Defining the Role of Attention in Hierarchical Auditory Processing

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Defining the Role of Attention in Hierarchical Auditory Processing Review Defining the Role of Attention in Hierarchical Auditory Processing Caitlin N. Price 1,2,* and Deborah Moncrieff 1,2 1 Institute for Intelligent Systems, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152, USA; [email protected] 2 School of Communication Sciences & Disorders, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: Communication in noise is a complex process requiring efficient neural encoding through- out the entire auditory pathway as well as contributions from higher-order cognitive processes (i.e., attention) to extract speech cues for perception. Thus, identifying effective clinical interventions for individuals with speech-in-noise deficits relies on the disentanglement of bottom-up (sensory) and top-down (cognitive) factors to appropriately determine the area of deficit; yet, how attention may interact with early encoding of sensory inputs remains unclear. For decades, attentional the- orists have attempted to address this question with cleverly designed behavioral studies, but the neural processes and interactions underlying attention’s role in speech perception remain unresolved. While anatomical and electrophysiological studies have investigated the neurological structures contributing to attentional processes and revealed relevant brain–behavior relationships, recent elec- trophysiological techniques (i.e., simultaneous recording of brainstem and cortical responses) may provide novel insight regarding the relationship between early sensory processing and top-down at- tentional influences. In this article, we review relevant theories that guide our present understanding of attentional processes, discuss current electrophysiological evidence of attentional involvement in auditory processing across subcortical and cortical levels, and propose areas for future study that will inform the development of more targeted and effective clinical interventions for individuals with speech-in-noise deficits. Citation: Price, C.N.; Moncrieff, D. Keywords: speech perception in noise; central auditory deficits; theories of attention; electrophysiology Defining the Role of Attention in Hierarchical Auditory Processing. Audiol. Res. 2021, 11, 112–128. https://doi.org/10.3390/audiolres 1. Introduction 11010012 Attentional selection of relevant inputs is a phenomenon that has been extensively studied in the auditory, somatosensory, and visual sensory modalities. In the auditory Received: 5 February 2021 Accepted: 10 March 2021 domain, attentional selection applies in everyday communicative interactions as the listener Published: 13 March 2021 must attend to signals of interest while ignoring, or suppressing, competing signals present in the soundscape. Difficulties extracting important speech information from background Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral noise have been linked to a variety of communication disorders in individuals of all ages with regard to jurisdictional claims in for several decades. In children, these difficulties may impair a child’s ability to access published maps and institutional affil- the phonological structure of language [1,2] and contribute to developmental learning iations. and reading difficulties [3]. In adults with [4,5] and without hearing impairment [6,7], difficulties hearing and communicating in noise may underlie greater social isolation [8], depression [9], poorer quality of life [10], and accelerated cognitive decline [11,12]. Both bottom-up perceptual and top-down cognitive processes contribute to a listener’s ability to extract a salient auditory signal from a competing stream of background informa- Copyright: © 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. tion. Uni- and bi-directional components of the auditory brain that contribute to adequate This article is an open access article processing of speech in background noise mature at different rates across development distributed under the terms and and are subject to a variety of acquired factors across adulthood and aging. Moore [13] conditions of the Creative Commons noted that the ascending pathway matures early in most children with normal hearing but Attribution (CC BY) license (https:// some children develop auditory perceptual skills as late as 10–12 years of age due to slower creativecommons.org/licenses/by/ maturation of top-down mechanisms [13,14]. 4.0/). Audiol. Res. 2021, 11, 112–128. https://doi.org/10.3390/audiolres11010012 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/audiolres Audiol. Res. 2021, 11, FOR PEER REVIEW 2 Audiol. Res. 2021, 11 Moore [13] noted that the ascending pathway matures early in most children with normal113 hearing but some children develop auditory perceptual skills as late as 10–12 years of age due to slower maturation of top-down mechanisms [13,14]. BothBoth MooreMoore [[15]15] andand HumesHumes [[16]16] attributeattribute difficultiesdifficulties inin auditoryauditory perception,perception, partic-partic- ularlyularly inin complexcomplex listening situations, to to underlying underlying pathologies pathologies within within various various levels levels of ofthe the auditory auditory system system (depicted (depicted in Figure in Figure 1). Humes1). Humes [16] describes [ 16] describes these as these the peripheral, as the pe- ripheral,central-auditory, central-auditory, and cognitive and cognitive hypotheses. hypotheses. The peripheral The peripheral hypothesis hypothesis suggests suggests that thatchanges changes in the in peripheral the peripheral auditory auditory system system result resultin poorer in poorerperipheral peripheral encoding encoding of acoustic of acousticsignals degrading signals degrading the quality the of quality speech of inputs speech to inputs higher to