The Psychology of Tyranny: the BBC Prison Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Psychology of Tyranny: the BBC Prison Study TYRANNYTHE PSYCHOLOGY OF POWER CORRUPTS, AND ABSOLUTE POWER CORRUPTS ABSOLUTELY—OR DOES IT? BY S. ALEXANDER HASLAM AND STEPHEN D. REICHER Images of inhumanity and atrocity are burned into our memories. Jewish men, women and children being herded into gas chambers. Entire villages destroyed by rampaging gangs in Rwanda. The systematic use of rape and the destruction of communities as part of “ethnic cleansing” in the Balkans. The massacre at My Lai in South Vietnam, the abuse of Iraqi prisoners in Abu Ghraib, and most re- cently, the carnage wrought by suicide bombers in Baghdad, Jerusalem, London and Madrid. Refl ecting on these events, we inevitably ask: What makes people so brutal? Are they mentally ill? Are they the products of dysfunctional families or cultures? Or, more disturbingly, is anyone capable of taking part in collective ruthlessness given the right—or rather, the wrong—circumstances? Now the latest research, including possibly the largest social-psychology experiment in three CREDIT CREDIT decades, is providing a new window on these conundrums. 44 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MIND COPYRIGHT 2005 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC. COPYRIGHT 2005 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC. Joseph Stalin, Questions about why groups can behave bad- early 1960s by Stanley Milgram. Ordinary, well- Adolf Hitler, ly have driven some of the most signifi cant devel- adjusted males who took part in a bogus memo- Idi Amin and opments in social psychology in the 60 years ry experiment were told to deliver electric shocks Saddam Hussein since World War II ended. Starting with the need of increasing magnitude to another person who had powerful to understand the psychological processes that posed as the learner. (In actuality, the learner, an personalities, but made possible the horrors of the Holocaust, sci- accomplice of the experimenter, received no their success as ); entists have wanted to know how large numbers shocks.) Amazingly, every single “teacher” was tyrants also ) required social of apparently civilized and decent people can prepared to administer “intense shocks” of 300 Stalin conditions that perpetrate appalling acts. volts, and two thirds obeyed all the experiment- Hussein made their Initially theorists sought answers to group er’s requests, dispensing what they believed were ( messages pathology in individual psychology. In 1961, 450 volts. Participants continued meting out acceptable. however, German-born American historian and punishments even after hearing the learner com- Corbis political philosopher Hannah Arendt witnessed plain of a heart condition and yell in apparent the trial in Jerusalem of Adolf Eichmann, one of agony. Milgram concluded: “Arendt’s concep- the chief architects of the Holocaust. She con- tion of the banality of evil comes closer to the cluded that far from the defendant demonstrat- truth than one might dare to imagine.” ing a “perverted and sadistic personality” (as The vivid culmination of this line of inquiry psychiatrists for the prosecution claimed), he was was the Stanford prison experiment, carried out PAVLOVSKY JACQUES ); utterly unremarkable and disarmingly ordinary. in 1971 by Stanford University psychologist Phil- Amin Arendt pronounced Eichmann to be an embodi- ip G. Zimbardo and his colleagues. The research- ( ment of “the banality of evil.” ers randomly assigned college students to be ei- ther prisoners or guards in a simulated prison in ( CORBIS COLLECTION HULTON-DEUTSCH PAGE: THIS Everyday Evil? the basement of the campus psychology building. Sygma Corbis Corbis Sygma First published in 1963 in the New Yorker, The goal was to explore the dynamics that devel- Arendt’s analysis was considered shocking and oped within and between the groups over a two- heretical. But a series of studies conducted week period. The study delivered these dynamics Getty Images; around the same time supported her observa- in abundance. Indeed, the guards (with Zimbar- tions. In experiments at U.S. summer camps dur- do as their superintendent) exerted force with ); ALAIN NOGUES ing the late 1950s, Muzafer Sherif, a Turkish- such harshness that the study was halted after born American social psychologist, learned that only six days. Hitler normal schoolboys became cruel and aggressive The experimenters concluded that group toward former friends once they had been placed members cannot resist the pressure of their as- in different groups that had to compete over sumed stations and that brutality is the “natural” scarce resources. Even more striking were obedi- expression of roles associated with groups who PRECEDING CARTER PAGES: MARC ence studies carried out at Yale University in the have unequal power. Accordingly, two maxims, BETTMANN ( CORBIS Scientists have wanted to know how apparently (civilized people can perpetrate appalling acts.) 46 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MIND COPYRIGHT 2005 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC. The “banality of evil” is clear in the hotel room in Rwanda (left) that served as the ordinary- looking setting for launching a plan for genocide (below). which have had immense infl uence at both a sci- entifi c and a cultural level—and which are taught as received knowledge to millions of students around the world every year—are routinely drawn from the Stanford experiment. The fi rst is that individuals lose their capacity for intellectual and moral judgment in groups; hence, groups are in- herently dangerous. The second is that there is an ) inevitable impetus for people to act tyrannically once they are put in groups and given power. bottom ( Reexamining Group Power structively. In variants of Milgram’s obedience The weight of the Stanford prison experiment trials, participants were much more likely to defy lies in both its dramatic fi ndings and the simple, the experimenter when they were supported by stark conclusions that have been drawn from it. confederates who were also disobedient. Magnum Photos Photos Magnum Over the years, however, social psychologists In addition, research after Stanford has con- have developed doubts about the resulting re- fi rmed the prosocial and enriching aspects of ceived wisdom. groups. One particularly infl uential approach to First, the idea that groups with power auto- understanding groups in psychology today is the matically become tyrannical ignores the active social identity theory developed in 1979 by social PATRICK ZACHMANN leadership that the experimenters provided. Zim- psychologists John Turner, now at the Australian ; ) bardo told his guards: “You can create in the National University, and Henri Tajfel, then at top ( prisoners ... a sense of fear to some degree, you Bristol University in England. This theory holds can create a notion of arbitrariness that their life that it is mostly in groups that people—particu- is totally controlled by us.... They’ll have no larly the individually powerless—can become ef- freedom of action, they can do nothing, say noth- fective agents who shape their own fate. Magnum Photos Photos Magnum ing that we don’t permit.... We’re going to take When individuals share a sense of identity (for away their individuality in various ways.” example, “we are all American,” “we are all Cath- Second, we know that groups do not perpe- olic”), they seek to reach agreement, they like and trate only antisocial acts. In studies—as in soci- trust one another more, they are more willing to ety at large—the group often emerges as a means follow group leaders, and they form more effective STUART FRANKLIN FRANKLIN STUART to resist oppression and the pressure to act de- organizations. This fact is shown, for example, in www.sciammind.com 47 COPYRIGHT 2005 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC. extensive studies of cooperation in groups con- als. Only when they believe that they cannot es- ducted recently by Steven L. Blader and Tom R. cape—that is, when boundaries between groups Tyler of New York University. As a result, people are seen to be impermeable, as feminists argued can pull together to create a social world based on when they identifi ed the “glass ceiling”—will their shared values—instilling a state of “collective they identify with the devalued group and act self-realization,” which is good for psychological collectively. In addition, they will be prepared to health. Having the social support to control one’s use their collective power to challenge the status fate results in higher self-esteem, less stress, and quo and try to improve the position of their group lower levels of anxiety and depression. only if they view the social system as unstable. People who share a sense of identity in a A large body of research, including controlled group demonstrate two social features. First, laboratory studies, extensive surveys and de- We aimed to represent institutions in which one group ( has more power and privilege than the other. ) they do not lose the capacity for judgment; in- tailed field observations, supports the social stead the basis for their decisions shifts from identity perspective (for a review, see Social Iden- their individual notions to their commonly held tity, edited by Naomi Ellemers of Leiden Univer- understandings. As fi eld studies by one of us sity in the Netherlands and her colleagues). Yet (Reicher) have shown (a summary appears in the until recently there was no single study in the Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: mold of Sherif, Milgram or Zimbardo that might Group Processes), even the most extreme collec- illustrate and combine the theory’s various prop- tive actions, such as a riot, reveal a pattern of ositions in a comprehensive and compelling man- behavior that refl ects the beliefs, norms and val- ner. What is more, it seemed impossible to con- ues of the group involved. Second, people’s re- duct such a study. Despite all the doubts sur- sponses vary depending on which group mem- rounding Stanford, its very severity seemed to bership is most important to them in any given put further studies of its type off-limits. situation. For instance, the norms and values that This situation recently changed with the BBC we use as employees in the workplace may differ prison experiment.
