Sensory Overresponsivity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Sensory Overresponsivity CHAPTER 19 Sensory Overresponsivity Timothy W. Soto Vivian M. Ciaramitaro Alice S. Carter Sensory overresponsivity (SOR) is character- Theoretical Orientations of Sensory Processing ized by extreme or atypical negative reactions to sensory stimuli across one or more sensory Sensory processing issues were first described domains (Dunn, 1999; Interdisciplinary Coun- in 1963 by Anna Jean Ayres, who was trained cil on Developmental and Learning Disorders as an occupational therapist and educational [ICDL], 2005; Lane, 2002; Parham & Mailloux, psychologist, to explain, assess, and treat the 2004; Reynolds & Lane, 2008). Broadly, there behavioral and learning challenges experi- are several hypothesized models that explain enced and expressed by children with various the neurological and behavioral underpinnings disabilities by relating neurological processing of atypical sensory processing. SOR is one par- of and behavioral responses to sensory stimuli ticular manifestation of sensory dysfunction; (Miller, Anzalone, Lane, Cermak, & Osten, it often co-occurs with psychiatric disorders 2007). Ayres (1985) used the term “sensory (i.e., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder integration” (SI) to describe these phenomena. [ADHD], generalized anxiety disorder, oppo- Underlying the developmental component of sitional defiant disorder) (Ben-Sasson, Soto, SI theory is the assumption that learning stems Heberle, Carter, & Briggs-Gowan, 2017; Carter, from one’s ability to detect, process, and use Ben-Sasson, & Briggs-Gowan, 2011; Conelea, sensory information from the environment and Carter, & Freeman, 2014) and is characterized from one’s movement to plan and organize be- as a possible symptom of autism spectrum dis- havior (Bundy, Lane, & Murray, 2002). More order (ASD) within the repetitive and restrictive specifically, individuals differ in their way of behavior criteria of DSM-5 (American Psychi- processing information from the visual, audito- atric Association, 2013). SOR also occurs and is ry, tactile, vestibular, proprioceptive, gustatory, associated with clinical impairment without co- and olfactory senses (Huebner & Dunn, 2001). morbid psychiatric or neurocognitive disorders Since Ayres’s initial work in SI, theoretical (Carter et al., 2011). We review in this chapter models and research evidence for this construct the theoretical orientations that have framed have been further developed, and through the SOR as a disorder, and current developmental advancement of such models, the operational- and neurocognitive understandings of sensory ization and clinical characterization of SOR has processing within and across the senses (i.e., become better understood. multimodal processing), and discuss potential The following three described models that Copyright @ 2019. The Guilford Press. All rights reserved. May not be reproduced in any form without permission from the publisher, except fair uses permitted under U.S. or applicable copyright law. future directions for research. have evolved from Ayres’s initial work provide 330 EBSCO : eBook Collection (EBSCOhost) - printed on 7/2/2019 3:12 PM via TUFTS UNIV AN: 1843598 ; Zeanah, Charles H..; Handbook of Infant Mental Health, Fourth Edition Account: s3579704.main.ehost 19. Sensory Overresponsivity 331 the foundation for the primary ways in which sponsivity (SOR), which describes exaggerated, sensory processing, particularly SOR, is cur- rapid onset and/or prolonged reactions to senso- rently conceptualized and measured. First, ry stimulation (e.g., distress from loud noises); Winnie Dunn’s (1997) conceptual model of sen- (2) sensory underresponsivity (SUR), which ex- sory processing combines neurological thresh- plains unawareness or slow response to sensory olds of responding (low or high) with self-reg- input (e.g., not noticing extreme changes in tem- ulation behavioral strategies (passive or active), perature); and (3) sensory seeking (SS), which and includes four hypothesized patterns: sen- describes craving of, and interest in, sensory sation seeking, sensation avoiding, poor reg- experiences that are prolonged or intense (e.g., istration, and sensitivity to stimuli (i.e., SOR). engaging in repeated, rhythmical movements) The model suggests that individuals with low (ICDL, 2005; Miller, Anzalone, et al., 2007). neurological thresholds need less stimulation These patterns are not mutually exclusive and than those with high