , p. 162). we refer to studies that focus on these states as well. While increasing research suggests that experiences In this chapter, we review the existing cross-cultural of compassion are associated with greater psycho­ research on compassion and then describe our own logical well-being and prosocial behavior (e.g. , work in the area. But first, we describe what we Allred, Mallozzi, Matsui, & Raia, 1997; Condon & mean by "culture." DeSteno, 2011; Hofmann, Grossman, & Hinton, 2011 ; Neff, Hsieh, & Dejitterat, 2005; Neff, What Is Culture? Kirkpatrick, & Rude, 2007; Pace et al., 2009), most By "culture," we refer to socially transmitted of this research has focused on Western samples. As and historically derived ideas that are instanti­ a result, we still know relatively little about the role ated in shared practices, products, and institutions that culture plays in the conception, experience, (Kroeber & Kluckhohn, 1952). Cultural ideas pro­ and expression of compassion, which has implica­ vide individuals with a framework for how to be a tions for which aspects of compassion are "basic" good person (Shweder, 1991), and by engaging in and "instinctual" (Wuthnow, 2012, p. 306). In this framework, individuals recreate this framework line with the other chapters in this Handbook, we for others (Markus & Kitayama, 2010). This proc­ define "compassion" as sensitivity to the pain or ess is called "the mutual constitution of cultures and suffering of another person, coupled with a deep selves" (Markus & Kirayama, 2010) or the "culture desire to alleviate that suffering (Goetz, Keltner, & cycle" (Markus & Conner, 2013). For example, 2 73 women's magazines contain advertisements with Bencharit, Chim, Yeung, & F ung, under review) . models that reflect the beauty ideals of the adver­ Consequently, experiences and expressions of tisers. Readers of these magazines may at least to excitement may mean something_ different in East so me degree consciously or unconsciously internal­ Asian vs. U.S. contexts. ize these beauty ideals. Consequently, they may try In this chapter, we argue that cultures shape dif­ to emulate these ideals by purchasing clothes and ferent aspects of compassion (for a discussion of other products that reflect the ideals. Furthermore, different factors that might bring about cultural dif­ readers may consciously or unconsciously use these ferences in compassion, see Chiao, Chapter 12 this ideals when judging the beauty of others. volume). More specifically, we propose that culture Culture not only shapes ideals of beauty, but may shape how people conceptualize compassion also ideals of emotion (Markus & Kitayama, 201 0). (i.e., which feelings, thoughts, and behaviors peo­ The "cultural construction" approach to emotion ple view as being compassionate/helpful), experi­ (Boiger & Mesquita, 2012) argues that people's cul­ ence compassion (i.e., how people feel when they tural contexts shape their emotions by providing a see others suffering), and express compassion (i .e., framework for interpreting each emotional episode. what people do when they see others suffering), as For example, in a cultural context like the United illustrated in Figure 21.1. Here we focus on the spe­ States, in which people are encouraged to influence cific case in which cultural differences in views of others, excitement, enthusiasm, and other high­ negative affect shape how individuals conceptualize arousal positive states are viewed as desirable, in part and express compassion. At the end of the chapter, because being excited helps individuals change their we discuss the implications of these cultural differ­ environments to be consistent with their desires, ences for cross-cultural counseling, health care, and beliefs, and preferences (Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, public service. 2006; Tsai, Miao, Seppala, Fung, & Yeung, 2007). Thus, in many U.S. contexts, people are encouraged What Do We Know About Compassion to show and express their excitement and enthusi­ Across Cultures? asm, and people who show these states are rated First we will review existing cross-cultural stud­ more positively (S ims &Tsai, 2015). In contrast, in ies of the conception, experience, and expression of Eas t Asian contexts, in which people are encouraged compassion and related states, which demonstrate to adjust to others, calm, peacefulness, and other cultural similarities and differences. low-arousal positive states are viewed as desirable in part because being calm helps individuals attend Conception to their environments and ultimately change their Across different cultures, people conceive of own desires, beliefs, and preferences to be consist­ compassion and other related states (e.g., sympa­ ent with those of others. Thus, in many East Asian thy, empathy) as emotional (e.g., Shaver, Murdaya, contexts, people are encouraged to show and express & Fraley, 2001). At the same time, cultures also their calm and peacefulness, and people who show appear to differ in what is construed as being com­ these states are rated more positively (Tsai, Blevins, passionate. For instance, most Western concepts of Culture l Views of Negative Affect Conception Experience