Recommended publications
  • Obedience to Authority”: from Blind Conformity to Engaged Followership
    LS13CH05-Haslam ARI 19 September 2017 7:37 Annual Review of Law and Social Science 50 Years of “Obedience to Authority”: From Blind Conformity to Engaged Followership S. Alexander Haslam1 and Stephen D. Reicher2 1School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia; email: [email protected] 2School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, Fife, KY16 9JP United Kingdom Annu. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 2017. 13:59–78 Keywords The Annual Review of Law and Social Science is Milgram, obedience, conformity, authority, identification, followership online at lawsocsci.annualreviews.org https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-lawsocsci- Abstract 110316-113710 Despite being conducted half a century ago, Stanley Milgram’s studies of Copyright c 2017 by Annual Reviews. ⃝ obedience to authority remain the most well-known, most controversial, and Access provided by 202.90.207.98 on 10/17/17. For personal use only. All rights reserved most important in social psychology. Yet in recent years, increased scrutiny has served to question the integrity of Milgram’s research reports, the va- Annu. Rev. Law. Soc. Sci. 2017.13:59-78. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org lidity of his explanation of the phenomena he reported, and the broader relevance of his research to processes of collective harm-doing. We review these debates and argue that the main problem with received understand- ings of Milgram’s work arises from seeing it as an exploration of obedience. Instead, we argue that it is better understood as providing insight into pro- cesses of engaged followership, in which people are prepared to harm others because they identify with their leaders’ cause and believe their actions to be virtuous.
    [Show full text]
  • Can We Still Use Milgram's 'Obedience to Authority' Experiments to Explain Mass Atrocities After the Op
    JPR Milgram Revisited: Can we still use Milgram’s ‘Obedience to Authority’ Experiments to Explain Mass Atrocities after the Opening of the Archives? Review Essay Alette Smeulers Introduction ilgram’s ‘obedience to authority experiments’ are, together with Zimbardo’s prison experiment, one of the most famous but also most controversial studies ever conducted.1 Since its first publication in 1963, Milgram’s Mresearch has drawn the attention not only of scholars but also of the media, and the experiment as well as the results have been widely debated and referenced, but also heavily criticized.2 The 50th anniversary of his experiments and the opening of the Yale archives led to a new wave of publications and criticism. A lot of material on the Milgram experiments which until then had been hidden from scholarly and public scrutiny cast serious doubts on Milgram’s actual findings and their relevance.3 Between 2011 and 2015, no fewer than four internation- al peer-reviewed journals published a special issue on Milgram’s experiments: The Psychologist in 2011, edited by Reicher and Haslam; Theoretical & Applied Ethics in 2013, edited by Herara; the Journal of Social Issues in 2014, edited by Reicher, Haslam and Miller; and Theory & Psychology in 2015, edited by Brannigan, Nicholson and Cherry. In addition, Gina Perry published a book on the Milgram experiments in 2012 entitled Behind the Shock Machine: The Untold Story of the Notorious I wish to thank Maria Ioannou, Chris Atkinson, George Smeulers, Nicola Quaedvlieg, and the editors of the journal for their useful suggestions, comments and corrections. 1 Stanley Milgram, Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View (New York: Harper and Row, 1974); Philip G.
