The Syllable

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Syllable PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/146148 Please be advised that this information was generated on 2021-09-23 and may be subject to change. The production of syllables in connected speech Monika Baumann THE PRODUCTION OF SYLLABLES IN CONNECTED SPEECH Layout: Monika Baumann Cover design: Bernadette Schmitt Printed and bound by: Benda drukkers, Nijmegen Copyright: ©1995 Monika Baumann No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without prior written permission of the author. Niets uit deze uitgave mag woorden verveelvuldigd en/of openbaar gemaakt door middel van druk, fotokopie, microfilm of welke andere wijze ook, zonder vorafgaande schriftelijke toestemming van de auteur. CIP-GEGEVENS KONINKLIJKE BIBLIOTHEEK, DEN HAAG Baumann, Monika The production of syllables in connected speech / Monika Baumann. - [S.l : s.n.] (Nijmegen : Benda drukkers). - 111. Thesis Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen. - With réf. - With summary in Dutch. ISBN 90-9008946-2 Subject heading: speech production THE PRODUCTION OF SYLLABLES IN CONNECTED SPEECH een wetenschappelijke proeve op het gebied van de Sociale Wetenschappen PROEFSCHRIFT ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen, volgens besluit van het College van Decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op maandag 5 februari 1996 des namiddags te 3.30 uur precies door MONIKA BAUMANN geboren op 27 maart 1967 te Aken (Duitsland) Promotor: Prof. dr. WJ.M. Levelt Co-promotor: Dr. A.S. Meyer The research reported in this thesis was supported by a grant from the Max-Planck- Gesellschaft zur Förderung der Wissenschaften, Munich (Germany) and carried out at the Max-Planck-Institut für Psycholinguistik, Nijmegen (The Netherlands). ISBN 90-9008946-2 TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements viii INTRODUCTION 1 The Syllable 1 The Syllable in Phonetics 3 The Syllable in Phonology 6 The Syllable and Syllabification in Dutch: Single Words 11 Syllabification Across Word Boundaries: Encliticized Forms in Dutch 13 The Syllable in Psycholinguistics 15 The Production of Syllables 21 Phonological Encoding in Speech Production 21 Levelt's Model of Phonological Encoding 25 Predictions of Levelt's Model 28 PART 1: THE TIME COURSE OF SYLLABIFICATION 30 Auditory Syllable Priming in a Ρ air ed-Associate Learning Task: Paradigm, Stimuli, and Hypotheses 30 The Picture-Word Interference Paradigm 30 The Semantic-Associate Learning Task 32 The Targets 33 The IS Conditions 34 Hypotheses 35 Overview of the Syllable Priming Experiments 40 Experiment 1: Single Verbs, 10% Related IS 41 Method 41 Results and Discussion 46 Experiment 2: Single Verbs, 40% Related IS 49 Method 50 Results 50 Reanalysis with the Items of Experiment 1 53 Discussion 55 Experiment 3: Single Verbs, IS Cut Out of Target Form 56 Method 56 Results 58 Discussion 59 Experiment 4: Encliticized Forms, 12.5% Related IS 61 Method 62 Results 65 Discussion 67 ν Experiments 5a,b: Encliticized Forms, 40% Related IS 68 Method of Experiment 5a 69 Results of Experiment 5a 71 Method of Experiment 5b 72 Results of Experiment 5b 74 Discussion 75 Experiment 6: Morphology Control 76 Method 77 Results 80 Discussion 83 Experiment 7: Lexical Control 84 Method 84 Results and Discussion 86 Reanalysis with Pink Noise as a Baseline 87 Summary and Discussion of Part 1 95 PART 2: THE LEVELS OF SYLLABIFICATION 100 Final Devoicing in Encliticized Forms: Phenomenon, Predictions, Paradigm 100 Final Devoicing in Dutch 100 Final Devoicing in Encliticized Forms: Predictions 103 Experimental Studies on Final Devoicing 103 Paradigm 105 Experiment 8a: The First Production Experiment 107 Method 107 Experiment 8b: The First Perception Experiment Ill Method Ill Results 114 Discussion 117 Judging the Materials 119 Experiment 9a: The Second Production Experiment 123 Method 124 Experiment 9b: The Second Perception Experiment 126 Method 126 Results and Discussion 127 Acoustic Measurements 132 Method 133 Results and Discussion 135 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 143 Summary of the Results 143 Implications for Phonological Theory 147 Implications for Phonological Encoding 150 vi REFERENCES 156 APPENDIX A: MATERIALS AND RESULTS 165 Al Experiment 1 165 Al.l Prétest 165 A1.2 Main Experiment 1 166 Al Experiment 2 168 A2.1 Materials 168 A3 Experiment 4 169 A 3.1 Pretest 169 A3.2 Materials of Main Experiment 4 170 A3.3 Mean RTs per Set in Experiment 4 173 A4 Experiment 5 174 A4.1 