Expression Which feelings, thoughts, How people feel when they What people do when and behaviors people see others suffering they see ochers suffering view as being compassionate/helpful Figure 21.l 1he cultural shaping of compassion through views of negative affect. TH E CULTURAL SHAPING OF COMPASSION 274 I compassion assume that people should feel the most motivation is associated with both types of help­ compass ion for people whom they can identify with­ ing behavior to similar degrees (Miller & Bersoff, (Batson, O 'Quin, Fultz, Vanderplas, & Isen, 1983), 1994). Together, these studies suggest char culcure whose perspectives they can share (Toi & Batson, may shape people's conceptions of compassion and 1982), and whom they feel similar to (Batson, ocher related scares. Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley, & Birch, 1981; Batson, Fultz, & Schoenrade, 1987). In contrast, Experience Buddhist conceptions of compassion assume that Several studies suggest char the elicicors of sym­ everyone and everything is interconnected (Dalai pathy are similar across cultures: people feel sympa­ Lama, 1997), and therefore, people should be able thy for others who suffer for reasons chat are beyond to feel compassion towards all beings, including their control (e.g., Zhang, Xia, & Li, 2007). In adversaries and transgressors. German, Israeli, Indonesian, and Malaysian con­ Differences in the conceptualization of com­ texts, children expressed sympathy (e.g.,
Recommended publications
  • On the Interaction of Social Affect and Cognition: Empathy, Compassion and Theory of Mind
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind 1 1,2 1 Katrin Preckel , Philipp Kanske and Tania Singer Empathy, compassion and Theory of Mind (ToM) are central which may be detrimental to the observer and to others topics in social psychology and neuroscience. While empathy and compassion, on the other hand, which is a feeling of enables the sharing of others’ emotions and may result in warmth and concern for the other. We conclude with a empathic distress, a maladaptive form of empathic resonance, concise summary and an opinion statement. or compassion, a feeling of warmth and concern for others, ToM provides cognitive understanding of someone else’s Defining and neurally characterizing empathy, thoughts or intentions. These socio-affective and socio- compassion and Theory of Mind cognitive routes to understanding others are subserved by Empathy describes the process of sharing feelings, that is, separable, independent brain networks. Nonetheless they are resonating with someone else’s feelings, regardless of jointly required in many complex social situations. A process valence (positive/negative), but with the explicit knowl- that is critical for both, empathy and ToM, is self-other edge that the other person is the origin of this emotion [1]. distinction, which is implemented in different temporoparietal This socio-affective process results from neural network brain regions. Thus, adaptive social behavior is a result of activations that resemble those activations observed dynamic interplay of socio-affective and socio-cognitive when the same emotion is experienced first-hand (shared processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotionet: an Accurate, Real-Time Algorithm for the Automatic Annotation of a Million Facial Expressions in the Wild
    EmotioNet: An accurate, real-time algorithm for the automatic annotation of a million facial expressions in the wild C. Fabian Benitez-Quiroz*, Ramprakash Srinivasan*, Aleix M. Martinez Dept. Electrical and Computer Engineering The Ohio State University ∗These authors contributed equally to this paper. Abstract it were possible to annotate each face image very fast by an expert coder (say, 20 seconds/image)1, it would take 5,556 Research in face perception and emotion theory requires hours to code a million images, which translates to 694 (8- very large annotated databases of images of facial expres- hour) working days or 2:66 years of uninterrupted work. sions of emotion. Annotations should include Action Units This complexity can sometimes be managed, e.g., in im- (AUs) and their intensities as well as emotion category. age segmentation [18] and object categorization [17], be- This goal cannot be readily achieved manually. Herein, cause everyone knows how to do these annotations with we present a novel computer vision algorithm to annotate minimal instructions and online tools (e.g., Amazon’s Me- a large database of one million images of facial expres- chanical Turk) can be utilized to recruit large numbers of sions of emotion in the wild (i.e., face images downloaded people. But AU coding requires specific expertise that takes from the Internet). First, we show that this newly pro- months to learn and perfect and, hence, alternative solutions posed algorithm can recognize AUs and their intensities re- are needed. This is why recent years have seen a number liably across databases. To our knowledge, this is the first of computer vision algorithms that provide fully- or semi- published algorithm to achieve highly-accurate results in automatic means of AU annotation [20, 10, 22,2, 26, 27,6].