    [Show full text]
  • Making 'Us' Better: High-Quality Athlete Leadership Relates to Health and Burnout in Professional Au
    European Journal of Sport Science ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tejs20 Making ‘us’ better: High-quality athlete leadership relates to health and burnout in professional Australian football teams Katrien Fransen , S. Alexander Haslam , Niklas K. Steffens , Clifford J. Mallett , Kim Peters & Filip Boen To cite this article: Katrien Fransen , S. Alexander Haslam , Niklas K. Steffens , Clifford J. Mallett , Kim Peters & Filip Boen (2020) Making ‘us’ better: High-quality athlete leadership relates to health and burnout in professional Australian football teams, European Journal of Sport Science, 20:7, 953-963, DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1680736 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2019.1680736 Published online: 27 Oct 2019. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 375 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 3 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tejs20 European Journal of Sport Science, 2020 Vol. 20, No. 7, 953–963, https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2019.1680736 Making ‘us’ better: High-quality athlete leadership relates to health and burnout in professional Australian football teams KATRIEN FRANSEN 1, S. ALEXANDER HASLAM 2, NIKLAS K. STEFFENS 2, CLIFFORD J. MALLETT3, KIM PETERS 2, & FILIP BOEN 1 1Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; 2School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia & 3School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia Abstract Overtraining, exhaustion, and burnout are widely recognized problems amongst elite athletes. The present research addresses this issue by exploring the extent to which high-quality athlete leadership is associated with elite athletes’ health and burnout.
    [Show full text]
  • Consensus Statement on the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study Professors Craig Haney, Alex Haslam, Stephen Reicher, and Philip Zimbardo
    Consensus Statement on the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study Professors Craig Haney, Alex Haslam, Stephen Reicher, and Philip Zimbardo We, the undersigned researchers who conducted the Stanford Prison Experiment (Philip Zimbardo and Craig Haney) and BBC Prison Study (Alex Haslam and Stephen Reicher), recognize that our studies, results, and public statements have engendered strong debate and, at times, misunderstanding within and beyond psychology. In an effort to promote constructive scientific dialogue, we are therefore releasing this consensus statement to highlight common ground and clarify our views on the research in question. First, we Jointly believe that it is critically important to develop a scientific understanding of toxic human behavior, including brutality and the abuse of authority and power. Second, we regard the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study as valid studies and valuable resources for advancing such understanding. At the same time, we recognize that both investigations have methodological limitations and are best viewed as one-trial demonstration studies rather than traditional experiments. Third, the BBC Prison Study differs from the Stanford Prison Experiment in essential ways and is not a direct replication of the earlier study. We do, however, see value in comparing the different outcomes of the two studies (and others) as a means of advancing conceptual understanding. Fourth, the behaviors observed in the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study were a function of many factors, including roles, norms, leadership, social identification, group pressure, and individual differences, not all of which are necessarily mutually exclusive. It is only natural that explanations of social behavior will be complex and multifactorial.
