Just Obeying Orders?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Just Obeying Orders? OPINION THE BIG IDEA Just obeying orders? Ordinary people can commit atrocities simply by following orders, iconic experiments from the 1960s concluded. But this notion of the “banality of evil” is wrong, argue psychologists Alexander Haslam and Stephen Reicher S oto AGNUM PH AGNUM M RICK ZACHMANN/RICK T PA 28 | NewScientist | 13 September 2014 For more opinion articles, visit newscientist.com/opinion IF YOU only know about one research NAZIS AND THE banaLITY OF EVIL programme in psychology, chances are it is Stanley Milgram’s “shock experiments”. In 1961, Adolf Eichmann, one of main lesson to be gleaned passive pen-pusher. Rather, he Conducted in the early 1960s at Yale the chief architects of the “final from his life was one of “the was a committed Nazi who took University, the participants were asked by an solution to the Jewish question”, fearsome, word-and-thought- on organisational challenges “Experimenter” to take on the role of “Teacher” was sentenced to death for the defying banality of evil”. with fervour and imagination. and administer an escalating series of electric murder of millions of people in Yet while this concept of the If he thought orders were not shocks to a “Learner” in the next room when Nazi extermination camps. banality of evil proved highly sufficiently “on message” he he chose the wrong answers in a memory test. Prior to his trial he had been influential – not least because would disobey them, and where This was supposedly part of a study into the portrayed in the media and by it gelled closely with Stanley none had been given, as was effect of punishment on learning. psychiatrists as a sadistic and Milgram’s account of obedience often the case, he would still The participants didn’t know that the shocks, psychopathic monster. But as to authority – in recent years “work towards the Führer” in and the cries they elicited from the Learner, political theorist Hannah Arendt historians have cast doubt on a creative way. weren’t genuine. Nevertheless, many acceded watched him give his defence its validity. A crucial point is that he did to the Experimenter’s requests and proved at the trial, she found that this A key problem is that Arendt this because he was convinced willing to deliver shocks labelled 450 volts picture did not hold true. On the mainly attended those parts of that the cause he was advancing to the powerless Learner (who was in fact a contrary, she was struck by the Eichmann’s trial at which his was right. The truly frightening stooge employed by Milgram to play this role). fact that Eichmann (pictured defence worked hard to present thing about Eichmann and his ilk The power of these studies was that they below) came across as a normal him as innocuous, precisely to is not that they didn’t know what appeared to provide startling evidence of our bureaucrat who had simply mitigate blame. they were doing, but that they capacity for blind obedience – evidence that been following orders – without The prosecution, however, knew full well what they were inhumanity springs not necessarily from question, imagination or insight. had presented compelling doing and believed their actions deep-seated hatred or pathology, but rather Famously, she claimed that the evidence that Eichmann was no to be justified, worthy and noble. from a much more mundane inclination to obey the orders of those in authority, however unreasonable or brutal these may be. This was In these, the proportion of participants who orders prove to be the least effective means the substance of the “agentic state theory” that kept on shocking to the bitter end varied from of securing obedience. We see this because Milgram developed to explain his findings 0 to 100 per cent. So it cannot be assumed Milgram scripted a number of verbal “prods” in his 1974 book Obedience to Authority. that people always obey. Indeed, as Milgram for the Experimenter to use if participants Importantly, it is an analysis that chimes with himself recognised in the title of an early were reticent about continuing, such as: political theorist Hannah Arendt’s notion of publication, these are studies of disobedience “You have no other choice, you must the “banality of evil”, which she famously as well as obedience. continue”. Yet almost every time this prod developed after observing the trial of the Next, even where there was obedience, it was used, participants refused to go on. So Nazi bureaucrat Adolf Eichmann (see “Nazis was far from blind. While participant Teachers whatever else the studies might show, the and the banality of evil”, above right). attended to the demands of the Experimenter, one thing they don’t show is that we have Milgram’s studies are influential to this they were well aware of the apparent suffering an inherent tendency to obey orders. day, but are also some of the most unethical of the Learner. Consequently, they were torn So what do they show? Putting the strands ever conducted in psychology. They could between these two voices, as film footage of of evidence together, we argue that the never be carried out in a similar form today Milgram’s experiment shows. What is more, balance between obedience and disobedience due to the extreme stress suffered by the hinges upon whether participants prioritise participants (see “Never again”, page 31). the voice of the Experimenter over that of the Ironically, these ethical problems have served Learner or vice versa. This, in turn, depends only to consolidate the influence of Milgram’s upon whether they identify more with the agentic state explanation. The impossibility of cause of science or more with the plight of replication has made it hard for an alternative the ordinary citizen. In these terms, the account to gain traction. problem with orders