Analytic Morphology – Merging the Paradigmatic and Syntagmatic Perspective in a Treebank

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Analytic Morphology – Merging the Paradigmatic and Syntagmatic Perspective in a Treebank Analytic Morphology – Merging the Paradigmatic and Syntagmatic Perspective in a Treebank Vladimír Petkevicˇ Alexandr Rosen Hana Skoumalová Premyslˇ Vítovec Charles University in Prague, Faculty of Arts [email protected] [email protected] Abstract (often even phonological word); (ii) AVFs may be expressed by a potentially discontinuous string We present an account of analytic verb of a content verb and multiple auxiliaries, some- forms in a treebank of Czech texts. Ac- times in an order determined by information struc- cording to the Czech linguistic tradition, ture rather than by rules of morphology or syntax description of periphrastic constructions is proper; (iii) some auxiliary forms share properties a task for morphology. On the other hand, with some content words – like weak pronouns, their components cannot be analyzed sep- the past tense auxiliary is a 2nd position clitic. arately from syntax. We show how the Our claim is that the two views are compati- paradigmatic and syntagmatic views can ble, complementary and amenable to formaliza- be represented within a single framework. tion within a single framework, combining the traditional paradigmatic view with a syntagmatic 1 Introduction view. This reconciliatory effort is part of a more Analytic verb forms (henceforth AVFs) consist of general goal: a choice of different interpretations one or more auxiliaries and a content verb. The of annotated corpus data, depending on the prefer- auxiliaries can be seen either as marking the con- ences of a user or an application. tent verb with morphological categories or as be- AVFs are assigned a syntactic structure: the (fi- ing part of a multi-word expression, to which the nite) auxiliary is treated as the surface head, gov- categories are assigned. This is the perspective erning the rest of the form – the deep head.2 taken by all standard grammar books of Czech, In Czech, AVFs are used to express the verbal which treat AVFs as a morphological rather than categories of mood, tense and voice in periphrastic a syntactic phenomenon. AVFs are listed in con- passive (all moods and tenses), in periphrastic fu- jugation paradigms quite like synthetic forms for a ture, in 1st and 2nd person past tense, in pluper- good reason: from a meaning-based view, whether fect and in present and past conditional. In all a certain category in a certain language happens to these forms the auxiliary is být ‘to be’. Here we be expressed by a single word or a string of words focus on past tense and conditional forms, includ- is an epiphenomenon. ing pluperfect and past conditional, but the solu- From a different perspective, each of the com- tion works for all the above AVFs, and covers also ponents has its role in satisfying a syntactic gram- negation of some components of the AVFs by the maticality constraint and in making a contribution prefix ne- and can be extended to some other kinds to the lexical, grammatical or semantic meaning of function words, such as prepositions and con- of the whole. This approach is common in both junctions. In (1)–(4) below we show some prop- corpus and generative linguistics (including the- erties of the past and conditional forms. The fi- 1 ories such as LFG or HPSG), where each form nite auxiliary is marked for person, number and is treated as a syntactic word and AVFs belong to mood, while the l-participle3 is marked for gender the domain of syntax. As a result, morphological and number. Past tense (1) consists of the aux- categories are not assigned to units spanning word iliary in the present tense and the l-participle of boundaries. This is for several reasons: (i) an AVF does not emerge as a single orthographical word 2We use the term government in the sense of “subcatego- rization” or “imposition of valency requirements.” 1See, e.g., Webelhuth (1995), Dalrymple (1999), Pollard 3We avoid the frequently used term past participle be- and Sag (1994). cause the same form is also used in present conditional. 