neurological thresholds to may co-occur in individual children across notice and respond to input. Indeed, high levels sensory modalities (Baranek et al., 2006; Liss, of stimulation may be aversive for individuals Saulnier, Fein, & Kinsbourne, 2006). For our with low neurological thresholds. Individuals purposes in this chapter, while the previously with active self-regulation strategies attempt to stated terms are used when referencing specific control sensory input through active behaviors, studies, the term “sensory dysfunction” is used whereas those with passive self-regulation strat- to describe abnormal sensory processing in a egies respond to the input as it occurs. Varia- general sense. tions of these four patterns are common in the broader population and do not always represent dysfunction. Mechanisms Underlying Sensory Dysfunction Second, Baranek, Reinhartsen and Wan- namaker (2001) developed a model of sensory Although sensory dysfunction may involve processing, which postulates that hyper- and hypersensitivity, hyposensitivity, or an atypi- hyporesponsive sensory modulation behavioral cal interaction and/or integration of informa- patterns may result from shifts in two sensory tion between sensory systems, the majority of processing thresholds: sensory aversion and studies considering the neural basis of sensory sensory orienting. According to their model, dysfunction in developmental disorders have optimal engagement in play and other activi- focused on a single sensory modality and on ties is determined by the width between the two neural mechanisms underlying hypersensitiv- thresholds. Moreover, Baranek and colleagues ity, or SOR. In SOR, behavioral responses to theorize that the width between the orientation sensory stimuli across one or more sensory do- and aversion thresholds is determined by both mains are exaggerated or negative (Dunn, 1999; child characteristics (e.g., attention, affect) and ICDL, 2005; Lane, 2002; Parham & Mailloux, external factors (e.g., environmental and con- 2004; Reynolds & Lane, 2008; Zero to Three, textual features, caregiver interactions). They 2016). This may arise from the nervous system further suggest that this optimal band of en- overenhancing or failing to weaken neural re- gagement may be narrower in individuals who sponsiveness to sensory input. have sensory dysfunction (e.g., children with SOR or autism), leading to variations and fluc- Atypical Sensory Adaptation tuations in responses. Third, and more recently, Miller, Anzalone, Evidence suggests that neural mechanisms for and colleagues (2007) proposed a diagnostic weakening neural responses to sensory input, classification of sensory dysfunction called sen- sensory adaptation, may be dysfunctional in sory processing disorder (SPD), which includes several disorders when SOR is comorbid, such three patterns: (1) sensory discrimination disor- as in children with ASD, and individuals with der (SDD)—difficulty processing and/or inter- social anxiety or schizophrenia. Adaptation is preting sensory information; (2) sensory-based a powerful mechanism, allowing our nervous motor disorder (SMBD)—a motor challenge systems to remain most sensitive and respon- with an underlying sensory basis, such as pos- sive to novel, nonredundant, information via a tural disorder and dyspraxia; and (3) sensory weakening or dampening of responsiveness to modulation disorder (SMD). SMD disorder is repeated, redundant stimuli. For example, adap- Copyright @ 2019. The Guilford Press. All rights reserved. May not be reproduced in any form without permission from the publisher, except fair uses permitted under U.S. or applicable copyright law. classified into three types: (1) sensory over-re- tive mechanisms contribute to our ability to dis- EBSCO : eBook Collection (EBSCOhost) - printed on 7/2/2019 3:12 PM via TUFTS UNIV AN: 1843598 ; Zeanah, Charles H..; Handbook of Infant Mental Health, Fourth Edition Account: s3579704.main.ehost 332 IV. Psychopathology tinguish between many exemplars of similar study, 11-year-old children were asked to dis- faces (Clifford & Rhodes, 2005). Adapting to criminate two facial identities when they had or noninformation stimuli also helps to highlight had not been previously adapted to an opposite the information in our environment of the high- identity face. Exposure to a given face identity est behavioral relevance, in which the allocation biased perception, resulting in an aftereffect of limited attentional and processing resources biased toward the opposite identity face. This should be optimized. Thus, a failure of adap- design allowed investigators to determine the tation maintains heightened responsiveness to degree of adaptation to faces independent of stimuli, failing to adjust the gain or sensitivity the ability of children to discriminate between to sensory inputs. A failure of adaptation also faces. While face identification was unimpaired limits the reprioritization of sensory process- in children with ASD—they could discriminate ing resources and the redirecting of attention to facial identities as well as controls—they exhib- allow recent