is that they undermine Nevertheless an alternative account is identification with the science by positioning needed. Not only have recent historical the Experimenter above and apart from studies led researchers to question Arendt’s participants, rather than as a collaborator in claims that Eichmann and his ilk simply went a common cause. And what this means is that along thoughtlessly with the orders of their those who shock do so not because they are superiors, but reanalysis of Milgram’s work unaware of the consequences of their actions, IS B has also led social psychologists to cast R but because they know what they are doing O serious doubt on the claim we are somehow C and believe it to be worthy. Rather than being programmed to obey authority. MANN/ blindly obedient, they are engaged followers. tt To start with, Milgram didn’t conduct just BE Moreover, participants are engaged one “obedience” study. He conducted over 25, because Milgram expended a great deal varying features of the set-up such as the Pathological, or just a workaday bureaucrat? of effort to engage them. In particular, he proximity of the Experimenter and Learner. The trial of Nazi chief Adolf Eichmann worked hard to persuade them that they > 13 September 2014 | NewScientist | 29 OPINION THE BIG IDEA THE PRISON EXPERIMENT were contributing to vitally important work ILGRAM which would bring about progress in scientific M Stanley Milgram’s obedience studies appear to understanding. In every detail he laboured provide compelling evidence that normal people to give the studies scientific authority, right LEXANDRA might be willing to kill a stranger simply if ordered down to the meticulously designed fake A ESY ESY to do so by someone in authority (see main story). shock generator (“Type ZLB”, supposedly T UR Co This aligns with conclusions typically drawn made by the Dyson Instrument Company). Y/ T from another classic piece of social psychological A 2012 paper we published in Perspectives research: the Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE). on Psychological Science provides preliminary NIVERSI U Conducted in 1971 by Philip Zimbardo, this ALE Y involved randomly assigning students to be “ Within days, ‘guards’ were either guards or prisoners within a mock prison. RCHIVES, A The objective was to observe how social subjecting the ‘prisoners’ S AND AND S relations within and between the two groups to degrading treatments” pt unfolded in the absence of an obviously malevolent authority. As in Milgram’s studies, the ANUSCRI support for this alternative analysis and is M results proved shocking: within a few days, the again rooted in Milgram’s own findings. In guards were subjecting prisoners to a host of this, we asked people to read descriptions of degrading and abusive treatments. This led to different variants of his study and then to the study being terminated after just six days. indicate how much they would identify with ERS, 1927-1993 (INCL.). (INCL.). 1927-1993 ERS, P the Experimenter and the Learner in each. We A P DESCENT INTO TYRANNY found that relative identification – the level of ILGRAM ILGRAM Zimbardo concluded that people descend into identification with the Experimenter minus M tyranny because they conform naturally and identification with the Learner – was highly ANLEY ANLEY unthinkingly to the toxic roles and scripts that correlated with the levels of shock that St accompany particular contexts – so that, for Milgram’s participants actually delivered. example, a brutal prison will inevitably create But while it is one thing to reinterpret elicit similar reactions to Milgram’s original. brutal guards. Like Milgram’s work, this analysis old data, it is quite another to produce new Initial findings show how participants orient to is closely aligned with the “banality of evil” thesis data to test alternative explanations. And in both the Learner and the Experimenter, trying devised by political theorist Hannah Arendt.
Recommended publications
  • Obedience to Authority”: from Blind Conformity to Engaged Followership
    LS13CH05-Haslam ARI 19 September 2017 7:37 Annual Review of Law and Social Science 50 Years of “Obedience to Authority”: From Blind Conformity to Engaged Followership S. Alexander Haslam1 and Stephen D. Reicher2 1School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia; email: [email protected] 2School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, Fife, KY16 9JP United Kingdom Annu. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 2017. 13:59–78 Keywords The Annual Review of Law and Social Science is Milgram, obedience, conformity, authority, identification, followership online at lawsocsci.annualreviews.org https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-lawsocsci- Abstract 110316-113710 Despite being conducted half a century ago, Stanley Milgram’s studies of Copyright c 2017 by Annual Reviews. ⃝ obedience to authority remain the most well-known, most controversial, and Access provided by 202.90.207.98 on 10/17/17. For personal use only. All rights reserved most important in social psychology. Yet in recent years, increased scrutiny has served to question the integrity of Milgram’s research reports, the va- Annu. Rev. Law. Soc. Sci. 2017.13:59-78. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org lidity of his explanation of the phenomena he reported, and the broader relevance of his research to processes of collective harm-doing. We review these debates and argue that the main problem with received understand- ings of Milgram’s work arises from seeing it as an exploration of obedience. Instead, we argue that it is better understood as providing insight into pro- cesses of engaged followership, in which people are prepared to harm others because they identify with their leaders’ cause and believe their actions to be virtuous.