9 Proceedings of the 5th Workshop on Balto-Slavic Natural Language Processing, pages 9–16, Hissar, Bulgaria, 10–11 September 2015. the content verb. Present conditional (2) consists and Karlík (2004), who analyze past tense and pe- of the conditional auxiliary and the content verb’s riphrastic passive within the Minimalist Program. l-participle. Past conditional (3) includes an addi- A non-transformational account was pursued by tional l-participle of the auxiliary. In (4) we show Karel Oliva in an HPSG-inspired prototype gram- that other words can be inserted, the auxiliary mar checker of Czech (Avgustinova et al., 1995). l-participle can be repeated, and any l-participle HPSG and LFG have been used to account for sim- can be negated. ilar phenomena in closely related Polish, where the border between morphology and syntax is even (1) Já jsem pˇrišel less apparent than in Czech: all forms of the past I be.PRS.1SG come.PTCP.M.SG tense and conditional auxiliaries are floating suf- ‘I have come.’ fixes, attached either directly to the l-participle, (2) Já bych pˇrišel or to some other preceding word. In the follow- I be.COND.1SG come.PTCP.M.SG ing, we briefly review several proposals for Polish, ‘I would come.’ with an extension to Czech. Based on the analysis of similar phenomena (3) Já bych byl pˇrišel in West European languages, Borsley (1999) pro- I be.COND.1SG be.PTCP.M.SG come.PTCP.M.SG poses two structures for modelling Polish AVFs: ‘I would have come.’ (i) classic VP complementation where the auxil- (4) Kdybys tenkrát nebyl iary is a subject-raising verb selecting a phrasal If-be.COND.2SG back then be.PTCP.M.SG.NEG complement headed by an l-participle (Fig. 1), býval tak duchapˇrítomnˇe and (ii) flat structures where the auxiliary subcat- be.PTCP.M.SG.ITER so readily egorizes for an l-participle and its complements 6 zasáhl... (Fig. 2). The former is used for future tense while intervene.PTCP.M.SG the latter for present conditional and past tense. ‘If you haven’t intervened so readily back This distinction is motivated by the ability or in- then...’ ability of the auxiliary to be preceded by the asso- ciated l-participle and its complements: while the We exemplify the solution using a treebank of future auxiliary allows for VP-preposing, the other 4 Czech. The framework is based on the HPSG. auxiliaries are prohibitive in this respect. The annotation, originally produced by a stochas- tic dependency parser, is checked by a formal VP grammar, using a valency lexicon and imple- Aux VP mented in Trale.5 Trees complying with grammat- ical and lexical constraints are augmented with in- Figure 1: VP complementation. formation derived from the lexicon and any anno- tation provided by a stochastic parser. VP 2 Previous Work Aux VCC... Grammars of Czech take a paradigmatic perspec- tive, treating AVFs as an exclusively morphologi- Figure 2: Flat structure. cal phenomenon (Karlík et al., 1995; Cvrcekˇ et al., 2010; Komárek et al., 1986), glossed over without Kups´c´ (2000) follows Borsley (1999) but rejects describing their syntagmatic and word-order prop- the flat structure for past tense and present con- erties. In Komárek et al. (1986), components of ditional as it makes incorrect predictions with re- AVFs are assigned a particular grammatical mean- spect to clitic climbing. Instead, she assumes VP ing (person, number, tense, mood, voice) but their complementation for all AVFs. syntactic status is not specified. Kups´c´ and Tseng (2005) argue against the uni- The syntagmatic approach has been introduced fied treatment of AVFs. Only the future tense aux- to Czech by Veselovská (2003) and Veselovská iliary behaves like a full syntactic word. In con- 4See, e.g., Pollard and Sag (1994). trast, the forms of conditional auxiliary, albeit syn- 5See http://www.sfs.uni-tuebingen.de/hpsg/archive/ projects/trale/. For more details see Jelínek et al. (2014). 6Heads are denoted by boxed nodes in the figures. 10 VP SurfHead DeepHead VP+MRK XP bych2 Figure 3: Local agreement marker. SurfHead DeepHead nebyl1 spal3 VP Figure 5: Structure of nebyl bych spal ‘I wouldn’t have slept’. XP+MRK VP Figure 4: Nonlocal agreement marker. 3 Our Approach 3.1 Two Types of Heads: Surface and Deep tactic words, are clitics and thus subject to spe- In addition to strictly linguistic criteria for an op- cific word order constraints (dependencies on var- timal analysis of AVFs, our choice of the core rep- ious clitic hosts). Past tense is viewed as a sim- resentation format was influenced by the treebank ple tense and the past tense agreement markings design, which should allow for the derivation of are treated as inflectional elements, even if they syntactic structure and categorial labels of vari- are not attached to the l-participle. This analysis ous shapes and flavours to be used in queries, re- builds on (i) an observation that agreement mark- sponses and exported data. Adopting a uniform ings are much more closely bound to the preceding analysis for all AVFs simplifies the task. Each word than conditional clitics, and (ii) the fact that AVF is represented as a syntactic phrase with two there are no agreement markings used in the third constituents: a surface head daughter represent- person. As a result, the l-participle becomes the ing the auxiliary, and a deep head daughter rep- head of the whole structure. In order to ensure that resenting the auxiliary’s VP complement, which the agreement marking appears somewhere in the includes the content verb.7 Multiple auxiliaries structure the head acts as its trigger, carried by an within a single AVF are surface heads within re- agreement marker, either the head itself (Fig.