processing higher processing centers. centers. The central- The central-auditoryauditory hypothesis hypothesis posits positsthat structural that structural or functional or functional changes changes within within the auditory the auditory path- pathwayway from from the brainstem the brainstem to auditory to auditory cortex cortex result result in altered in altered neural neural transmission transmission and and fea- featureture extraction extraction vital vital for for the the optimization optimization of cu ofes cues (i.e., (i.e., temporal, temporal, spectral, spectral, spatial) spatial) to im- to improveprove speech speech understanding understanding in incomplex complex listening listening environments. environments. Lastly, Lastly, the the cognitive cognitive hy- hypothesispothesis implies implies that that a ageneral general cognitive cognitive defi deficitcit influences influences the efficiencyefficiency ofof informationinformation processing,processing, labeling,labeling, andand storagestorage withinwithin memorymemory [[16].16]. AA breakdownbreakdown inin processingprocessingat at any any levellevel ofof thethe systemsystem couldcould lead lead to to errors errors in in perceptual perceptual understanding. understanding. Auditory Brainstem Sound Cortex Periphery Pathways Peripheral Neural Transmission & Information Processing, Encoding Feature Extraction Labeling, & Storage Attention Figure 1. Attention is thought to influence the initial encoding and subsequent processing of auditory inputsFigure across 1. Attention all levels is thought of the auditory to influence system. the Adapted initial encoding from Humes, and subsequent 1996 with permission processing fromof audi- the Americantory inputs Academy across all of levels Audiology. of the auditory system. Adapted from Humes, 1996 with permission from the American Academy of Audiology. As speech-in-noise (SIN) deficits are present even in normal hearing listeners [6,7], theseAs difficulties speech-in-noise cannot be (SIN) attributed deficits to are audibility presen withint even in the normal peripheral hearing system listeners alone. [6,7], The underlyingthese difficulties issue cannot likely involvesbe attributed a central-auditory to audibility within or cognitive the peripheral deficit orsystem an interaction alone. The betweenunderlying the issue two. Whenlikely involves the efficiency a central-auditory and synchrony or ofcognitive bottom-up, deficit perceptual or an interaction process- ingbetween of auditory the two. stimuli When isthe disrupted, efficiency listeningand synchrony and learning of bottom-up, difficulties perceptual are exacerbated processing dueof auditory to an impaired stimuli abilityis disrupted, to extract listening necessary and learning acoustic difficulties features for are comprehension. exacerbated due For to instance,an impaired school-aged ability to extract children necessary with listening acoustic and features learning for difficultiescomprehension. demonstrate For instance, less preciseschool-aged and robust children early with sensory listening encoding and learning of speech difficulties signals demonstrate at the level ofless the precise auditory and brainstemrobust early [17 sensory–20] as wellencoding as weaker of speech cortical signal processings at the [level17] despite of the auditory normal peripheral brainstem hear- [17– ing20] sensitivity.as well as weaker Likewise, cortical young processing adults with [17] normal despite hearing normal who peripheral perform hearing poorly sensitiv- on SIN tasksity. Likewise, demonstrate young less robustadults brainstemwith normal [21,22 hearing] and cortical who perform responses poorly in noise on [22 SIN,23], tasks and olderdemonstrate adults who less arerobust poor brainstem SIN performers [21,22] and demonstrate cortical responses weaker
Recommended publications
  • Categorical Perception of Natural and Unnatural Categories: Evidence for Innate Category Boundaries1
    UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics, vol.13, December 2005 Papers in Psycholinguistics 2, Okabe & Nielsen (eds.) CATEGORICAL PERCEPTION OF NATURAL AND UNNATURAL CATEGORIES: EVIDENCE FOR INNATE CATEGORY BOUNDARIES1 JILL GILKERSON [email protected] The results reported here support the claim that naturally occurring phonemic contrasts are easier to acquire than unnatural contrasts. Results are reported from two experiments in which English speakers were exposed to non-native phonemic categories using a bi-modal statistical frequency distribution modeled after Maye and Gerken (2000). Half of the participants heard a distribution in which the category boundary was that of the Jordanian Arabic uvular/pharyngeal contrast, while the other half heard a distribution with an unnatural category boundary. Immediately after exposure, participants completed an A-X delayed comparison task, where they were presented with stimuli that crossed category boundaries. Results indicated that participants in the natural training group responded “different” to across-category pairs significantly more often than participants in the unnatural training group. 1. INTRODUCTION The purpose of this research is to investigate whether adult acquisition of non-native phonemic contrasts is solely dependent on general learning principles or whether it is influenced by principles specific to natural language. Theories of innate grammar claim that humans are endowed with a genetically predetermined system specifically designed to facilitate language acquisition (cf. Chomsky 1965, among others). Nativists maintain that humans are “prewired” with sensitivity to linguistically relevant information in the input. Others have suggested that there are no learning mechanisms specific to language acquisition; it is facilitated by an interaction between statistical distribution of elements in the input and general learning mechanisms not specific to language (cf.