Pretest 174 A4.2 Materials of Main Experiment 5 175 A5 Experiment 6 176 A5.1 Pretest: Materials 176 A5.2 Pretest: Results 176 A5.3 Materials of Main Experiment 6 177 A6 Experiment 7 179 A6.1 Materials 179 A7.1 Table of Experiments Ito 7 180 A7.2 Difference Scores from Pink Noise Baseline 180 A8 Experiments 8a,b 182 A8.1 Materials of the First Production Experiment (8a) 182 A8.2 Materials of the First Perception Experiment (8b) 184 A8.3 Results for the Voiced Responses ('d' - (4)+(5)) 184 A8.4 Reanalysis of the Responses to Items Judged as Encliticized 185 A9 Experiments 9a,b 185 A9.1 Materials of the Second Production Experiment (9a) 185 A9.2 Materials of the Second Perception Experiment (9b) 186 A9.3 Results for the Voiced Responses ('d' - (4)+(5)) 187 A9.4 Example for a Segmented Speechwave 187 APPENDIX B: TESTS OF SIMPLE EFFECTS 188 SAMENVATTING 191 vii Acknowledgements I've just realized that looking forward to writing the acknowledgements for your thesis in happy anticipation is much easier than actually writing them. Anyway, I owe a lot of thanks to many people who in different ways contributed to the work reported in this book. I would like to thank Pirn Levelt, my promotor, for talking to me about "strategy" at the right moments in time and for his support. Antje Meyer intensively and excellently fed me, the theoretical linguist, with experimental psychology and statistics. As my co-promotor, she carefully read every paper I wrote and always had time to discuss things and came up with many suggestions and critical questions. Co-operating with Antje was both effective and fun. Herbert Baumann and John Nagengast provided valuable technical support for the experiments, Christa Hausmann-Jamin kept the system running, and Ad Verbunt helped optimizing the equipment for writing and printing. Thanks also to the other people from the technical group who were always willing to help, and Richard Piepenbrock of "CELEX". Nora van Eenennaam and especially Paul Verleg called more than 800 participants - I think Paul lost some nanometers of skin dialing all those telephone numbers. Moreover, while waiting at Paul's reception desk for the participants to come (or not to come ...), I learned - between two incoming phone calls - quite a lot about Dutch language, people, history and geography over the years. Jos van Berkum, Geert Booij, Etta Drews, T.A. Hall, Jörg Jescheniak, James McQueen, Ardi Roelofs, Bernadette Schmitt, Herbert Schriefers, Miranda van Turennout, Linda Wheeldon, and Pienie Zwitserlood helped me in thinking about many of the issues in this thesis. Cecile Kuijpers and Daan Broeder gave support with formants and transitions. Silvia Aal, Jos van Berkum, Ger Desserjer, Marc Fleischheuers, Alette Haveman, Paul Verleg, and Marianne Wissink carefully checked lists of Dutch materials for me. Agnes Bolwiender, Ingrid Schophuis, and Miranda van Turennout provided the spoken stimuli. André Luissen ran the sessions of the main experiments of Experiments 1 and 2 when he was a trainee at the Institute. I owe many thanks to Jos van Berkum, who translated the Dutch summary that you find at the end of this book, although he was busy finishing his own thesis at the time. The jumping coda consonant on the front cover would have never come out of its syllable bubble without the graphic skills of Bernadette Schmitt. A whole bunch of nice fellow PhD students and colleagues contributed in many ways towards making my stay at the institute a very pleasant one. Heike Behrens, the excellent cook and perfect host, provided valuable psychological and food support. Jörg Jescheniak and Janpeter de Ruiter, my subsequent officemates, heroically tolerated (and lucky enough survived) my need for fresh air. A final "Danke!" to my family and friends back home for keeping in close contact all the time and cheering me up whenever necessary - and not only then! die zeit vergeht lustig luslustigtig lusluslustigtigtig luslusluslustigtigtigtig lusluslusluslustigtigtigtigtig luslusluslusluslustigtigtigtigtigtig lusluslusluslusluslustigtigtigtigtigtigtig luslusluslusluslusluslustigtigtigtigtigtigtigtig (ernst jandl) INTRODUCTION The syllable is a unit that phoneticians have a hard time defining, but that nevertheless almost all phonologists have incorporated into their theories as a useful concept. Psycholinguists have collected experimental evidence for the syllable. The first part of the Introduction deals with the syllable in phonetics, phonology, and psycholinguistics. It furthermore includes a description of syllables