Recommended publications
  • Guide to Sensory Processing.Pdf
    Guide to Sensory Processing Prepared by Allison Travnik, MSOTS Level II Fieldwork Student Project Kavitha N Krishnan MS OTR/L Fieldwork Instructor Sensory Processing In order to understand what is going on around us, we need to organize all of the incoming sensory information (Ayres, 2005). The sensory information involves what we see, smell, taste, hear, feel on our body, where our body is in relation to others, and how well we are balanced. This is a lot of information that our brains need to process in order to engage in productive behavior, learn, and form accurate perceptions. Proprioceptive Where are body is in space Tactile Auditory What we feel The noise on our skin around us Sensory Smell Processing The Sight difference What we see scents around us around us Oral Sensory Processing Vestibular The sensations Jean Ayres developed the sensory Our sense of Disorder + balance that food give integration (SI) theory. SI gives us in our mouth meaning to what our senses are recognizing. When the sensations are not being organized properly may notice some of the same qualities in the brain, Ayres compared it to about yourself.It is important to a traffic jam. The traffic jam of remember that everyone has some sensory information can lead to quirks about their sensory processing learning difficulties and problem whether it be a sensitivity to loud behavior (Ayres, 2005). Children noises or dislike of light touch. with Sensory Processing Disorder However the identification of SPD is (SPD) are struggling with this reserved for individuals whose traffic jam. sensory quirks are outside of the Sensory processing is a typical range and affect their daily dynamic and complex theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Sensory Processing: Looking at Children's Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing
    Understanding Sensory Processing: Looking at Children’s Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing Communities of Practice in Autism September 24, 2009 Charlottesville, VA Dianne Koontz Lowman, Ed.D. Early Childhood Coordinator Region 5 T/TAC James Madison University MSC 9002 Harrisonburg, VA 22807 [email protected] ______________________________________________________________________________ Dianne Koontz Lowman/[email protected]/2008 Page 1 Looking at Children’s Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing Do you know a child like this? Travis is constantly moving, pushing, or chewing on things. The collar of his shirt and coat are always wet from chewing. When talking to people, he tends to push up against you. Or do you know another child? Sierra does not like to be hugged or kissed by anyone. She gets upset with other children bump up against her. She doesn’t like socks with a heel or toe seam or any tags on clothes. Why is Travis always chewing? Why doesn’t Sierra liked to be touched? Why do children react differently to things around them? These children have different ways of reacting to the things around them, to sensations. Over the years, different terms (such as sensory integration) have been used to describe how children deal with the information they receive through their senses. Currently, the term being used to describe children who have difficulty dealing with input from their senses is sensory processing disorder. _____________________________________________________________________ Sensory Processing Disorder
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Gain Modulation by Closed-Loop Environmental Feedback