Recommended publications
  • The Syntactic and Semantic Relationship in Synthetic Languages
    This is the published version of the bachelor thesis: Navarro Caparrós, Raquel; van Wijk Adan, Malou, dir. The Syntactic and Semantic Relationship in Synthetic Languagues : Linear Modification in Latin and Old English. 2016. 41 pag. (838 Grau en Estudis d’Anglès i de Clàssiques) This version is available at https://ddd.uab.cat/record/164293 under the terms of the license The Syntactic and Semantic Relationship in Synthetic Languages: Linear Modification in Latin and Old English TFG – Grau d’Estudis d’Anglès i Clàssiques Supervisor: Malou van Wijk Adan Raquel Navarro Caparrós June 2016 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisor Ms. Malou van Wijk Adan for her assistance and patience during the period of my research. This project would not have been possible without her guidance and valuable comments throughout. I would also like to thank all my teachers of Classical Studies and English Studies for sharing their knowledge with me throughout these four years. Finally, I wish to acknowledge my mum and my sister Ruth whose invaluable support helped me to overcome any obstacle, especially in the last two years. Without them, I am sure that I would have never been able to get where I am now. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Abbreviations ...................................................................................................... ii List of Tables .................................................................................................................. iii Abstract ..........................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • II Levels of Language
    II Levels of language 1 Phonetics and phonology 1.1 Characterising articulations 1.1.1 Consonants 1.1.2 Vowels 1.2 Phonotactics 1.3 Syllable structure 1.4 Prosody 1.5 Writing and sound 2 Morphology 2.1 Word, morpheme and allomorph 2.1.1 Various types of morphemes 2.2 Word classes 2.3 Inflectional morphology 2.3.1 Other types of inflection 2.3.2 Status of inflectional morphology 2.4 Derivational morphology 2.4.1 Types of word formation 2.4.2 Further issues in word formation 2.4.3 The mixed lexicon 2.4.4 Phonological processes in word formation 3 Lexicology 3.1 Awareness of the lexicon 3.2 Terms and distinctions 3.3 Word fields 3.4 Lexicological processes in English 3.5 Questions of style 4 Syntax 4.1 The nature of linguistic theory 4.2 Why analyse sentence structure? 4.2.1 Acquisition of syntax 4.2.2 Sentence production 4.3 The structure of clauses and sentences 4.3.1 Form and function 4.3.2 Arguments and complements 4.3.3 Thematic roles in sentences 4.3.4 Traces 4.3.5 Empty categories 4.3.6 Similarities in patterning Raymond Hickey Levels of language Page 2 of 115 4.4 Sentence analysis 4.4.1 Phrase structure grammar 4.4.2 The concept of ‘generation’ 4.4.3 Surface ambiguity 4.4.4 Impossible sentences 4.5 The study of syntax 4.5.1 The early model of generative grammar 4.5.2 The standard theory 4.5.3 EST and REST 4.5.4 X-bar theory 4.5.5 Government and binding theory 4.5.6 Universal grammar 4.5.7 Modular organisation of language 4.5.8 The minimalist program 5 Semantics 5.1 The meaning of ‘meaning’ 5.1.1 Presupposition and entailment 5.2
    [Show full text]
  • Grammatical Analyticity Versus Syntheticity in Varieties of English