    [Show full text]
  • Guide to Sensory Processing.Pdf
    Guide to Sensory Processing Prepared by Allison Travnik, MSOTS Level II Fieldwork Student Project Kavitha N Krishnan MS OTR/L Fieldwork Instructor Sensory Processing In order to understand what is going on around us, we need to organize all of the incoming sensory information (Ayres, 2005). The sensory information involves what we see, smell, taste, hear, feel on our body, where our body is in relation to others, and how well we are balanced. This is a lot of information that our brains need to process in order to engage in productive behavior, learn, and form accurate perceptions. Proprioceptive Where are body is in space Tactile Auditory What we feel The noise on our skin around us Sensory Smell Processing The Sight difference What we see scents around us around us Oral Sensory Processing Vestibular The sensations Jean Ayres developed the sensory Our sense of Disorder + balance that food give integration (SI) theory. SI gives us in our mouth meaning to what our senses are recognizing. When the sensations are not being organized properly may notice some of the same qualities in the brain, Ayres compared it to about yourself.It is important to a traffic jam. The traffic jam of remember that everyone has some sensory information can lead to quirks about their sensory processing learning difficulties and problem whether it be a sensitivity to loud behavior (Ayres, 2005). Children noises or dislike of light touch. with Sensory Processing Disorder However the identification of SPD is (SPD) are struggling with this reserved for individuals whose traffic jam. sensory quirks are outside of the Sensory processing is a typical range and affect their daily dynamic and complex theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Sensory Processing: Looking at Children's Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing
    Understanding Sensory Processing: Looking at Children’s Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing Communities of Practice in Autism September 24, 2009 Charlottesville, VA Dianne Koontz Lowman, Ed.D. Early Childhood Coordinator Region 5 T/TAC James Madison University MSC 9002 Harrisonburg, VA 22807 [email protected] ______________________________________________________________________________ Dianne Koontz Lowman/[email protected]/2008 Page 1 Looking at Children’s Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing Do you know a child like this? Travis is constantly moving, pushing, or chewing on things. The collar of his shirt and coat are always wet from chewing. When talking to people, he tends to push up against you. Or do you know another child? Sierra does not like to be hugged or kissed by anyone. She gets upset with other children bump up against her. She doesn’t like socks with a heel or toe seam or any tags on clothes. Why is Travis always chewing? Why doesn’t Sierra liked to be touched? Why do children react differently to things around them? These children have different ways of reacting to the things around them, to sensations. Over the years, different terms (such as sensory integration) have been used to describe how children deal with the information they receive through their senses. Currently, the term being used to describe children who have difficulty dealing with input from their senses is sensory processing disorder. _____________________________________________________________________ Sensory Processing Disorder
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Gain Modulation by Closed-Loop Environmental Feedback
    RESEARCH ARTICLE A theory of how active behavior stabilises neural activity: Neural gain modulation by closed-loop environmental feedback Christopher L. Buckley1,2*, Taro Toyoizumi1* 1 Laboratory for Neural Computation and Adaptation, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Saitama, Japan, 2 Department of Informatics and Engineering, University of Sussex, Falmer, United Kingdom * [email protected] (CLB); [email protected] (TT) a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 Abstract a1111111111 During active behaviours like running, swimming, whisking or sniffing, motor actions shape sensory input and sensory percepts guide future motor commands. Ongoing cycles of sen- sory and motor processing constitute a closed-loop feedback system which is central to motor control and, it has been argued, for perceptual processes. This closed-loop feedback OPEN ACCESS is mediated by brainwide neural circuits but how the presence of feedback signals impacts Citation: Buckley CL, Toyoizumi T (2018) A theory on the dynamics and function of neurons is not well understood. Here we present a simple of how active behavior stabilises neural activity: Neural gain modulation by closed-loop theory suggesting that closed-loop feedback between the brain/body/environment can mod- environmental feedback. PLoS Comput Biol 14(1): ulate neural gain and, consequently, change endogenous neural fluctuations and responses e1005926. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. to sensory input. We support this theory with modeling and data analysis in two vertebrate pcbi.1005926 systems. First, in a model of rodent whisking we show that negative feedback mediated by Editor: Daniel Bush, University College London, whisking vibrissa can suppress coherent neural fluctuations and neural responses to sen- UNITED KINGDOM sory input in the barrel cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Sensory Defensiveness? by Ann Stensaas, M.S., OTR/L