    RESEARCH ARTICLE A theory of how active behavior stabilises neural activity: Neural gain modulation by closed-loop environmental feedback Christopher L. Buckley1,2*, Taro Toyoizumi1* 1 Laboratory for Neural Computation and Adaptation, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Saitama, Japan, 2 Department of Informatics and Engineering, University of Sussex, Falmer, United Kingdom * [email protected] (CLB); [email protected] (TT) a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 Abstract a1111111111 During active behaviours like running, swimming, whisking or sniffing, motor actions shape sensory input and sensory percepts guide future motor commands. Ongoing cycles of sen- sory and motor processing constitute a closed-loop feedback system which is central to motor control and, it has been argued, for perceptual processes. This closed-loop feedback OPEN ACCESS is mediated by brainwide neural circuits but how the presence of feedback signals impacts Citation: Buckley CL, Toyoizumi T (2018) A theory on the dynamics and function of neurons is not well understood. Here we present a simple of how active behavior stabilises neural activity: Neural gain modulation by closed-loop theory suggesting that closed-loop feedback between the brain/body/environment can mod- environmental feedback. PLoS Comput Biol 14(1): ulate neural gain and, consequently, change endogenous neural fluctuations and responses e1005926. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. to sensory input. We support this theory with modeling and data analysis in two vertebrate pcbi.1005926 systems. First, in a model of rodent whisking we show that negative feedback mediated by Editor: Daniel Bush, University College London, whisking vibrissa can suppress coherent neural fluctuations and neural responses to sen- UNITED KINGDOM sory input in the barrel cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Sensory Defensiveness? by Ann Stensaas, M.S., OTR/L
    Super Duper® Handy Handouts!® Number 174 What Is Sensory Defensiveness? by Ann Stensaas, M.S., OTR/L Does your child get upset by tags in clothing, the sound of the vacuum cleaner, or certain smells in the environment? If so, your child may be showing signs of sensory defensiveness. Sensory defensiveness is a negative reaction to one or more types of sensations (such as touch, movement, sound, taste/texture, or smell), often requiring you to control his/her daily routine to avoid such things. Types of Sensory Defensiveness There are different types of sensory defensiveness including tactile (touch), gravitational (movement and balance), auditory (hearing), and oral defensiveness (taste, smell, texture). Tactile Defensiveness (Touch) The tactile system is our sense of touch. It protects us from danger and helps us identify different objects in the environment. A child showing signs of tactile defensiveness may: Overreact to ordinary touch experiences (e.g., touching play dough or being touched by someone). Avoid daily activities (e.g., washing face/hands or brushing hair). Avoid light touch (e.g., a kiss) but seek out deep touch (e.g., a bear hug). Vestibular Insecurity (Balance/Movement) The vestibular system is our sense of movement and balance. It tells us where our head and body are in relation to gravity and other objects and supports our vision, posture, emotions, and coordination skills. A child showing signs of gravitational insecurity may: Have an excessive fear of falling during ordinary movement activities (e.g., swinging, riding a bicycle, or climbing). Become overwhelmed by changes in head position (e.g., being upside down).
    [Show full text]
  • Prenatal Stress Induces Schizophrenia-Like Alterations of Serotonin 2A and Metabotropic Glutamate 2 Receptors in the Adult Offspring: Role of Maternal Immune System
    1088 • The Journal of Neuroscience, January 16, 2013 • 33(3):1088–1098 Neurobiology of Disease Prenatal Stress Induces Schizophrenia-Like Alterations of Serotonin 2A and Metabotropic Glutamate 2 Receptors in the Adult Offspring: Role of Maternal Immune System Terrell Holloway,1 Jose´ L. Moreno,1 Adrienne Umali,1 Vinayak Rayannavar,1 Georgia E. Hodes,3 Scott J. Russo,3,4 and Javier Gonza´lez-Maeso1,2,4 Departments of 1Psychiatry, 2Neurology, and 3Neuroscience and 4Friedman Brain Institute, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York 10029 It has been suggested that severe adverse life events during pregnancy increase the risk of schizophrenia in the offspring. The serotonin 5-HT2A and the metabotropic glutamate 2 (mGlu2) receptors both have been the target of considerable attention regarding schizophrenia and antipsychotic drug development. We tested the effects of maternal variable stress during pregnancy on expression and behavioral functionofthesetworeceptorsinmice.Prenatalstressincreased5-HT2A anddecreasedmGlu2expressioninfrontalcortex,abrainregion involved in perception, cognition, and