in Dutch. The second part of the Introduction deals with the production of syllables. In particular, Levelt's model of phonological encoding will be introduced. Predictions drawn from this model were tested in the experiments reported in the remaining chapters. The Syllable Imagine you are participating in one of these games where the entertainment increases with the number of friends participating. It is your turn, and you are shown a little card with a concept printed on it that you have to explain without 2 Introduction: The Syllable naming it, while your team has to guess what is printed on your card as quickly as possible. Imagine furthermore, that on the card that was just handed to you it said 'SYLLABLE'. The chance is high that in order to help making your team win, you will not start a report on suprasegmental phonology, sonority hierarchies of phonological segments, or stress assignment - especially since it is rather unlikely that all members of your team are phonologists.
Recommended publications
  • Vowel Quality and Phonological Projection
    i Vowel Quality and Phonological Pro jection Marc van Oostendorp PhD Thesis Tilburg University September Acknowledgements The following p eople have help ed me prepare and write this dissertation John Alderete Elena Anagnostop oulou Sjef Barbiers Outi BatEl Dorothee Beermann Clemens Bennink Adams Bo domo Geert Bo oij Hans Bro ekhuis Norb ert Corver Martine Dhondt Ruud and Henny Dhondt Jo e Emonds Dicky Gilb ers Janet Grijzenhout Carlos Gussenhoven Gert jan Hakkenb erg Marco Haverkort Lars Hellan Ben Hermans Bart Holle brandse Hannekevan Ho of Angeliek van Hout Ro eland van Hout Harry van der Hulst Riny Huybregts Rene Kager HansPeter Kolb Emiel Krah mer David Leblanc Winnie Lechner Klarien van der Linde John Mc Carthy Dominique Nouveau Rolf Noyer Jaap and Hannyvan Oosten dorp Paola Monachesi Krisztina Polgardi Alan Prince Curt Rice Henk van Riemsdijk Iggy Ro ca Sam Rosenthall Grazyna Rowicka Lisa Selkirk Chris Sijtsma Craig Thiersch MiekeTrommelen Rub en van der Vijver Janneke Visser Riet Vos Jero en van de Weijer Wim Zonneveld Iwant to thank them all They have made the past four years for what it was the most interesting and happiest p erio d in mylife until now ii Contents Intro duction The Headedness of Syllables The Headedness Hyp othesis HH Theoretical Background Syllable Structure Feature geometry Sp ecication and Undersp ecicati on Skeletal tier Mo del of the grammar Optimality Theory Data Organisation of the thesis Chapter Chapter
    [Show full text]
  • Using 'North Wind and the Sun' Texts to Sample Phoneme Inventories
    Blowing in the wind: Using ‘North Wind and the Sun’ texts to sample phoneme inventories Louise Baird ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University [email protected] Nicholas Evans ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University [email protected] Simon J. Greenhill ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, The Australian National University & Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History [email protected] Language documentation faces a persistent and pervasive problem: How much material is enough to represent a language fully? How much text would we need to sample the full phoneme inventory of a language? In the phonetic/phonemic domain, what proportion of the phoneme inventory can we expect to sample in a text of a given length? Answering these questions in a quantifiable way is tricky, but asking them is necessary. The cumulative col- lection of Illustrative Texts published in the Illustration series in this journal over more than four decades (mostly renditions of the ‘North Wind and the Sun’) gives us an ideal dataset for pursuing these questions. Here we investigate a tractable subset of the above questions, namely: What proportion of a language’s phoneme inventory do these texts enable us to recover, in the minimal sense of having at least one allophone of each phoneme? We find that, even with this low bar, only three languages (Modern Greek, Shipibo and the Treger dialect of Breton) attest all phonemes in these texts.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstract: Crossing Morpheme Boundaries in Dutch