    [Show full text]
  • Can We Still Use Milgram's 'Obedience to Authority' Experiments to Explain Mass Atrocities After the Op
    JPR Milgram Revisited: Can we still use Milgram’s ‘Obedience to Authority’ Experiments to Explain Mass Atrocities after the Opening of the Archives? Review Essay Alette Smeulers Introduction ilgram’s ‘obedience to authority experiments’ are, together with Zimbardo’s prison experiment, one of the most famous but also most controversial studies ever conducted.1 Since its first publication in 1963, Milgram’s Mresearch has drawn the attention not only of scholars but also of the media, and the experiment as well as the results have been widely debated and referenced, but also heavily criticized.2 The 50th anniversary of his experiments and the opening of the Yale archives led to a new wave of publications and criticism. A lot of material on the Milgram experiments which until then had been hidden from scholarly and public scrutiny cast serious doubts on Milgram’s actual findings and their relevance.3 Between 2011 and 2015, no fewer than four internation- al peer-reviewed journals published a special issue on Milgram’s experiments: The Psychologist in 2011, edited by Reicher and Haslam; Theoretical & Applied Ethics in 2013, edited by Herara; the Journal of Social Issues in 2014, edited by Reicher, Haslam and Miller; and Theory & Psychology in 2015, edited by Brannigan, Nicholson and Cherry. In addition, Gina Perry published a book on the Milgram experiments in 2012 entitled Behind the Shock Machine: The Untold Story of the Notorious I wish to thank Maria Ioannou, Chris Atkinson, George Smeulers, Nicola Quaedvlieg, and the editors of the journal for their useful suggestions, comments and corrections. 1 Stanley Milgram, Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View (New York: Harper and Row, 1974); Philip G.
    [Show full text]
  • Making 'Us' Better: High-Quality Athlete Leadership Relates to Health and Burnout in Professional Au
    European Journal of Sport Science ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tejs20 Making ‘us’ better: High-quality athlete leadership relates to health and burnout in professional Australian football teams Katrien Fransen , S. Alexander Haslam , Niklas K. Steffens , Clifford J. Mallett , Kim Peters & Filip Boen To cite this article: Katrien Fransen , S. Alexander Haslam , Niklas K. Steffens , Clifford J. Mallett , Kim Peters & Filip Boen (2020) Making ‘us’ better: High-quality athlete leadership relates to health and burnout in professional Australian football teams, European Journal of Sport Science, 20:7, 953-963, DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1680736 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2019.1680736 Published online: 27 Oct 2019. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 375 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 3 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tejs20 European Journal of Sport Science, 2020 Vol. 20, No. 7, 953–963, https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2019.1680736 Making ‘us’ better: High-quality athlete leadership relates to health and burnout in professional Australian football teams KATRIEN FRANSEN 1, S. ALEXANDER HASLAM 2, NIKLAS K. STEFFENS 2, CLIFFORD J. MALLETT3, KIM PETERS 2, & FILIP BOEN 1 1Department of Movement Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium; 2School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia & 3School of Human Movement and Nutrition Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia Abstract Overtraining, exhaustion, and burnout are widely recognized problems amongst elite athletes. The present research addresses this issue by exploring the extent to which high-quality athlete leadership is associated with elite athletes’ health and burnout.