mood. This pattern of expression of 5-HT2A and mGlu2 receptors was consistent with behavioral alterations, including increased head-twitch response to the hallucinogenic 5-HT2A agonist DOI [1-(2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodophenyl)-2- aminopropane] and decreased mGlu2-dependent antipsychotic-like effect of the mGlu2/3 agonist LY379268 (1R,4R,5S,6R-2-oxa-4- aminobicyclo[3.1.0]hexane-4,6-dicarboxylate) in adult, but not prepubertal, mice born to stressed mothers during pregnancy. Cross-fosteringstudiesdeterminedthatthesealterationswerenotattributabletoeffectsofprenatalstressonmaternalcare.Additionally, a similar pattern of biochemical and behavioral changes were observed in mice born to mothers injected with polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid [poly(I:C)] during pregnancy as a model of prenatal immune activation.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Integration: Understanding and Meeting Your Child’S Needs
    Sensory Integration: Understanding and Meeting Your Child’s Needs Sandra Falk,OTR Carrie D. Gross, PTA Le Bonheur Early Intervention and Development (LEAD) What are the Senses? The common senses are: Taste Smell Vision Hearing Touch Two hidden senses are: Vestibular (sense of movement) Proprioception (sensations from muscles and joints). What is Sensory Integration? It is our ability to take in information from the world around us through our senses, sort it out in the brain, and respond to it successfully. It takes place automatically, without us having to “think” about it. How Does Sensory Integration Develop? Sensory integration is pre-programmed to develop from conception However, it takes sensory experiences to activate and/or enhance this process We are designed to enjoy things that promote the development of our brain, and therefore seek out sensations that help organize our brain All of us have sensory preferences and “issues” that we accommodate for on a daily basis. It is when sensory difficulties interfere with everyday life that it becomes a problem. When there is a problem: If the brain is unable to understand and process sensory information efficiently, then that child may have a difficult time functioning in every day life. The child may be awkward and clumsy, fearful and withdrawn, or hostile and aggressive. It can affect how a child moves and learns, how he behaves, how he plays and makes friends, and how he feels about himself. A lot children have some sensory issues, and would benefit from their caregivers understanding their sensory needs. If the problems are severe and interfere with everyday life, a therapist can evaluate and help establish a plan of care.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Perception in Autism
    REVIEWS Sensory perception in autism Caroline E. Robertson1,2,3* and Simon Baron-Cohen4 Abstract | Autism is a complex neurodevelopmental condition, and little is known about its neurobiology. Much of autism research has focused on the social, communication and cognitive difficulties associated with the condition. However, the recent revision of the diagnostic criteria for autism has brought another key domain of autistic experience into focus: sensory processing. Here, we review the properties of sensory processing in autism and discuss recent computational and neurobiological insights arising from attention to these behaviours. We argue that sensory traits have important implications for the development of animal and computational models of the condition. Finally, we consider how difficulties in sensory processing may relate to the other domains of behaviour that characterize autism. Cognitive empathy The ability to reflect on our own and others’ thoughts However, the issue of primacy is key. Is autism, as The ability to understand and and emotions (that is, theory of mind) is a defining often posited, a disorder of the ‘social brain’ (REF. 15), with respond appropriately to characteristic of human cognition. Children with sensory differences representing either secondary con‑ others’ mental states and autism spectrum conditions (ASCs; henceforth ‘autism’) sequences after a lifetime of reduced social interaction emotions (unlike affective 1 empathy, the ability to respond show delays in the development of this capacity , with or alterations in domain‑general mechanisms (such as 2 with an appropriate emotion to knock‑on consequences for cognitive empathy across attention) that affect both social processing and sensory others’ mental states or the lifespan.