    Abstract: Crossing morpheme boundaries in Dutch On the basis of Dutch data, this article argues that many differences between types of affixes which are usually described by arbitrary morphological diacritics in the literature, can be made to follow from the phonological shape of affixes. Two cases are studied in some detail: the difference between prefixes and suffixes, and differences between 'cohering' and 'noncohering' suffixes. Prefixes in many languages of the world behave as prosodically more independent than suffixes with respect to syllabification: the former usually do not integrate with the stem, whereas the latter do. Dutch is an example of a language to which this applies. It is argued that this asymmetry does not have to be stipulated, but can be made to follow from the fact that the phonotactic reason for crossing a morpheme boundary usually involves the creation of an onset, which is on the left-hand side of a vowel and not on its right-hand side, plus some general (symmetric) conditions on the interface between phonology and morphology. The differences between 'cohering' and 'noncohering' suffixes with respect to syllable structure and stress in Dutch is argued to similarly follow from the phonological shape of these affixes. Once we have set up an appropriately sophisticated structure for every affix, there is no need any more to stipulate arbitrary morphological markings: differences in phonological behaviour follow from differences in phonological shape alone, given a sufficiently precise theory of universal constraint and their interactions. Keywords: Dutch phonology, phonology-morphology interface, prosody 1 Crossing Morpheme Boundaries in Dutch 1. Introduction Asymmetries in phonological behaviour between types of affixes are not uncommon in languages of the world.1 For instance, prefixes in a given language may behave quite differently from suffixes.
    [Show full text]
  • Finding Phonological Features in Perception
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Finding phonological features in perception Chládková, K. Publication date 2014 Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Chládková, K. (2014). Finding phonological features in perception. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:01 Oct 2021 4 PERCEPTUALSENSITIVITYTOCHANGESINVOWEL DURATIONREVEALSTHESTATUSOFTHE PHONOLOGICALLENGTHFEATURE 4.1 pre-attentive sensitivity to vowel duration reveals native phonology and predicts learning of second- language sounds This section has been published as: KateˇrinaChládková, Paola Escudero, & Silvia Lipski (2013). Pre-attentive sensitivity to vowel duration reveals native phonology and predicts learning of second-language sounds. Brain and Language, 126 (3): 243-252. Abstract In some languages (e.g. Czech), changes in vowel duration affect word meaning, while in others (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Dutch. a Linguistic History of Holland and Belgium
    Dutch. A linguistic history of Holland and Belgium Bruce Donaldson bron Bruce Donaldson, Dutch. A linguistic history of Holland and Belgium. Uitgeverij Martinus Nijhoff, Leiden 1983 Zie voor verantwoording: http://www.dbnl.org/tekst/dona001dutc02_01/colofon.php © 2013 dbnl / Bruce Donaldson II To my mother Bruce Donaldson, Dutch. A linguistic history of Holland and Belgium VII Preface There has long been a need for a book in English about the Dutch language that presents important, interesting information in a form accessible even to those who know no Dutch and have no immediate intention of learning it. The need for such a book became all the more obvious to me, when, once employed in a position that entailed the dissemination of Dutch language and culture in an Anglo-Saxon society, I was continually amazed by the ignorance that prevails with regard to the Dutch language, even among colleagues involved in the teaching of other European languages. How often does one hear that Dutch is a dialect of German, or that Flemish and Dutch are closely related (but presumably separate) languages? To my knowledge there has never been a book in English that sets out to clarify such matters and to present other relevant issues to the general and studying public.1. Holland's contributions to European and world history, to art, to shipbuilding, hydraulic engineering, bulb growing and cheese manufacture for example, are all aspects of Dutch culture which have attracted the interest of other nations, and consequently there are numerous books in English and other languages on these subjects. But the language of the people that achieved so much in all those fields has been almost completely neglected by other nations, and to a degree even by the Dutch themselves who have long been admired for their polyglot talents but whose lack of interest in their own language seems never to have disturbed them.