    Language Variation and Change, 00 (2009), 1–35. © Cambridge University Press, 2009 0954-3945/09 $16.00 doi:10.1017/S0954394509990123 1 2 3 4 Typological parameters of intralingual variability: 5 Grammatical analyticity versus syntheticity in varieties 6 7 of English 8 B ENEDIKT S ZMRECSANYI 9 Freiburg Institute for Advanced Studies 10 11 12 ABSTRACT 13 Drawing on terminology, concepts, and ideas developed in quantitative morphological 14 typology, the present study takes an exclusive interest in the coding of grammatical 15 information. It offers a sweeping overview of intralingual variability in terms of 16 overt grammatical analyticity (the text frequency of free grammatical markers), 17 grammatical syntheticity (the text frequency of bound grammatical markers), and 18 grammaticity (the text frequency of grammatical markers, bound or free) in English. 19 The variational dimensions investigated include geography, text types, and real time. 20 Empirically, the study taps into a number of publicly accessible text corpora that 21 comprise a large number of different varieties of English. Results are interpreted in 22 terms of how speakers and writers seek to achieve communicative goals while 23 minimizing different types of complexity. 24 25 This study surveys language-internal variability and short-term diachronic change, 26 along dimensions that are familiar from the cross-linguistic typology of languages. 27 The terms analytic and synthetic have a long and venerable tradition in linguistics, 28 going back to the 19th century and August Wilhelm von Schlegel, who is usually 29 credited for coining the opposition (Schlegel, 1818). This is, alas, not the place to 30 even attempt to review the rich history of thought in this area (but see Schwegler, “ 31 1990:chapter 1 for an excellent overview).
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling Infant Segmentation of Two Morphologically Diverse Languages
    Modeling infant segmentation of two morphologically diverse languages Georgia Rengina Loukatou1 Sabine Stoll 2 Damian Blasi2 Alejandrina Cristia1 (1) LSCP, Département d’études cognitives, ENS, EHESS, CNRS, PSL Research University, Paris, France (2) University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland [email protected] RÉSUMÉ Les nourrissons doivent trouver des limites de mots dans le flux continu de la parole. De nombreuses études computationnelles étudient de tels mécanismes. Cependant, la majorité d’entre elles se sont concentrées sur l’anglais, une langue morphologiquement simple et qui rend la tâche de segmen- tation aisée. Les langues polysynthétiques - pour lesquelles chaque mot est composé de plusieurs morphèmes - peuvent présenter des difficultés supplémentaires lors de la segmentation. De plus, le mot est considéré comme la cible de la segmentation, mais il est possible que les nourrissons segmentent des morphèmes et non pas des mots. Notre étude se concentre sur deux langues ayant des structures morphologiques différentes, le chintang et le japonais. Trois algorithmes de segmentation conceptuellement variés sont évalués sur des représentations de mots et de morphèmes. L’évaluation de ces algorithmes nous mène à tirer plusieurs conclusions. Le modèle lexical est le plus performant, notamment lorsqu’on considère les morphèmes et non pas les mots. De plus, en faisant varier leur évaluation en fonction de la langue, le japonais nous apporte de meilleurs résultats. ABSTRACT A rich literature explores unsupervised segmentation algorithms infants could use to parse their input, mainly focusing on English, an analytic language where word, morpheme, and syllable boundaries often coincide. Synthetic languages, where words are multi-morphemic, may present unique diffi- culties for segmentation.