    [Show full text]
  • Finding the Golden Mean: the Overuse, Underuse, and Optimal Use of Character Strengths
    Counselling Psychology Quarterly ISSN: 0951-5070 (Print) 1469-3674 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ccpq20 Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths Ryan M. Niemiec To cite this article: Ryan M. Niemiec (2019): Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths, Counselling Psychology Quarterly, DOI: 10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 Published online: 20 May 2019. Submit your article to this journal View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ccpq20 COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY QUARTERLY https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2019.1617674 ARTICLE Finding the golden mean: the overuse, underuse, and optimal use of character strengths Ryan M. Niemiec VIA Institute on Character, Cincinnati, OH, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY The science of well-being has catalyzed a tremendous amount of Received 28 February 2019 research with no area more robust in application and impact than Accepted 8 May 2019 the science of character strengths. As the empirical links between KEYWORDS character strengths and positive outcomes rapidly grow, the research Character strengths; around strength imbalances and the use of strengths with problems strengths overuse; strengths and conflicts is nascent. The use of character strengths in understand- underuse; optimal use; ing and handling life suffering as well as emerging from it, is particularly second wave positive aligned within second wave positive psychology. Areas of particular psychology; golden mean promise include strengths overuse and strengths underuse, alongside its companion of strengths optimaluse.Thelatterisviewedasthe golden mean of character strengths which refers to the expression of the right combination of strengths, to the right degree, and in the right situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Types of Weltschmerz in German Poetry
    TY PE S O F WE LT S C H M ERZ I N GERMAN POETRY ' L R D B RA N P h D A F E U . WILH ELM , S OM ETI M E F E LLOW I N GE RMAN I C LANGUA GE S AN D U CO L U M B I A U NI V E RS I TY LITERAT RES . mmmm TH E CO LU MB I A UNIVERSITY P RES S TH E MAC MI LLAN COM PANY , A GE NT S L ONDON : MA CM I L LA N Co L TD . 1 905 A ll rig h ts reserved C OLU MBI A UNIV ERSI TY GERMANI C STU DI ES Edite d WI LLI AM C AR P ENT ER an d by H . CALVIN THOMAS V ol I . SCAN DINAVIAN INFLUENCE ON SOUT H E RN W ND S A o b u o t o LO LA C O TCH . C ntri ti n the St u dy o f the Lingu istic Re latio ns o f English an d d a a B O B F OM h D o S a . G O S P . 8v c n in vi n y E RG E T IA L , , P c ne t . a . xv ! 82 . p per , pp ri e , S M B o e a No . OS G . a G 2 IAN IN ER ANY ibli gr phy , en r l ’ Su e Ossi an s n fl u c o n K o s ock an d t he a ds rv y , I en e l p t B r .
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Development of Empathy: How, When, and Why Nicole M. Mcdonald & Daniel S. Messinger University of Miami Department Of
    1 The Development of Empathy: How, When, and Why Nicole M. McDonald & Daniel S. Messinger University of Miami Department of Psychology 5665 Ponce de Leon Dr. Coral Gables, FL 33146, USA 2 Empathy is a potential psychological motivator for helping others in distress. Empathy can be defined as the ability to feel or imagine another person’s emotional experience. The ability to empathize is an important part of social and emotional development, affecting an individual’s behavior toward others and the quality of social relationships. In this chapter, we begin by describing the development of empathy in children as they move toward becoming empathic adults. We then discuss biological and environmental processes that facilitate the development of empathy. Next, we discuss important social outcomes associated with empathic ability. Finally, we describe atypical empathy development, exploring the disorders of autism and psychopathy in an attempt to learn about the consequences of not having an intact ability to empathize. Development of Empathy in Children Early theorists suggested that young children were too egocentric or otherwise not cognitively able to experience empathy (Freud 1958; Piaget 1965). However, a multitude of studies have provided evidence that very young children are, in fact, capable of displaying a variety of rather sophisticated empathy related behaviors (Zahn-Waxler et al. 1979; Zahn-Waxler et al. 1992a; Zahn-Waxler et al. 1992b). Measuring constructs such as empathy in very young children does involve special challenges because of their limited verbal expressiveness. Nevertheless, young children also present a special opportunity to measure constructs such as empathy behaviorally, with less interference from concepts such as social desirability or skepticism.