    [Show full text]
  • Consensus Statement on the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study Professors Craig Haney, Alex Haslam, Stephen Reicher, and Philip Zimbardo
    Consensus Statement on the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study Professors Craig Haney, Alex Haslam, Stephen Reicher, and Philip Zimbardo We, the undersigned researchers who conducted the Stanford Prison Experiment (Philip Zimbardo and Craig Haney) and BBC Prison Study (Alex Haslam and Stephen Reicher), recognize that our studies, results, and public statements have engendered strong debate and, at times, misunderstanding within and beyond psychology. In an effort to promote constructive scientific dialogue, we are therefore releasing this consensus statement to highlight common ground and clarify our views on the research in question. First, we Jointly believe that it is critically important to develop a scientific understanding of toxic human behavior, including brutality and the abuse of authority and power. Second, we regard the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study as valid studies and valuable resources for advancing such understanding. At the same time, we recognize that both investigations have methodological limitations and are best viewed as one-trial demonstration studies rather than traditional experiments. Third, the BBC Prison Study differs from the Stanford Prison Experiment in essential ways and is not a direct replication of the earlier study. We do, however, see value in comparing the different outcomes of the two studies (and others) as a means of advancing conceptual understanding. Fourth, the behaviors observed in the Stanford Prison Experiment and BBC Prison Study were a function of many factors, including roles, norms, leadership, social identification, group pressure, and individual differences, not all of which are necessarily mutually exclusive. It is only natural that explanations of social behavior will be complex and multifactorial.
    [Show full text]
  • Debate Around Leadership in the Stanford Prison Experiment: Reply to Zimbardo and Haney (2020) and Chan Et Al
    American Psychologist © 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 75, No. 3, 406–407 ISSN: 0003-066X http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000627 REPLY Debate Around Leadership in the Stanford Prison Experiment: Reply to Zimbardo and Haney (2020) and Chan et al. (2020) Stephen D. Reicher Jay J. Van Bavel University of St. Andrews New York University S. Alexander Haslam University of Queensland Access to the Stanford University archive has revealed new material that makes it possible to debate the precise nature and causes of events in the Stanford Prison Experiment. What the authors see as important is that these materials show the experimenters engaged in processes of identity leadership, which encouraged guard cruelty by presenting it as necessary for the achievement of noble collective goals. However, the authors encourage students, teachers, and researchers to engage with this new material themselves to explore alternative perspec- tives on what actually occurred in the study. Keywords: leadership, followership, social identification, Stanford Prison Experiment, open science Two decades ago, when we were planning our BBC of the SPE. This concerns what Moscovici (1976) identified Prison Study (Reicher & Haslam, 2006), we argued for the as a general conformity bias in social psychology. The focus resumption of normal scientific debate around the Stanford in the SPE, as elsewhere, was on the ways in which people Prison Experiment (SPE; Haney, Banks, & Zimbardo, conform to expectations (in this case, role expectations), to 1973). In particular, we sought to invigorate discussion the exclusion of acts of nonconformity. Yet even from the around the critical question of what can lead ordinary hu- limited material then available, there was considerable ev- mans to act in extraordinarily inhumane ways.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Identity Pathways Associated with Divergent Traumatic Responses
    European Review of Social Psychology ISSN: 1046-3283 (Print) 1479-277X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pers20 The social psychology of responses to trauma: social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses Orla T. Muldoon, S. Alexander Haslam, Catherine Haslam, Tegan Cruwys, Michelle Kearns & Jolanda Jetten To cite this article: Orla T. Muldoon, S. Alexander Haslam, Catherine Haslam, Tegan Cruwys, Michelle Kearns & Jolanda Jetten (2019) The social psychology of responses to trauma: social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses, European Review of Social Psychology, 30:1, 311-348 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2020.1711628 © 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group. Published online: 10 Jan 2020. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 395 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pers20 EUROPEAN REVIEW OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2020, VOL. 30, NO. 1, 311–348 https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2020.1711628 ARTICLE The social psychology of responses to trauma: social identity pathways associated with divergent traumatic responses Orla T. Muldoon a, S. Alexander Haslam b, Catherine Haslam b, Tegan Cruwys c, Michelle Kearns a,d and Jolanda Jetten b aCentre for Social Issues Research, Department of Psychology, University of Limerick, Limerick V94 T9PX, Ireland; bSchool of Psychology, McElwain Building University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia; cResearch School of Psychology, The Australian National University, 39 Science Road, ACT 0200, Australia; dInternational Rescue Committee, Freetown, Sierra Leone ABSTRACT Research in clinical psychology and social psychiatry has highlighted the impor- tance of social factors for outcomes following trauma.