    [Show full text]
  • Articulatory Complexity and Phoneme Frequency Was Examined in Relation to the Child’S Profile of Motor Or Perceptual Difficulties
    Predictors of consonant development and the development of a test of French phonology by Marianne Paul School of Communication Sciences and Disorders McGill University Montreal, Quebec, Canada A thesis submitted to McGill University in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degree of Master of Science December 2009 © Marianne Paul (2009) Abstract The goal of this study was to predict accuracy of consonant production by French-speaking children with speech sound disorders. Articulatory complexity and phoneme frequency was examined in relation to the child’s profile of motor or perceptual difficulties. The participants were preschoolers receiving speech therapy. Three had difficulties in the motor domain and five had difficulties with speech perception. The percentage of consonants correct on the Test of French Phonology, developed for this study, was calculated. For both groups, the best predictor was the phoneme’s articulatory complexity combined with its phonological context but phoneme frequency was not predictive. The Motor Group had more difficulty with one- and four-syllable words and syllable onsets than the Perceptual Group whereas the Perceptual Group demonstrated lower accuracy for consonants in the syllable coda position. Non-linear phonology as the theoretical framework for the development of the Test of French Phonology was validated. i Résumé L’objectif de cette étude était de prédire la justesse articulatoire (JA) d’enfants francophones ayant un trouble primaire de l’articulation. La complexité articulatoire et la fréquence d’occurrence des consonnes ont été examinées en lien avec le profile de difficultés des participants. Tous étaient d’âge préscolaire et recevaient des services en orthophonie; trois avaient des difficultés motrices et cinq des difficultés perceptuelles.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonological Errors by Dutch Exchange Students in Reading Indonesian Texts
    DOI: 10.9744/kata.19.2.48-54 ISSN 1411-2639 (Print), ISSN 2302-6294 (Online) OPEN ACCESS http://kata.petra.ac.id The Phonological Errors by Dutch Exchange Students in Reading Indonesian Texts Theodorus Yohanes Mustamu1, Henny Putri Saking Wijaya1 1 English Department, Petra Christian University, Surabaya, INDONESIA e-mails: [email protected], [email protected] ABSTRACT In this study, the five non-existing Indonesian sounds in Dutch sound system were observed because these sounds cause a problem. Moreover, the writers analyzed the phonological errors produced by the Dutch exchange students. The theories were from Moeliono and Darwowidjojo (2003) for the Indonesian consonants and from Mennen, Levelt and Gerrits (2006) for the Dutch consonants. The findings show that there were five Indonesian sounds that do not exist in Dutch sound system. Furthermore, the Dutch exchange students produced phonological errors in initial, medial and final positions. In conclusion, the phonological errors in five observed sounds produced by the Dutch exchange students happened because of the L1 transfer and the lack of knowledge of Indonesian consonants. Keywords: Phonological error, Dutch consonants, Indonesian consonants. INTRODUCTION transfer. Negative transfer is a condition when “an L1 structure or rule is used in an L2 utterance and that use When learning a new language, learners have a is inappropriate and considered an “error”” (p.19). In tendency to transfer the knowledge from their L1. Dutch [j] symbol is used to represent the [y] sound There are two kinds of transfer or cross-linguistic (Fenoulhet, 1992, p.12). It means the students used influence: positive and negative transfers.