    [Show full text]
  • Debate Around Leadership in the Stanford Prison Experiment: Reply to Zimbardo and Haney (2020) and Chan Et Al
    American Psychologist © 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 75, No. 3, 406–407 ISSN: 0003-066X http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000627 REPLY Debate Around Leadership in the Stanford Prison Experiment: Reply to Zimbardo and Haney (2020) and Chan et al. (2020) Stephen D. Reicher Jay J. Van Bavel University of St. Andrews New York University S. Alexander Haslam University of Queensland Access to the Stanford University archive has revealed new material that makes it possible to debate the precise nature and causes of events in the Stanford Prison Experiment. What the authors see as important is that these materials show the experimenters engaged in processes of identity leadership, which encouraged guard cruelty by presenting it as necessary for the achievement of noble collective goals. However, the authors encourage students, teachers, and researchers to engage with this new material themselves to explore alternative perspec- tives on what actually occurred in the study. Keywords: leadership, followership, social identification, Stanford Prison Experiment, open science Two decades ago, when we were planning our BBC of the SPE. This concerns what Moscovici (1976) identified Prison Study (Reicher & Haslam, 2006), we argued for the as a general conformity bias in social psychology. The focus resumption of normal scientific debate around the Stanford in the SPE, as elsewhere, was on the ways in which people Prison Experiment (SPE; Haney, Banks, & Zimbardo, conform to expectations (in this case, role expectations), to 1973). In particular, we sought to invigorate discussion the exclusion of acts of nonconformity. Yet even from the around the critical question of what can lead ordinary hu- limited material then available, there was considerable ev- mans to act in extraordinarily inhumane ways.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Identity Pathways Associated with Divergent Traumatic Responses
    European Review of Social Psychology ISSN: 1046-3283 (Print) 1479-277X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pers20 The social psychology of responses to trauma: social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses Orla T. Muldoon, S. Alexander Haslam, Catherine Haslam, Tegan Cruwys, Michelle Kearns & Jolanda Jetten To cite this article: Orla T. Muldoon, S. Alexander Haslam, Catherine Haslam, Tegan Cruwys, Michelle Kearns & Jolanda Jetten (2019) The social psychology of responses to trauma: social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses, European Review of Social Psychology, 30:1, 311-348 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2020.1711628 © 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group. Published online: 10 Jan 2020. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 395 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pers20 EUROPEAN REVIEW OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020, VOL. 30, NO. 1, 311–348 https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2020.1711628 ARTICLE The social psychology of responses to trauma: social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses Orla T. Muldoon a, S. Alexander Haslam b, Catherine Haslam b, Tegan Cruwys c, Michelle Kearns a,d and Jolanda Jetten b aCentre for Social Issues Research, Department of Psychology, University of Limerick, Limerick V94 T9PX, Ireland; bSchool of Psychology, McElwain Building University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia; cResearch School of Psychology, The Australian National University, 39 Science Road, ACT 0200, Australia; dInternational Rescue Committee, Freetown, Sierra Leone ABSTRACT Research in clinical psychology and social psychiatry has highlighted the impor- tance of social factors for outcomes following trauma.
    [Show full text]
  • *17 Haslam CV Small
    Curriculum Vitae S. Alexander HASLAM Professor of Psychology, Australian Laureate Fellow University of Queensland Date of Birth December, 1962 Academic Qualifications 1985 M.A. Hons (first class), Psychology University of St Andrews 1991 PhD, Psychology Macquarie University PhD title: Social Comparative Context, Self-Categorization and Stereotyping Supervisor: J. C. Turner Main Research areas Psychology in organizations — with an emphasis on the contribution of social identity to leadership, motivation, communication, decision-making, negotiation, and productivity. The social psychology of stereotyping, prejudice, and tyranny — exploring the role of group processes in the dynamics of intergroup relations and conflict. Social