    [Show full text]
  • *17 Haslam CV Small
    Curriculum Vitae S. Alexander HASLAM Professor of Psychology, Australian Laureate Fellow University of Queensland Date of Birth December, 1962 Academic Qualifications 1985 M.A. Hons (first class), Psychology University of St Andrews 1991 PhD, Psychology Macquarie University PhD title: Social Comparative Context, Self-Categorization and Stereotyping Supervisor: J. C. Turner Main Research areas Psychology in organizations — with an emphasis on the contribution of social identity to leadership, motivation, communication, decision-making, negotiation, and productivity. The social psychology of stereotyping, prejudice, and tyranny — exploring the role of group processes in the dynamics of intergroup relations and conflict. Social processes in health and well-being — looking at the contribution of group life to stress, coping and well-being, especially in vulnerable populations. Research methodology — focusing on issues of research design, ethics, and uncertainty management. Previous Appointments 2/1990 – 2/1991 Lecturer, Department of Behavioural Science, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney 3/1991 – 2/2001 Research Officer, Lecturer, Senior Lecturer, Reader, School of Psychology, The Australian National University 3/2001 – 2012 Professor, School of Psychology, University of Exeter 8/2012 – present Professor and ARC Laureate Fellow, School of Psychology, University of Queensland Academic Awards and Distinctions 1982 Ferguson Prize and Class Medal for Psychology, University of St Andrews 1982 Lawson Prize and Class Medal for
    [Show full text]
  • RUNNING HEAD: Identification in Group Therapy Predictors of Social Identification in Group Therapy TEGAN CRUWYS 1 * NIKLAS K. ST
    RUNNING HEAD: Identification in group therapy Predictors of social identification in group therapy TEGAN CRUWYS 1 * NIKLAS K. STEFFENS 2 S. ALEXANDER HASLAM 2 CATHERINE HASLAM 2 MATTHEW J. HORNSEY 3 CRAIG MCGARTY 4 DANIEL P. SKORICH 2 Citation: Cruwys, T., Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., Haslam, C., Hornsey, M. J., McGarty, C., & Skorich, D. P. (2020). Predictors of social identification in group therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 30(3), 348-361. 1. Research School of Psychology, Australian National University, CANBERRA ACT 2601 2. School of Psychology, University of Queensland, ST LUCIA QLD 4072 3. UQ Business School, ST LUCIA QLD 4072 4. School of Social Sciences and Psychology, Western Sydney University, PENRITH NSW 2751 *Corresponding author: 1. Dr Tegan Cruwys, [email protected] Research School of Psychology, Australian National University, CANBERRA ACT 2601 AUSTRALIA Acknowledgements: This project was supported by funding from the Australian Research Council (DE160100592 and FL110100199). The funder had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, writing, or decision to publish. We are grateful for the support of Genevieve A. Dingle, Nicole Fox, Hayley McMahon and Tian P. S. Oei. IDENTIFICATION IN GROUP THERAPY | 2 Abstract Background: Group therapy is a popular and effective format for psychological intervention, and both anecdotal and empirical data consistently point to group dynamics as a primary driver of its benefits. However, to date there has been no systematic investigation of what facilitates an engaged, cohesive group environment. We argue that this is social identification and explore the features of groups that help to build this. Method: We present two longitudinal studies of group therapy and examine the predictors of social identification.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Identification-Building Interventions to Improve Health: a Systematic Review and Meta- Analysis
    Health Psychology Review ISSN: 1743-7199 (Print) 1743-7202 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rhpr20 Social identification-building interventions to improve health: a systematic review and meta- analysis Niklas K. Steffens, Crystal J. LaRue, Catherine Haslam, Zoe C. Walter, Tegan Cruwys, Katie A. Munt, S. Alexander Haslam, Jolanda Jetten & Mark Tarrant To cite this article: Niklas K. Steffens, Crystal J. LaRue, Catherine Haslam, Zoe C. Walter, Tegan Cruwys, Katie A. Munt, S. Alexander Haslam, Jolanda Jetten & Mark Tarrant (2019): Social identification-building interventions to improve health: a systematic review and meta-analysis, Health Psychology Review, DOI: 10.1080/17437199.2019.1669481 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2019.1669481 View supplementary material Accepted author version posted online: 17 Sep 2019. Published online: 07 Oct 2019. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 178 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rhpr20 HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2019.1669481 Social identification-building interventions to improve health: a systematic review and meta-analysis Niklas K. Steffens a, Crystal J. LaRue a, Catherine Haslam a, Zoe C. Walter a, Tegan Cruwys b, Katie A. Munta, S. Alexander Haslam a, Jolanda Jetten a and Mark Tarrantc aSchool of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia; bResearch School of Psychology, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australia; cUniversity of Exeter Medical School, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY There is growing evidence that social identity processes play an important Received 9 May 2019 role in a range of health outcomes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology of Tyranny: the BBC Prison Study