    [Show full text]
  • Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes?
    Tennessee Advisory Commission on Intergovernmental Relations Staff Information Report January 2003 TACIR Publication Policy Staff Information Reports, Staff Briefs, Staff Technical Reports and Staff Working Papers and TACIR Fast Facts are issued to promote the mission and objectives of the Commission. These reports are intended to share information and research findings relevant to important public policy issues in an attempt to promote wider understanding. Only reports clearly labeled as Commission Reports represent the official position of the Commission. Others are informational. The Tennessee Advisory Commission on Intergovernmental Relations Suite 508, 226 Capitol Boulevard, Nashville,Tennessee 37243 (615) 741-3012 FAX (615) 532-2443 email: [email protected] website: www.state.tn.us/tacir Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes? A Staff Information Report The following staff have contributed to this report: Ed Young, Ph.D. Research Consultant Principal Author Harry A. Green, Ph.D. Executive Director Lynnisse Roehrich-Patrick, J.D. Director of Special Projects Project Manager, Editor Linda Joseph Publications Assistant Teresa Gibson Publications Assistant January 2003 ii Do K-12 School Facilities Affect Education Outcomes? Foreword In 1988, more than half of Tennessee’s school systems joined together and filed a lawsuit charging the state with failing to fund all systems, rich and poor, equitably, and in 1993, they won. In the meantime and after two years of deliberation, the Tennessee General Assembly adopted a new funding formula as part of the Education Improvement Act (EIA) of 1992. The plaintiffs were not entirely satisfied with that remedy and challenged it on three bases: it did not solve the problem of inequity among the systems in teachers’ salaries, it should be funded immediately and not phased in over six years as the legislature planned, and it did not provide adequate funds to bring all school buildings up to modern standards.
    [Show full text]
  • Zest and Work
    Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 30, 161–172 (2009) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/job.584 Zest and work CHRISTOPHER PETERSON1*, NANSOOK PARK2, NICHOLAS HALL3 AND MARTIN E.P.SELIGMAN 3 1University of Michigan, Michigan, U.S.A. 2University of Rhode Island, Rhode Island, U.S.A. 3University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania, U.S.A. Summary Zest is a positive trait reflecting a person’s approach to life with anticipation, energy, and excitement. In the present study, 9803 currently employed adult respondents to an Internet site completed measures of dispositional zest, orientation to work as a calling, and satisfaction with work and life in general. Across all occupations, zest predicted the stance that work was a calling (r ¼.39), as well as work satisfaction (r ¼.46) and general life satisfaction (r ¼.53). Zest deserves further attention from organizational scholars, especially how it can be encouraged in the workplace. Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Your work is to discover your work, and then with all of your heart to give yourself to it.—the Buddha Introduction Recent years have seen a widespread call for the study of work organizations in which people can be well and do well (Cameron, Dutton, & Quinn, 2003; Gardner, Csikszentmihalyi, & Damon, 2001; Luthans, 2003; Wright, 2003). The emergence of the positive perspective within organizational psychology has brought new attention to the venerable topic of work satisfaction (Hoppock, 1935). Satisfaction with the work that one does is seen not just as a contributor to good performance and increased profitability but as a worthy end in its own right (Heslin, 2005).