    [Show full text]
  • Polysynthetic Structures of Lowland Amazonia
    OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – REVISES, Sat Aug 19 2017, NEWGEN Chapter 15 Polysynthetic Structures of Lowland Amazonia Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald 15.1 Lowland Amazonian languages: a backdrop The Amazon basin is an area of high linguistic diversity (rivalled only by the island of New Guinea). It comprises around 350 languages grouped into over fifteen language families, in addition to a number of isolates. The six major linguistic families of the Amazon basin are as follows. • The Arawak language family is the largest in South America in terms of its geographical spread, with over forty extant languages between the Caribbean and Argentina. Well- established subgroups include Campa in Peru and a few small North Arawak groupings in Brazil and Venezuela. Arawak languages are spoken in at least ten locations north of the River Amazon, and in at least ten south of it. European languages contain a number of loans from Arawak languages, among them hammock and tobacco. • The Tupí language family consists of about seventy languages; nine of its ten branches are spoken exclusively in Amazonia. The largest branch, Tupí- Guaraní, extends beyond the Amazonian Basin into Bolivia and Paraguay. Loans from Tupí-Guaraní languages include jaguar and jacaranda. • Carib languages number about twenty five, and are spoken in various locations in Brazil and Venezuela in northern Amazonia, and in the region of the Upper Xingu and adjacent areas of Mato Grosso in Brazil south of the River Amazon. The place name ‘Caribbean’ and the noun cannibal (a version of the ethnonym ‘Carib’) are a legacy from Carib languages. • Panoan languages number about thirty, and are spoken on the eastern side of the Andes in Peru and adjacent areas of Brazil.
    [Show full text]
  • WORD ORDER in ENGLISH and SERBIAN Jelisaveta Safranj
    DISCOURSE and INTERACTION 4/1/2011 WORD ORDER IN ENGLISH AND SERBIAN Jelisaveta Safranj Abstract The paper deals with case and grammatical relations in English and Serbian. Serbian has a very rich case system involving the infl ection for case of nouns, pronouns and adjectives. Since there are seven cases in Serbian, they bear the main burden of marking the syntactic function of a noun phrase and the word order is relatively free. However, in English nouns are not case marked and only the English pronominal system can be said to have the grammatical category of case and most of the grammarians would say that English word order is fi xed. Due to their different nature, Serbian being a synthetic language and English an analytic one, word order seems to have different functional values in the two languages. In English it is the main syntactic means, while in Serbian it is mainly a pragmatic, textual and stylistic means. Key words word order, grammatical relations, case, English, Serbian 1 Introduction According to Mathesius (1975), Firbas (1992) and Vachek (1994) the main principles determining word order in Indo-European languages are the linearity principle, i.e. ordering elements in accordance with linear modifi cation (Bolinger 1952), and the grammatical principle, i.e. ordering elements in accordance with a grammaticalized word-order pattern. The linearity principle is stronger in languages with fl exible word order, such as Serbian, where gradation of meaning is produced more easily than in languages with fi xed word order, such as English, in which the linearity principle is subordinate to the grammatical principle.
    [Show full text]
  • A Dictionary of Linguistics
    MID-CENTURY REFERENCE LIBRARY DAGOBERT D. RUNES, Ph.D., General Editor AVAILABLE Dictionary of Ancient History Dictionary of the Arts Dictionary of European History Dictionary of Foreign Words and Phrases Dictionary of Linguistics Dictionary of Mysticism Dictionary of Mythology Dictionary of Philosophy Dictionary of Psychoanalysis Dictionary of Science and Technology Dictionary of Sociology Dictionary of Word Origins Dictionary of World Literature Encyclopedia of Aberrations Encyclopedia of the Arts Encyclopedia of Atomic Energy Encyclopedia of Criminology Encyclopedia of Literature Encyclopedia of Psychology Encyclopedia of Religion Encyclopedia of Substitutes and Synthetics