    [Show full text]
  • Dutch Sonorants
    Dutch sonorants The role of place of articulation in phonotactics Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Trans 10 fax: +31 30 253 6000 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.let.uu.nl/LOT/ Cover photograph: Ben Harris ISBN 90-76864-42-X NUR 632 Copyright © 2003 by Erik Jan van der Torre. All rights reserved. Dutch sonorants The role of place of articulation in phonotactics PROEFSCHRIFT ter verkrijging van de graad van Doctor aan de Universiteit Leiden, op gezag van de Rector Magnificus Dr. D.D. Breimer, hoogleraar in de faculteit der Wiskunde en Natuurwetenschappen en die der Geneeskunde, volgens besluit van het College voor Promoties te verdedigen op donderdag 25 september 2003 klokke 16.15 uur door Erik Jan van der Torre geboren te Dordrecht in 1975 Promotiecommissie promotor: Prof. dr. C.J. Ewen co-promotor: Dr. M. van Oostendorp (Meertens Instituut) referent: Prof. dr. K.D. Rice (University of Toronto) overige leden: Prof. dr. G.E. Booij (Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam) Prof. dr. J.G. Kooij Dr. J.M. van de Weijer Contents Acknowledgements.......................................................................................................... ix 1 Introduction.............................................................................................................. 1 1.1. Manner-based frameworks.............................................................................. 1 1.2. A role for place in phonotactics ...................................................................... 2 1.3. Organisation of
    [Show full text]
  • De Verwerving Van Een Morfologisch Proces: Nederlandse Meervoudsvorming
    De verwerving van een morfologisch proces: Nederlandse meervoudsvorming WIM ZONNEVELD* Abstract This paper addresses the issue of the acquisition of the Dutch plural, against the background of the Pinker-Marcus-Clahsen dual route model for the analysis of morphological processes in natural languages. The Dutch case is special in that two suffixes seem to meet all criteria for the default plural suffix; these suffixes are phonologically conditioned, but cannot be derived from a single underlying phonological form. It may be argued that Dutch is a half- way house between English (phonologically conditioned suffix forms, derivable from a sing- le underlying form) and German (no single majority suffix). Data gleaned from a large sing- le child acquisition file (longitudinal, focusing on 1;5-2;2) support these ideas about Dutch. Moreover, crucial aspects of the pluralization system seem to be acquired at a stage earlier than hitherto suspected, giving phonological constraints the opportunity to affect the ear- liest stages. The paper winds up speculating briefly on a constraint-based interpretation of the Dutch situation. • 1 Inleiding Dit artikel behandelt de verwerving van meervoudsvorming in het Nederlands. Deze onderwerpskeuze bevat drie aparte elementen: meervoudsvorming, het Nederlands en ver- werving. In deze inleiding wordt uitgelegd waarom het de moeite waard is deze drie ele- menten in samenhang te bespreken. Dit zal gebeuren tegen de achtergrond van het ‘dual- route-model’ zoals ontwikkeld in recent werk van Pinker (Pinker 1991, 1994, 1999) en anderen. In het eigenlijke artikel worden verwervingsfeiten uit een Nederlands corpus besproken om te zien hoe die geïnterpreteerd kunnen worden met gebruikmaking van dit model.
    [Show full text]
  • 'Egg, Onion, Ouch! on the Representation of Dutch Diphthongs'
    ‘Egg, Onion, Ouch! On the Representation of Dutch Diphthongs’ Wim Zonneveld & Mieke Trommelen bron Wim Zonneveld & Mieke Trommelen, ‘Egg, Onion, Ouch! On the Representation of Dutch Diphthongs.’ In: Wim Zonneveld et al. (eds.), Studies in Dutch phonology. Martinus Nijhoff, Den Haag 1980 [Dutch Studies, volume 4], p. 265-292. Zie voor verantwoording: http://www.dbnl.org/tekst/zonn004eggo01_01/colofon.htm © 2002 dbnl / Wim Zonneveld & Mieke Trommelen 265 Egg, onion, ouch! On the representation of Dutch diphthongs Wim Zonneveld and Mieke Trommelen It is hard to read the stubborn attacks on this problem without feeling that since it evidently does not lend itself to a traditional phonemic solution at all, application of an entirely different phonological theory will some day make it evaporate (Shetter, 1972: 1390). 1. Introduction The phonological component of a transformational-generative grammar is defined by a set of (partially) ordered rules which link the underlying phonological representation (ideally the output of the syntactic component of the grammar) to the surface phonetic representation (ideally the set of nerval instructions to the speech mechanisms). While logically this linking function of the phonological component could take any form, it was established from the very outset of generative phonology that this form is in fact a very natural one. Generative phonologists expressed this naturalness not, for instance, by limiting the sheer number of rules allowed to appear in any single phonological component - although this is logically a not at all implausible initial hypothesis - but rather by constraining the types of rules that could appear in these components. Thus, Postal (1968) proposed that only maximally general phonological rules appear in the phonological component of any grammar, by dint of his so-called Naturalness Condition: (1) Naturalness Condition: The underlying representation of a form equals its surface phonetic representation, unless one has a reason (a generalization) to deviate.