    [Show full text]
  • Cognition, Affect, and Learning —The Role of Emotions in Learning
    How People Learn: Cognition, Affect, and Learning —The Role of Emotions in Learning Barry Kort Ph.D. and Robert Reilly Ed.D. {kort, reilly}@media.mit.edu formerly MIT Media Lab Draft as of date January 2, 2019 Learning is the quintessential emotional experience. Our species, Homo Sapiens, are the beings who think. We are also the beings who learn, and the beings who simultaneously experience a rich spectrum of affective emotional states, including a selected suite of emotional states specifically and directly related to learning. This proposal reviews previously published research and theoretical models relating emotions to learning and cognition and presents ideas and proposals for extending that research and reducing it to practice. Our perspective The concept of affect in learning (i.e., emotions in learning) is the same pedagogy applied by an athletic coach at a sporting event. A coach recognizes the affective state of an athlete, and, for example, exhorts that athlete toward increased performance (e.g., raises the level of enthusiasm), or, redirects a frustrated athlete to a productive affective state (e.g., instills confidence, or pride). A coach recognizes that an athlete’s affective state is a critical factor during performance; and, when appropriate, a coach will intervene with a meaningful strategy or tactic. Athletic coaches are skilled at recognizing affective states and intervening appropriately. Educators can have the same impact on a learner by understanding a learner’s affective state and intervening with appropriate strategies or tactics that will meaningfully manage and guide a person’s learning journey. There are several learning theories and a great deal of neuroscience/affective research.
    [Show full text]
  • GERMAN LITERARY FAIRY TALES, 1795-1848 by CLAUDIA MAREIKE
    ROMANTICISM, ORIENTALISM, AND NATIONAL IDENTITY: GERMAN LITERARY FAIRY TALES, 1795-1848 By CLAUDIA MAREIKE KATRIN SCHWABE A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2012 1 © 2012 Claudia Mareike Katrin Schwabe 2 To my beloved parents Dr. Roman and Cornelia Schwabe 3 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, I would like to thank my supervisory committee chair, Dr. Barbara Mennel, who supported this project with great encouragement, enthusiasm, guidance, solidarity, and outstanding academic scholarship. I am particularly grateful for her dedication and tireless efforts in editing my chapters during the various phases of this dissertation. I could not have asked for a better, more genuine mentor. I also want to express my gratitude to the other committee members, Dr. Will Hasty, Dr. Franz Futterknecht, and Dr. John Cech, for their thoughtful comments and suggestions, invaluable feedback, and for offering me new perspectives. Furthermore, I would like to acknowledge the abundant support and inspiration of my friends and colleagues Anna Rutz, Tim Fangmeyer, and Dr. Keith Bullivant. My heartfelt gratitude goes to my family, particularly my parents, Dr. Roman and Cornelia Schwabe, as well as to my brother Marius and his wife Marina Schwabe. Many thanks also to my dear friends for all their love and their emotional support throughout the years: Silke Noll, Alice Mantey, Lea Hüllen, and Tina Dolge. In addition, Paul and Deborah Watford deserve special mentioning who so graciously and welcomingly invited me into their home and family. Final thanks go to Stephen Geist and his parents who believed in me from the very start.
    [Show full text]
  • When Cultures Collide: LEADING ACROSS CULTURES
    When Cultures Collide: LEADING ACROSS CULTURES Richard D. Lewis Nicholas Brealey International 31573 01 i-xxiv 1-176 r13rm 8/18/05 2:56 PM Page i # bli d f li 31573 01 i-xxiv 1-176 r13rm 8/18/05 2:56 PM Page ii page # blind folio 31573 01 i-xxiv 1-176 r13rm 8/18/05 2:56 PM Page iii ✦ When Cultures Collide ✦ LEADING ACROSS CULTURES # bli d f li 31573 01 i-xxiv 1-176 r13rm 8/18/05 2:56 PM Page iv page # blind folio 31573 01 i-xxiv 1-176 r13rm 8/18/05 2:56 PM Page v ✦ When Cultures Collide ✦ LEADING ACROSS CULTURES A Major New Edition of the Global Guide Richard D. Lewis # bli d f li 31573 01 i-xxiv 1-176 r13rm 8/18/05 2:56 PM Page vi First published in hardback by Nicholas Brealey Publishing in 1996. This revised edition first published in 2006. 100 City Hall Plaza, Suite 501 3-5 Spafield Street, Clerkenwell Boston, MA 02108, USA London, EC1R 4QB, UK Information: 617-523-3801 Tel: +44-(0)-207-239-0360 Fax: 617-523-3708 Fax: +44-(0)-207-239-0370 www.nicholasbrealey.com www.nbrealey-books.com © 2006, 1999, 1996 by Richard D. Lewis All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced in any manner whatsoever without written permission from the publisher, except in the case of brief quotations embodied in critical articles or reviews. Printed in Finland by WS Bookwell. 10 09 08 07 06 12345 ISBN-13: 978-1-904838-02-9 ISBN-10: 1-904838-02-2 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Lewis, Richard D.