Encyclopedia of Vocational Guidance Illustrated Technical Dictionary Labor Dictionary Liberal Arts Dictionary Military and Naval Dictionary New Dictionary of American History New Dictionary of Psychology Protestant Dictionary Slavonic Encyclopedia Theatre Dictionary Tobacco Dictionary FORTHCOMING Beethoven Encyclopedia Dictionary of American Folklore Dictionary of American Grammar and Usage Dictionary of American Literature Dictionary of American Maxims Dictionary of American Proverbs Dictionary of American Superstitions Dictionary of American Synonyms Dictionary of Anthropology Dictionary of Arts and Crafts Dictionary of Asiatic History Dictionary of Astronomy Dictionary of Child Guidance Dictionary of Christian Antiquity Dictionary of Discoveries and Inventions Dictionary of Etiquette Dictionary of Forgotten Words Dictionary of French Literature Dictionary of Geography
    [Show full text]
  • Analytic and Synthetic: Typological Change in Varieties of European Languages
    1 In: Buchstaller, Isabelle & Siebenhaar, Beat (eds.) 2017. Language Variation – European Perspectives VI: Selected papers from the 8th International Conference on Language Variation in Europe (ICLaVE 8), Leipzig 2015. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Analytic and synthetic: Typological change in varieties of European languages MARTIN HASPELMATH & SUSANNE MARIA MICHAELIS 1. The macro-comparative perspective: Language typology and language contact Since the early 19th century, linguists have sometimes tried to understand language change from a broader perspective, as affecting the entire character of a language. Since A.W. von Schlegel (1818), it has been commonplace to say that Latin was a SYNTHETIC language, while the Romance languages are (more) ANALYTIC, i.e. make more use of auxiliary words and periphrastic constructions of various kinds. In this paper, we adopt a macro-comparative perspective on language variation in Europe, corresponding to our background in world-wide typology of contact languages (Michaelis et al. 2013) and general world-wide typology (Haspelmath et al. 2005). While variationist studies typically ask for patterns of variation within a single language, we ask whether there is a “big picture” in addition to all the details, in the tradition of A.W. von Schlegel. In particular, the topic of this paper is the replacement of synthetic patterns by analytic patterns that has interested typologists and historical linguists since the 19th century and that has recently been the focus of some prominent research in variationist studies of English and English-lexified creoles (Szmrecsanyi 2009; Kortmann & Szmrecsanyi 2011; Siegel et al. 2014; Szmrecsanyi 2016). By way of a first simple illustration, consider the examples in Table 1, where the symbol “>>” means that the newer pattern on the right-hand side competes with and tends to replace the older pattern on the left-hand side.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphology Handout 1
    Kirk Hazen HEL Morphology Handout 1. Analytic. 1.1. Most words are single morphemes (even for concepts like "we" which is both first person and plural (i.e. two different concepts and two different words in an analytic language). 1.2. Mandarin Chinese. 1.2.1. [wò mën tan tcin] 'I plural play piano' 1.2.2. [wò mën tan tcin lë] 'I plural play piano past ' 1.2.3. [ta da wò mën] 'S/he hit I plural ' 2. Synthetic. 2.1. Synthetic languages combine several morphemes into single words. How they combine them determines what kind of synthetic language they are. 2.1.1. Hungarian 2.1.1.1. [òz èmber la…tjò ò kuca…t] 'the man sees the dog-object ' 2.1.1.2. [òz kucò la…tjò ò èmbert] 'the dog sees the man-object ' 2.1.1.3. [ò ha…zunk zold] 'the house-our green' Our house is green. 2.1.1.4. [ò ha…zòd zold] 'the house-your green' Your house is green. 2.1.2. Agglutinative 2.1.2.1. Swahili 2.1.2.1.1. nitakupenda 'I will like you' (literally ni 'I' ta 'future' ku 'second-person object', penda 'like'; 'I will you like'). 2.1.2.1.2. The parts which correspond to each concept or grammatical function are clearly discrete (albeit joined) from the other elements in the word. 2.1.3. Fusional 2.1.3.1. Spanish 2.1.3.1.1. hablo (I am speaking), habla (S/he is speaking), hable‰ (I spoke), but also hablamos (We are speaking), hablan (They are speaking).