    [Show full text]
  • Lexical Phonology
    Lexical phonology Marc van Oostendorp December 6, 2005 Background • Until now, we have presented phonological theory as if it is a mono- lithic unit. • However, there is evidence that phonology consists of at least two lay- ers; that in many natural languages there is a difference between phonol- ogy as it is applied within words (lexical phonology) and phonology as it is applied across words (postlexical phonology) 1 Two layers of phonology Consider the following facts of Dutch phonology: (1) /Ik hEb @t/[Ik.hE.p@t] ‘I have it’ /hEi hAd @t/[hEi.hA.t@t] ‘He had it’ /Ik hEb @r/[ik.hE.p@r] ‘I have her’ The question which raises about these examples is: why are the final obstru- ents of heb and had devoiced? We know by now that Dutch has a process of syllable-final devoicing: the feature [voice] is not allowed to appear in the syllable coda. But the strange thing is that in cases such as this, the obstru- ents in question do not appear in the coda: they are in the onset of the next syllable, since Dutch syllables cannot begin with a schwa. The point about these forms of course is that the obstruents are at the end of a syllable if we syllabify these words disregarding their syntactic context. One of the ideas of the theory of Lexical Phonology is that this idea is es- sentially correct: there are (at least) two phonologies — each an Optimality Theoretic system — which are serialised: first we apply phonology to words (lexical phonology) and the output of this is then, after the operation of syn- tax, applied to phrases (postlexical phonology): 1 2 Two layers of phonology (2) input ↓ Lexical Phonology (Gen + Eval) ↓ output of LP = input of P-LP ↓ Post-Lexical Phonology (Gen + Eval) ↓ output In the Dutch case at hand, this works as follows.
    [Show full text]
  • Possessor Truncation in Kinship Terms in Dutch Dialects
    Possessor truncation in kinship terms in Dutch dialects Gertjan Postma TET 69 (2): 159–194 DOI: 10.5117/TET2017.2.POST Abstract Possessor truncation in kinship terms in Dutch dialects In this study we report a hardly noticed, poorly studied, and non-understood property of kinship terms in many Dutch dialects: a distinct, more impover- ished possessor inflection in kinship terms, which was coined “possessor truncation” in Goeman et al. (2008). After reporting dialect-geographical, diachronic, and morphological properties of possessor truncation, we give a morphosyntactic account inspired on determiner drop in kinship terms in Italian. Possessor truncation in Dutch and determiner drop in Italian can be unified under the assumption that kinship terms generate their referential role within in the sub-lexical domain, while ordinary nouns merge these argumental properties in the supra-lexical domain. Keywords: kinship terms, possessive pronouns, inflection, chain reduction, referential role, morphosyntax, distributive morphology, Dutch, Italian 1 Introduction As was observed in Goeman et al. (2008), some Dutch dialects have a deviant possessive pronoun inflection in kinship terms. (1) a onze knecht our-infl servant.masc b ons vader our-ø father.masc VOL. 69, NO. 2, 2017 159 Guest (guest) IP: 170.106.202.126 TAAL & TONGVAL The difference in inflection between onze ‘our’ in (1a) and ons in (1b) cannot be attributed to gender as both nouns are masculine nouns.1 The suggested dimension seems to be that masculine kinship terms show this “subtractive effect”, as Goeman et al (2008) call it. Goeman et al, however, leave open the possibility that it is a phonological effect, as a voiceless plosive in (1a) might have a different effect on a preceding schwa than a voiced fricative in (1b).
    [Show full text]