    [Show full text]
  • Risk Aversion and Emotions
    bs_bs_banner Pacific Economic Review, 19: 3 (2014) pp. 296–312 doi: 10.1111/1468-0106.12067 RISK AVERSION AND EMOTIONS YEN NGUYEN Tilburg University CHARLES N. NOUSSAIR* Tilburg University Abstract. We consider the relationship between emotions and decision-making under risk. Specifi- cally, we examine the emotional correlates of risk-averse decisions. In our experiment, individuals’ facial expressions are monitored with face reading software, as they are presented with risky lotteries. We then correlate these facial expressions with subsequent decisions in risky choice tasks. We find that a more positive emotional state is positively correlated with greater risk taking. The strength of a number of emotions, including fear, happiness, anger and surprise, is positively correlated with risk aversion. JEL Classification Code: C9 1. INTRODUCTION The mechanisms that underpin choice under risk have been a primary focus of research in economics, psychology and neuroscience. The traditional approach in economics has been to postulate the existence of a utility function, possessing the expected utility property, that represents an individual’s preferences, and to note that concavity implies a preference for relatively safe lotteries, while convexity corresponds to a preference for relatively risky lotteries. Neuroscien- tists have documented the physiological correlates of risky decision-making. Psychologists and behavioural economists stress the role of emotions such as happiness or fear in guiding individuals’ choices in risky situations (see e.g. Forgas, 1995; Loewenstein and Lerner, 2003; Coget et al., 2011). The focus of the work reported here is to better understand the connection between emotions, which are ultimately psychological terms describing profiles of physiological responses, and the level of risk aversion exhibited in financial decision-making tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Altering the Trajectory of Affect and Affect Regulation: the Impact of Compassion Training
    Mindfulness DOI 10.1007/s12671-017-0773-3 ORIGINAL PAPER Altering the Trajectory of Affect and Affect Regulation: the Impact of Compassion Training Hooria Jazaieri1 & Kelly McGonigal2 & Ihno A. Lee3 & Thupten Jinpa 2 & James R. Doty2,4 & James J. Gross3 & Philippe R. Goldin5 # Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017 Abstract A growing literature has begun to document the lesser use of expressive suppression and greater acceptance effects of compassion training on a variety of important inter- when experiencing stress/anxiety. These results suggest that personal behaviors (e.g., helping behavior). What is not yet interventions such as compassion training may help modulate well understood, however, is what impact compassion training specific affective states and modify the use of and self-efficacy has on affect and affect regulation. To examine this issue, we for specific regulatory strategies. implemented a 9-week compassion training program in which 51 adults provided twice-daily ratings of four affective states Keywords Affect . Affect regulation . Affective trajectories . (anxiety, calm, fatigue, alertness) as well as their desire and Affect dynamics . Self-efficacy . Compassion . Diary study . capability to regulate these affective states. In addition, partic- Experience sampling . Ecological momentary assessment . ipants provided weekly responses regarding five specific reg- Multilevel analysis ulatory strategies. Analysis of day-to-day trajectories of affec- tive experience showed a decrease in anxiety and increase in calmness. Day-to-day trajectories of affect regulation demon- One commonly cited definition of compassion is a Bfeeling strated that participants were more likely to choose to accept that arises in witnessing another’s suffering and that motivates and thus not influence or modulate affective experience (as a subsequent desire to help^ (Goetz et al.
    [Show full text]