processes in health and well-being — looking at the contribution of group life to stress, coping and well-being, especially in vulnerable populations. Research methodology — focusing on issues of research design, ethics, and uncertainty management. Previous Appointments 2/1990 – 2/1991 Lecturer, Department of Behavioural Science, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney 3/1991 – 2/2001 Research Officer, Lecturer, Senior Lecturer, Reader, School of Psychology, The Australian National University 3/2001 – 2012 Professor, School of Psychology, University of Exeter 8/2012 – present Professor and ARC Laureate Fellow, School of Psychology, University of Queensland Academic Awards and Distinctions 1982 Ferguson Prize and Class Medal for Psychology, University of St Andrews 1982 Lawson Prize and Class Medal for
    [Show full text]
  • RUNNING HEAD: Identification in Group Therapy Predictors of Social Identification in Group Therapy TEGAN CRUWYS 1 * NIKLAS K. ST
    RUNNING HEAD: Identification in group therapy Predictors of social identification in group therapy TEGAN CRUWYS 1 * NIKLAS K. STEFFENS 2 S. ALEXANDER HASLAM 2 CATHERINE HASLAM 2 MATTHEW J. HORNSEY 3 CRAIG MCGARTY 4 DANIEL P. SKORICH 2 Citation: Cruwys, T., Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., Haslam, C., Hornsey, M. J., McGarty, C., & Skorich, D. P. (2020). Predictors of social identification in group therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 30(3), 348-361. 1. Research School of Psychology, Australian National University, CANBERRA ACT 2601 2. School of Psychology, University of Queensland, ST LUCIA QLD 4072 3. UQ Business School, ST LUCIA QLD 4072 4. School of Social Sciences and Psychology, Western Sydney University, PENRITH NSW 2751 *Corresponding author: 1. Dr Tegan Cruwys, [email protected] Research School of Psychology, Australian National University, CANBERRA ACT 2601 AUSTRALIA Acknowledgements: This project was supported by funding from the Australian Research Council (DE160100592 and FL110100199). The funder had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, writing, or decision to publish. We are grateful for the support of Genevieve A. Dingle, Nicole Fox, Hayley McMahon and Tian P. S. Oei. IDENTIFICATION IN GROUP THERAPY | 2 Abstract Background: Group therapy is a popular and effective format for psychological intervention, and both anecdotal and empirical data consistently point to group dynamics as a primary driver of its benefits. However, to date there has been no systematic investigation of what facilitates an engaged, cohesive group environment. We argue that this is social identification and explore the features of groups that help to build this. Method: We present two longitudinal studies of group therapy and examine the predictors of social identification.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Identification-Building Interventions to Improve Health: a Systematic Review and Meta- Analysis
    Health Psychology Review ISSN: 1743-7199 (Print) 1743-7202 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rhpr20 Social identification-building interventions to improve health: a systematic review and meta- analysis Niklas K. Steffens, Crystal J. LaRue, Catherine Haslam, Zoe C. Walter, Tegan Cruwys, Katie A. Munt, S. Alexander Haslam, Jolanda Jetten & Mark Tarrant To cite this article: Niklas K. Steffens, Crystal J. LaRue, Catherine Haslam, Zoe C. Walter, Tegan Cruwys, Katie A. Munt, S. Alexander Haslam, Jolanda Jetten & Mark Tarrant (2019): Social identification-building interventions to improve health: a systematic review and meta-analysis, Health Psychology Review, DOI: 10.1080/17437199.2019.1669481 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2019.1669481 View supplementary material Accepted author version posted online: 17 Sep 2019. Published online: 07 Oct 2019. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 178 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rhpr20 HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2019.1669481 Social identification-building interventions to improve health: a systematic review and meta-analysis Niklas K. Steffens a, Crystal J. LaRue a, Catherine Haslam a, Zoe C. Walter a, Tegan Cruwys b, Katie A. Munta, S. Alexander Haslam a, Jolanda Jetten a and Mark Tarrantc aSchool of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia; bResearch School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australia; cUniversity of Exeter Medical School, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY There is growing evidence that social identity processes play an important Received 9 May 2019 role in a range of health outcomes.
    [Show full text]
  • Just Obeying Orders?