    TYRANNYTHE PSYCHOLOGY OF POWER CORRUPTS, AND ABSOLUTE POWER CORRUPTS ABSOLUTELY—OR DOES IT? BY S. ALEXANDER HASLAM AND STEPHEN D. REICHER Images of inhumanity and atrocity are burned into our memories. Jewish men, women and children being herded into gas chambers. Entire villages destroyed by rampaging gangs in Rwanda. The systematic use of rape and the destruction of communities as part of “ethnic cleansing” in the Balkans. The massacre at My Lai in South Vietnam, the abuse of Iraqi prisoners in Abu Ghraib, and most re- cently, the carnage wrought by suicide bombers in Baghdad, Jerusalem, London and Madrid. Refl ecting on these events, we inevitably ask: What makes people so brutal? Are they mentally ill? Are they the products of dysfunctional families or cultures? Or, more disturbingly, is anyone capable of taking part in collective ruthlessness given the right—or rather, the wrong—circumstances? Now the latest research, including possibly the largest social-psychology experiment in three CREDIT CREDIT decades, is providing a new window on these conundrums. 44 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MIND COPYRIGHT 2005 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC. COPYRIGHT 2005 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC. Joseph Stalin, Questions about why groups can behave bad- early 1960s by Stanley Milgram. Ordinary, well- Adolf Hitler, ly have driven some of the most signifi cant devel- adjusted males who took part in a bogus memo- Idi Amin and opments in social psychology in the 60 years ry experiment were told to deliver electric shocks Saddam Hussein since World War II ended. Starting with the need of increasing magnitude to another person who had powerful to understand the psychological processes that posed as the learner.
    [Show full text]
  • Shared Social Identity in Leadership Stephen D Reicher School Of
    Shared social identity in leadership Stephen D Reicher School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews S. Alexander Haslam School of Psychology, University of Queensland Michael J Platow School of Psychology, Australian National University Address for correspondence: Stephen Reicher; [email protected] Abstract In this paper we review recent evidence on the social identity model of leadership. First, we explain how this model is rooted in the social identity approach in social psychology and, specifically, the notion that shared reality and joint action in groups derives from shared social identity. We then show how effective leadership is a process of social identity management and we examine both the antecedents, the psychological and the political consequences of managing social identities Keywords: Leadership; social identity; group processes; shared reality; identity management; entrepreneurs of identity The group context of leadership Approaches to leadership differ in the breadth of their analytical focus. The trait approach1 concentrates on the characteristics of the leader alone. Transactional2 and transformational3,4 approaches broaden this out to address the relationships between leaders and followers. The social identity model of leadership5,6,7 extends the focus still further. It starts from the premise that a leader is always the leader of some specific constituency: a political party, a religion, a country or whatever. Accordingly, it argues that leadership should be understood in terms of the relations between the leader and followers within a particular social group8. It follows that an understanding of leadership depends upon an understanding of group processes more generally. The social identity approach in social psychology — comprised of social identity theory9 and self-categorization theory10 — argues that group behavior is underpinned by a sense of social identity (or ‘we-ness’) that is shared by a given a set of people.
    [Show full text]
  • After Shock Towards a Social Identity Explanation of the Milgram Obedience Studies
    163 The British Psychological British Journal of Social Psychology (2011), 50, 163–169 Society C 2011 The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com Commentary After shock? Towards a social identity explanation of the Milgram ‘obedience’ studies ∗ Stephen Reicher1 and S. Alexander Haslam2 1University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife, UK 2University of Exeter, Exeter, Devon, UK Russell’s forensic archival investigations reveal the great lengths that Milgram went to in order to construct an experiment that would ‘shock the world’. However, in achieving this goal it is also apparent that the drama of the ‘basic’ obedience paradigm draws attention away both from variation in obedience and from the task of explaining that variation. Building on points that Russell and others have made concerning the competing ‘pulls’ that are at play in the Milgram paradigm, this paper outlines the potential for a social identity perspective on obedience to provide such an explanation. The theatre of ‘obedience’ The Milgram studies are great drama as well as great science. Therein lies much of the secret of their success. Few can be left unmoved by reading transcripts of the unfolding sessions – or still better, by watching these on grainy black-and-white film. Few can escape being gripped as the participants agonize over their predicament before finally, seemingly inexorably, pressing the buttons on Milgram’s bogus shock generator. Certainly, Milgram’s films and the narrative style of his classic 1974 text have contributed to his work becoming one of the best-known pieces of psychological research ever conducted – not only in the academic world but also in the larger culture (Blass, 2004; Novick, 2000).
    [Show full text]