    Super Duper® Handy Handouts!® Number 174 What Is Sensory Defensiveness? by Ann Stensaas, M.S., OTR/L Does your child get upset by tags in clothing, the sound of the vacuum cleaner, or certain smells in the environment? If so, your child may be showing signs of sensory defensiveness. Sensory defensiveness is a negative reaction to one or more types of sensations (such as touch, movement, sound, taste/texture, or smell), often requiring you to control his/her daily routine to avoid such things. Types of Sensory Defensiveness There are different types of sensory defensiveness including tactile (touch), gravitational (movement and balance), auditory (hearing), and oral defensiveness (taste, smell, texture). Tactile Defensiveness (Touch) The tactile system is our sense of touch. It protects us from danger and helps us identify different objects in the environment. A child showing signs of tactile defensiveness may: Overreact to ordinary touch experiences (e.g., touching play dough or being touched by someone). Avoid daily activities (e.g., washing face/hands or brushing hair). Avoid light touch (e.g., a kiss) but seek out deep touch (e.g., a bear hug). Vestibular Insecurity (Balance/Movement) The vestibular system is our sense of movement and balance. It tells us where our head and body are in relation to gravity and other objects and supports our vision, posture, emotions, and coordination skills. A child showing signs of gravitational insecurity may: Have an excessive fear of falling during ordinary movement activities (e.g., swinging, riding a bicycle, or climbing). Become overwhelmed by changes in head position (e.g., being upside down).
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Integration: Understanding and Meeting Your Child’S Needs
    Sensory Integration: Understanding and Meeting Your Child’s Needs Sandra Falk,OTR Carrie D. Gross, PTA Le Bonheur Early Intervention and Development (LEAD) What are the Senses? The common senses are: Taste Smell Vision Hearing Touch Two hidden senses are: Vestibular (sense of movement) Proprioception (sensations from muscles and joints). What is Sensory Integration? It is our ability to take in information from the world around us through our senses, sort it out in the brain, and respond to it successfully. It takes place automatically, without us having to “think” about it. How Does Sensory Integration Develop? Sensory integration is pre-programmed to develop from conception However, it takes sensory experiences to activate and/or enhance this process We are designed to enjoy things that promote the development of our brain, and therefore seek out sensations that help organize our brain All of us have sensory preferences and “issues” that we accommodate for on a daily basis. It is when sensory difficulties interfere with everyday life that it becomes a problem. When there is a problem: If the brain is unable to understand and process sensory information efficiently, then that child may have a difficult time functioning in every day life. The child may be awkward and clumsy, fearful and withdrawn, or hostile and aggressive. It can affect how a child moves and learns, how he behaves, how he plays and makes friends, and how he feels about himself. A lot children have some sensory issues, and would benefit from their caregivers understanding their sensory needs. If the problems are severe and interfere with everyday life, a therapist can evaluate and help establish a plan of care.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Perception in Autism
    REVIEWS Sensory perception in autism Caroline E. Robertson1,2,3* and Simon Baron-Cohen4 Abstract | Autism is a complex neurodevelopmental condition, and little is known about its neurobiology. Much of autism research has focused on the social, communication and cognitive difficulties associated with the condition. However, the recent revision of the diagnostic criteria for autism has brought another key domain of autistic experience into focus: sensory processing. Here, we review the properties of sensory processing in autism and discuss recent computational and neurobiological insights arising from attention to these behaviours. We argue that sensory traits have important implications for the development of animal and computational models of the condition. Finally, we consider how difficulties in sensory processing may relate to the other domains of behaviour that characterize autism. Cognitive empathy The ability to reflect on our own and others’ thoughts However, the issue of primacy is key. Is autism, as The ability to understand and and emotions (that is, theory of mind) is a defining often posited, a disorder of the ‘social brain’ (REF. 15), with respond appropriately to characteristic of human cognition. Children with sensory differences representing either secondary con‑ others’ mental states and autism spectrum conditions (ASCs; henceforth ‘autism’) sequences after a lifetime of reduced social interaction emotions (unlike affective 1 empathy, the ability to respond show delays in the development of this capacity , with or alterations in domain‑general mechanisms (such as 2 with an appropriate emotion to knock‑on consequences for cognitive empathy across attention) that affect both social processing and sensory others’ mental states or the lifespan.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Processing Disorder