    OPINION THE BIG IDEA Just obeying orders? Ordinary people can commit atrocities simply by following orders, iconic experiments from the 1960s concluded. But this notion of the “banality of evil” is wrong, argue psychologists Alexander Haslam and Stephen Reicher S oto AGNUM PH AGNUM M RICK ZACHMANN/RICK T PA 28 | NewScientist | 13 September 2014 For more opinion articles, visit newscientist.com/opinion IF YOU only know about one research NAZIS AND THE banaLITY OF EVIL programme in psychology, chances are it is Stanley Milgram’s “shock experiments”. In 1961, Adolf Eichmann, one of main lesson to be gleaned passive pen-pusher. Rather, he Conducted in the early 1960s at Yale the chief architects of the “final from his life was one of “the was a committed Nazi who took University, the participants were asked by an solution to the Jewish question”, fearsome, word-and-thought- on organisational challenges “Experimenter” to take on the role of “Teacher” was sentenced to death for the defying banality of evil”. with fervour and imagination. and administer an escalating series of electric murder of millions of people in Yet while this concept of the If he thought orders were not shocks to a “Learner” in the next room when Nazi extermination camps. banality of evil proved highly sufficiently “on message” he he chose the wrong answers in a memory test. Prior to his trial he had been influential – not least because would disobey them, and where This was supposedly part of a study into the portrayed in the media and by it gelled closely with Stanley none had been given, as was effect of punishment on learning.
    [Show full text]
  • The Stanford Prison Experiment 1992, a Documentary Written by Zimbardo
    Stanford Prison Experiment What happens when experiments are misplanned Giovanni Viviani Stanford Prison Experiment Why do we need ethics standards in research Giovanni Viviani Stanford Prison Experiment Study of psychological efect of becoming a prisoner or a prison guard Conducted at Stanford University, August 1971, by professor Philip Zimbardo One of the most criticised experiment of history Often related to Abu Ghraib prison scandal Stanford Prison Experiment 24 males participants, chosen to exclude those with a criminal background Participants were divided in 2 groups, prisoners and prison guards, arbitrarily assigned by Zimbardo The experiment was planned to last for 2 weeks More than 50 researchers observed the experiment https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=sYtX2sEaeFE Real policemen were involved to arrest the prisoners Prisoners were brought to the police station to take fngerprints and mug shots Prisoners were then transferred to a mockup prison in the basement of Jordan Hall Prisoners were strip searched, chained, assigned a number and closed in their cells Guards were given wooden batons, a uniform and mirrored sunglasses https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=x3wxEmHqVCY "… the guards were told to routinely performs counts…" "…it provided a regular occasion for the guards to interact with and exercise control over the prisoners…" The second morning a rebellion started when the prisoner of one cell barricaded themselves inside After putting the rebellion down guard started using psychological and physical punishment to keep prisoners
    [Show full text]
  • Tyranny Revisited Groups, Psychological Well-Being and the Health of Societies
    Tyranny revisited Groups, psychological well-being and the health of societies VIL acts, we like to think, are the preserve of psychopaths. Yet 30 to E 40 years ago, a series of classic STEPHEN REICHER and psychology experiments showed that the behaviour of ordinary people can be S.ALEXANDER HASLAM discuss transformed in groups and that the most decent of individuals can be led to behave results from their BBC Prison Study. in the most indecent ways. These studies raise critical questions about the processes In this, ordinary young men were divided agency and hence to become helpless to through which groups can transform us, randomly into prisoners and guards and resist antisocial impulses. Groups are and whether such transformations are placed in a prison-like setting. Very inevitably bad for you. Groups with power always for the worse. Yet for decades it has inevitably abuse it. Or, in the researchers’ been impossible to conduct studies with the own words, the aggression of the guards same power as the classic studies and to ‘Can collaborations between ‘was emitted simply as a “natural” interrogate their conclusions. The BBC the media and academia ever consequence of being in the uniform of Prison Study has broken this impasse and be of scientific value?’ a “guard” and asserting the power inherent provides a surprising new set of answers in that role’ (Haney et al., 1973, p.12). with important social, clinical and organisational ramifications. quickly, some of the guards began to act A powerful phenomenon… brutally. They set out to humiliate the but a questionable explanation Are groups ‘naturally’ bad prisoners and to deprive them of their Although few doubt what happened at for us? rights.
    [Show full text]