    [Show full text]
  • Old, Middle, and Early Modern Morphology and Syntax Through Texts
    Old, Middle, and Early Modern Morphology and Syntax through Texts Elly van Gelderen 6 August 2016 1 Table of Contents List of Abbreviations Chapter 1 Introduction 1 The history of English in a nutshell 2 Functions and case 3 Verbal inflection and clause structure 4 Language change 5 Sources and resources 6 Conclusion Chapter 2 The Syntax of Old, Middle, and Early Modern English 1 Major Changes in the Syntax of English 2 Word Order 3 Inflections 4 Demonstratives, pronouns, and articles 5 Clause boundaries 6 Dialects in English 7 Conclusion Chapter 3 Old English Before 1100 1 The script 2 Historical prose narrative: Orosius 2 3 Sermon: Wulfstan on the Antichrist 4 Biblical gloss and translation: Lindisfarne, Rushworth, and Corpus Versions 5 Poetic monologue: the Wife’s Lament and the Wanderer 6 Conclusion Chapter 4 Early Middle English 1100‐1300 1 Historical prose narrative: Peterborough Chronicle 2 Prose Legend: Seinte Katerine 3 Debate poetry: The Owl and the Nightingale 4 Historical didactic poetry: Physiologus (Bestiary) 5 Prose: Richard Rolle’s Psalter Preface 6 Conclusion Chapter 5 Late Middle English and Early Modern 1300‐1600 1 Didactic poem: Cleanness 2 Instructional scientific prose: Chaucer’s Astrolabe 3 Religious: Margery of Kempe 4 Romance: Caxton 5 Chronicle and letters: Henry Machyn and Queen Elizabeth 6 Conclusion Chapter 6 Conclusion Appendix I Summary of all grammatical information 3 Appendix II Background on texts Appendix III Keys to the exercises Bibliography Glossary 4 Preface This book examines linguistic characteristics of English texts by studying manuscript images. I divide English into Old English, before 1100 but excluding runic inscriptions; Early Middle English, between 1100 and 1300; Late Middle English, between 1300 and 1500; and Early Modern English, after 1500.
    [Show full text]
  • Bachelor Thesis
    2007:085 BACHELOR THESIS A comparison of the morphological typology of Swedish and English noun phrases Emma Andersson Palola Luleå University of Technology Bachelor thesis English Department of Language and Culture 2007:085 - ISSN: 1402-1773 - ISRN: LTU-CUPP--07/085--SE A comparison of the morphological typology of Swedish and English noun phrases Emma Andersson Palola English 3 for Teachers Supervisor: Marie Nordlund Abstract The purpose of this study has been to examine the similarities and differences of the morphological typology of English and Swedish noun phrases. The principal aim has been to analyse the synthetic and analytic properties that English and Swedish possess and compare them to each other. The secondary aim has been to analyse and compare their agglutinative and fusional properties. According to their predominant characteristics, the languages have been positioned on the Index of Synthesis and the Index of Fusion respectively. The results show that English is mainly an analytic language, while Swedish is mainly a synthetic language. English also shows predominantly agglutinating characteristics, while Swedish shows mainly fusional characteristics. Table of contents 1. Introduction ............................................................................................................................ 1 1.1. Aim.................................................................................................................................. 2 1.2. Method and material........................................................................................................2
    [Show full text]
  • English Abstracts
    English abstracts Andreu Bauçà i Sastre The Diathesis: Concept, form and uses in the Romance languages The characterisation of Proto-Indo-European as a synthetic language,wherethe semantic features of the performance of the process within the same agent/causer (or at least in a background narrowly related to it) is morphologically materi- alised by the use of various verbal affixes, clearly distinguishable, enhances the idea that the voice (sic), even in the mother of all modern Romance languages, was a fully outstanding concept: active (in the cases of an outer agent together with a ∅ morpheme) and middle (in the opposite case and with a –r morpheme) (Benveniste 1966). Its further evolution, from Latin to the different Romances, of a set of varied features mainly analytic-based will cause the question about the current rel- evance/importance of the voice as a grammatical category to arise: is it still possible to treat about voices? And if it is, how could it be (re)defined and with which uses and forms should it be manifested? Keywords: Diathesis, voice, verbal category, active, passive, middle, ergative, imper- sonal. Francesc González i Planas Syntactic behaviour of the pronominal clitics in Asturian- Leonese This paper is placed in the framework of the comparative grammar of Ro- mance languages, being its aim to describe the placing of pronominal clitics in Asturian-Leonese in contrast to standard Galician and European Portuguese. The description of the performance of clitics in these languages has important consequences for theoretical grammar, since it proves the validity of the theory of left periphery developed by Rizzi (1997), which allows us to consider preverbal subjects (non-quantified and non-focalised) as left dislocations (Barbosa 2000).
    [Show full text]