    [Show full text]
  • Prenatal Stress and Child Development: a Scoping Review of Research in Low- and Middle-Income Countries
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Prenatal stress and child development: A scoping review of research in low- and middle-income countries Giavana Buffa1, Salome Dahan2, Isabelle Sinclair3, Myriane St-Pierre3, Noushin Roofigari4, 5 6 3 Dima Mutran , Jean-Jacques RondeauID , Kelsey Needham DancauseID * 1 Ross University School of Medicine, Portsmouth, Dominica, 2 University of Haifa, School of Public Health, Haifa, Israel, 3 UQAM, DeÂpartement des sciences de l'activite physique, MontreÂal, Canada, 4 Universite Laval, Faculte de pharmacie, QueÂbec City, Canada, 5 E cole des hautes eÂtudes commerciales, DeÂpartement a1111111111 de sciences de la gestion, MontreÂal, Canada, 6 Universite du QueÂbec à MontreÂal (UQAM), Bibliothèque des a1111111111 Sciences, MontreÂal, Canada a1111111111 a1111111111 * [email protected] a1111111111 Abstract OPEN ACCESS Citation: Buffa G, Dahan S, Sinclair I, St-Pierre M, Introduction Roofigari N, Mutran D, et al. (2018) Prenatal stress Past research has shown relationships between stress during pregnancy, and related psy- and child development: A scoping review of research in low- and middle-income countries. chosocial health measures such as anxiety and depressive symptoms, with infant, child, PLoS ONE 13(12): e0207235. https://doi.org/ and adult outcomes. However, most research is from high-income countries. We conducted 10.1371/journal.pone.0207235 a scoping review to identify research studies on prenatal stress and outcomes of the preg- Editor: Thomas M. Olino, Temple University, nancy or offspring in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), and to synthesize the UNITED STATES stress measures and outcomes assessed, the findings observed, and directions for future Received: June 7, 2018 research. Accepted: October 27, 2018 Published: December 28, 2018 Methods Copyright: © 2018 Buffa et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Processing Disorder
    Sensory Integration Sensory processing (sometimes called "sensory integration" or SI) is a term that refers to the way the nervous system receives messages from the senses and turns them into appropriate motor and behavioral responses. Whether you are biting into a hamburger, riding a bicycle, or reading a book, your successful completion of the activity requires processing sensation or "sensory integration." Sensory Processing Disorder (SPD, formerly known as "sensory integration dysfunction") is a condition that exists when sensory signals don't get organized into appropriate responses. Pioneering occupational therapist and neuroscientist A. Jean Ayres, PhD, likened SPD to a neurological "traffic jam" that prevents certain parts of the brain from receiving the information needed to interpret sensory information correctly. A person with SPD finds it difficult to process and act upon information received through the senses, which creates challenges in performing countless everyday tasks. Motor clumsiness, behavioral problems, anxiety, depression, school failure, and other impacts may result if the disorder is not treated effectively. One study (Ahn, Miller, Milberger, McIntosh, 2004) shows that at least 1 in 20 children’s daily lives is affected by SPD. Another research study by the Sensory Processing Disorder Scientific Work Group (Ben-Sasson, Carter, Briggs-Gowen, 2009) suggests that 1 in every 6 children experiences sensory symptoms that may be significant enough to affect aspects of everyday life functions. Symptoms of Sensory Processing Disorder, like those of most disorders, occur within a broad spectrum of severity. While most of us have occasional difficulties processing sensory information, for children and adults with SPD, these difficulties are chronic, and they disrupt everyday life.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Prenatal Stress and Poverty on Fetal Growth