    Sensory Integration Sensory processing (sometimes called "sensory integration" or SI) is a term that refers to the way the nervous system receives messages from the senses and turns them into appropriate motor and behavioral responses. Whether you are biting into a hamburger, riding a bicycle, or reading a book, your successful completion of the activity requires processing sensation or "sensory integration." Sensory Processing Disorder (SPD, formerly known as "sensory integration dysfunction") is a condition that exists when sensory signals don't get organized into appropriate responses. Pioneering occupational therapist and neuroscientist A. Jean Ayres, PhD, likened SPD to a neurological "traffic jam" that prevents certain parts of the brain from receiving the information needed to interpret sensory information correctly. A person with SPD finds it difficult to process and act upon information received through the senses, which creates challenges in performing countless everyday tasks. Motor clumsiness, behavioral problems, anxiety, depression, school failure, and other impacts may result if the disorder is not treated effectively. One study (Ahn, Miller, Milberger, McIntosh, 2004) shows that at least 1 in 20 children’s daily lives is affected by SPD. Another research study by the Sensory Processing Disorder Scientific Work Group (Ben-Sasson, Carter, Briggs-Gowen, 2009) suggests that 1 in every 6 children experiences sensory symptoms that may be significant enough to affect aspects of everyday life functions. Symptoms of Sensory Processing Disorder, like those of most disorders, occur within a broad spectrum of severity. While most of us have occasional difficulties processing sensory information, for children and adults with SPD, these difficulties are chronic, and they disrupt everyday life.
    [Show full text]
  • Categorical Perception of Consonants and Vowels: Evidence from a Neurophonetic Model of Speech Production and Perception
    Categorical Perception of Consonants and Vowels: Evidence from a Neurophonetic Model of Speech Production and Perception Bernd J. Kröger, Peter Birkholz, Jim Kannampuzha, and Christiane Neuschaefer-Rube Department of Phoniatrics, Pedaudiology, and Communication Disorders, University Hospital Aachen and RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany {bkroeger,pbirkholz,jkannampuzha,cneuschaefer}@ukaachen.de Abstract. While the behavioral side of categorical perception in speech is al- ready well investigated, little is known concerning its underlying neural mecha- nisms. In this study, a computer-implemented neurophonetic model of speech production and perception is used in order to elucidate the functional neural mechanisms responsible for categorical perception. 20 instances of the model (“virtual listeners/speakers”) underwent a speech acquisition training procedure and then performed behavioral tests, i.e. identification and discrimination expe- riments based on vocalic and CV-syllabic speech stimuli. These virtual listeners showed the expected behavioral results. The inspection of the neural organi- zation of virtual listeners indicated clustering in the case of categorical percep- tion and no clustering in the case of non-categorical (continuous) perception for neurons representing the stimuli. These results highlight a possible neural or- ganization underlying categorical and continuous perception. Keywords: speech perception, categorical perception, identification, discrimi- nation, neural model of speech production. 1 Introduction Categorical perception is an important feature of speech, needed for successfully differentiating and identifying sounds, syllables, or words. Categorical speech per- ception enables humans to map different realizations of one speech sound into one category. This is important in order to achieve a robust discrimination of different speech items, even if these items are realized by different speakers or by different articulations of the same speaker.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Sign Language Experience on Categorical Perception of Dynamic ASL Pseudosigns
    Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (3), 747-762 doi:10.3758/APP.72.3.747 Effects of sign language experience on categorical perception of dynamic ASL pseudosigns CATHERINE T. BEST University of Western Sydney, Penrith, New South Wales, Australia and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut GAURAV MATHUR Gallaudet University, Washington, D.C. and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut KAREN A. M IRANDA Massachusetts School of Professional Psychology, Boston, Massachusetts AND DIANE LI ll O -MARTIN University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut We investigated effects of sign language experience on deaf and hearing participants’ categorical perception of minimal manual contrast stimuli that met key criteria of speech perception research. A continuum of meaningless dynamic stimuli was created with a morphing approach, which manipulated videorecorded productions of phono- tactically permissible pseudosigns differing between American Sign Language (ASL) handshapes that contrast on a single articulatory dimension (U–V: finger-spreading). AXB discrimination and AXB categorization and goodness ratings on the target items were completed by deaf early (native) signers (DE), deaf late (nonnative) signers (DL), hearing late (L2) signers (HL), and hearing nonsigners (HN). Categorization and goodness functions were less categorical and had different boundaries for DL participants than for DE and HL participants. Shape and level of discrimination functions also differed by ASL experience and hearing status, with DL signers showing better perfor- mance than DE, HL, and especially HN participants, particularly at the U end of the continuum. Although no group displayed a peak in discrimination at the category boundary, thus failing to support classic categorical perception, discrimination was consistent with categorization in other ways that differed among the groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistic Effect on Speech Perception Observed at the Brainstem
    Linguistic effect on speech perception observed at the brainstem T. Christina Zhaoa and Patricia K. Kuhla,1 aInstitute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195 Contributed by Patricia K. Kuhl, May 10, 2018 (sent for review January 5, 2018; reviewed by Bharath Chandrasekaran and Robert J. Zatorre) Linguistic experience affects speech perception from early infancy, as 11 mo of age were observed to be significantly reduced compared previously evidenced by behavioral and brain measures. Current with MMRs observed at 7 mo age (15). research focuses on whether linguistic effects on speech perception The current study set out to examine whether the effects of lin- canbeobservedatanearlierstageinthe neural processing of speech guistic experience on consonant perception already manifest at a (i.e., auditory brainstem). Brainstem responses reflect rapid, auto- deeper and earlier stage than the cortex, namely, at the level of matic, and preattentive encoding of sounds. Positive experiential auditory brainstem. The auditory brainstem encodes acoustic in- effects have been reported by examining the frequency-following formation and relays them to the auditory cortex. Its activity can be response (FFR) component of the complex auditory brainstem recorded noninvasively at the scalp using a simple setup of three response (cABR) in response to sustained high-energy periodic electroencephalographic (EEG) electrodes (16). Using extremely portions of speech sounds (vowels and lexical tones). The current brief stimuli such as clicks or tone pips, the auditory brainstem re- study expands the existing literature by examining the cABR onset sponse (ABR) can be elicited and has been used clinically to assess component in response to transient and low-energy portions of the integrity of the auditory pathway (17).
    [Show full text]
  • Enhanced Sensitivity to Subphonemic Segments in Dyslexia: a New Instance of Allophonic Perception
    brain sciences Article Enhanced Sensitivity to Subphonemic Segments in Dyslexia: A New Instance of Allophonic Perception Willy Serniclaes 1,* and M’ballo Seck 2 1 Speech Perception Lab., CNRS & Paris Descartes University, 75006 Paris, France 2 Human & Artificial Cognition Lab., Paris 8 University, 93526 Saint-Denis, France; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] or [email protected]; Tel.: +33-680-230-886 Received: 28 February 2018; Accepted: 22 March 2018; Published: 26 March 2018 Abstract: Although dyslexia can be individuated in many different ways, it has only three discernable sources: a visual deficit that affects the perception of letters, a phonological deficit that affects the perception of speech sounds, and an audio-visual deficit that disturbs the association of letters with speech sounds. However, the very nature of each of these core deficits remains debatable. The phonological deficit in dyslexia, which is generally attributed to a deficit of phonological awareness, might result from a specific mode of speech perception characterized by the use of allophonic (i.e., subphonemic) units. Here we will summarize the available evidence and present new data in support of the “allophonic theory” of dyslexia. Previous studies have shown that the dyslexia deficit in the categorical perception of phonemic features (e.g., the voicing contrast between /t/ and /d/) is due to the enhanced sensitivity to allophonic features (e.g., the difference between two variants of /d/). Another consequence of allophonic perception is that it should also give rise to an enhanced sensitivity to allophonic segments, such as those that take place within a consonant cluster.
    [Show full text]