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 12-2014 Effects of Prenatal Stress and Poverty on Fetal Growth Teresa Anne Lefmann University of Tennessee - Knoxville, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss Part of the Social Work Commons Recommended Citation Lefmann, Teresa Anne, "Effects of Prenatal Stress and Poverty on Fetal Growth. " PhD diss., University of Tennessee, 2014. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss/3148 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a dissertation written by Teresa Anne Lefmann entitled "Effects of Prenatal Stress and Poverty on Fetal Growth." I have examined the final electronic copy of this dissertation for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, with a major in Social Work. Terri Combs-Orme, Major Professor We have read this dissertation and recommend its acceptance: John Orme, Rebecca Bolen, Matthew Cooper Accepted for the Council: Carolyn R. Hodges Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School (Original signatures are on file with official studentecor r ds.) Effects of Prenatal Stress and Poverty on Fetal Growth A Dissertation Presented for the Doctor of Philosophy Degree The University of Tennessee, Knoxville Teresa Anne Lefmann December 2014 Dedication This work is dedicated to all the women in Tennessee who have suffered stress disproportionately due to their standing in society and to their babies who deserve the love and nurture required to develop to their full potential.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Processing Disorder Facts
    Sensory Processing Disorder Facts: Sensory processing disorder is a condition in which the brain has trouble receiving and responding to information that comes in through the senses. The condition used to be called sensory integration dysfunction. Sensory Processing involves the brains ability to organize and understand an array of incoming sensory information entering the brain at the same time. Sensory processing is fundamental to the development of all motor and social skills. This is a filtering system to determine pathways for incoming sensory information. There are the familiar senses of sight, taste, smell, and hearing, but sensory processing involves 3 additional specialized sensory systems, which are very influential with regard to how effectively we recognize and organize incoming sensory information. These are: •Tactile System: how our body perceives touch •Proprioceptive System: how are body perceives where we are in space •Vestibular System: how are body perceives directionality and sense of movement. Some people with sensory processing disorder are oversensitive to things in their environment. Common sounds may be painful or overwhelming. The light touch of a shirt may chafe the skin. This could manifest in: • Being uncoordinated • Bumping into things • Being unable to tell where their limbs are in space • Being hard to engage in conversation or play Sensory processing problems are usually identified in children. But they can also affect adults. Sensory processing problems are commonly seen in developmental disorders like autism. Sensory processing disorder is not recognized as a stand- alone disorder. But many experts think that should change. Sensory processing disorder may affect one sense, like hearing, touch, or taste.
    [Show full text]
  • Impact of Maternal Undernutrition on the Hypothalamic–Pituitary– Adrenal Axis Responsiveness in Sheep at Different Ages Postnatal
    495 Impact of maternal undernutrition on the hypothalamic–pituitary– adrenal axis responsiveness in sheep at different ages postnatal S E Chadio1, B Kotsampasi1, G Papadomichelakis2, S Deligeorgis3, D Kalogiannis1, I Menegatos1 and G Zervas2 1Laboratory of Anatomy and Physiology of Domestic Animals, 2Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, 3Laboratory of Animal Breeding, Department of Animal Science, Agricultural University of Athens, 75, Iera odos, 11855 Athens, Greece (Requests for offprints should be addressed to S E Chadio; Email: [email protected]) Abstract Epidemiological and experimental data support the lower (P!0.05) in group R2 compared with control. Birth hypothesis of ‘fetal programming’, which proposes that weight of lambs was not affected by the maternal nutritional alterations in fetal nutrition and endocrine status lead to manipulation. The area under the curve for ACTH and permanent adaptations in fetal homeostatic mechanisms, cortisol response to CRH challenge was greater (P!0.05) in producing long-term changes in physiology and determine lambs of group R1 at two months of age, whereas no susceptibility to later disease. Altered hypothalamic–pituitary– difference was detected at the ages of 5.5 and 10 months. adrenal (HPA) axis function has been proposed to play an However, significantly higher (P!0.01) basal cortisol levels important role in programming of disease risk. The aim of the were observed in lambs of R1 group at 5.5 months of age. present study was to examine the effects of maternal nutrient There was no interaction between treatment and sex for both restriction imposed during different periods of gestation on pituitary and adrenal responses to the challenge.
    [Show full text]