(Part 2) Some Affixes Do Not Change the Grammatical Category

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

(Part 2) Some Affixes Do Not Change the Grammatical Category LING 220 LECTURE #11 MORPHOLOGY (Part 2) Some affixes do not change the grammatical category: in-competent ADJ. un-happy ADJ. re-consider VERB foot-age NOUN There are some accidental gaps in the lexicon, for example: ideal-ist but: *idea-ian (see grammar-ian) *idea-ist (see Marx-ist) COMPLEX DERIVATIONS “authorizational” A N V N Af Af Af | | | | author ize ation al Study Figure 4.6 on p. 133 “unhappiness” -- there are two ways to analyze this word: N A Af A Af | | | un happy ness * N N Af A Af | | | un happy ness The prefix un- combines with adjectives! un- + A unable, unwise un- + N *unknowledge, *unproduction (exception: uncola) Study Table 4.14 on p. 134 3. COMPOUNDING: the combination of lexical categories (Noun, Verb, Adjective, or Preposition) to form a larger word. When the lexical categories are the same, the compound will have the same category, for example: mailman (N + N → N). When the members of the compound are of different categories, the class of the final word will determine the category of the compound, for example: dryclean (A + V → V) When there is a Preposition in the compound, the category of the other member will determine the category of the compound, for example: uplift (P + V → V). Formalization: Study Figures 4.9, 4.10 and 4.11 on pp. 136-137 The morpheme that determines the category of the compound is called HEAD. Example: dryclean ↓ clean (V) : HEAD STRESS PATTERN: the first member of the compound has the primary stress. Compare: bláckboàrd bláck boàrd ‘a board that is black’ MEANING OF COMPOUNDS Sometimes the meaning of compounds cannot be predicted, for example: blackboard may be green, white, etc. On the basis of their MEANING, compounds are of two types: a. ENDOCENTRIC: the rightmost member (HEAD) identifies the general class to which the meaning of the entire class belongs. steamboat (a type of boat) catfood (a type of food) b. EXOCENTRIC: the meaning does not follow from the meaning of its parts. redneck (not a type of neck) Sometimes the difference between these two types of English compounds can be seen in the Plural forms: maple leaves (a type of leaf: Endocentric compound) Maple Leafs (members of Toronto’s hockey team: Exocentric compound) COMPOUNDS IN OTHER LANGUAGES Mandarin: daxiao ‘size’ ‘big’ ‘small’ Finnish: maailma ‘world’ ‘earth’ ‘sky’ ylioppilastutkintolautakunta (5 words) ‘university examination committee’ Hungarian: novér ‘sister’ ‘woman’ ‘blood’ 4. OTHER TYPES OF WORD FORMATION a. CONVERSION: a process that creates a new word by assigning a word to a new syntactic category (= zero derivation). NOUN → VERB bottle bottle (something) father father (a child) VERB → NOUN condúct cónduct call call (a phone call) PREPOSITION → VERB up up (they up the prices) Study Table 4.22 on p. 141. b. CLIPPING: a word of more than one syllable is reduced to a shorter form. advertisement → ad influenza → flu c. BLENDS: the creation of words from non-morphemic parts of two already existing words. smoke and fog → smog breakfast and lunch → brunch d. BACKFORMATION: a process that creates a new word by removing a real or supposed affix from another word. option → opt enthusiasm → enthuse pease → pea (the final [z] was incorrectly interpreted as the Plural suffix). e. ACRONYMS: words are formed by taking the initial letters of (some or all) of a set of other words. self-contained underwater breathing apparatus → scuba radio detecting and ranging → radar MADD, AIDS etc. The combined initial letters follow the phonotactics of English: if they do not, each letter is sounded out separately, e.g., UBC, SFU. f. COINAGE: words are created from scratch, for example: Kodak, Teflon,etc. g. WORDS FROM NAMES: kleenex, xerox, boycott, cardigan, etc. INFLECTIONAL MORPHOLOGY INFLECTION is a morphological process that modifies the form of a word to indicate the grammatical subclass to which it belongs. dogs: Plural subclass of nouns played: Past Tense subclass of verbs INFLECTION VERSUS DERIVATION 1. Inflection does not change the grammatical category of a word or the meaning of the word to which it applies. N V N Af V Af | | | | window s play ed Compare with the following derivations: A N N Af N Af | | | | nation al king dom (new meaning!) 2. A derivational affix is closer to the root: hunters : -er (deriv. affix), -s (inflect. affix) kingdom: *kingsdom 3. Inflectional affixes have relatively more freedom with which they can combine with words: derivational affixes have some restrictions. dogs, clocks, radios etc. but: hospitalize *clinicize terrorize *horrorize In English there are 8 inflectional affixes: Study Table 4.25 on p. 144 CASE: a category that encodes information about the grammatical role of the morpheme (subject, direct object, location etc.). In Modern English these functions are expressed largely with word order and the use of prepositions. Turkish : Table 4.29 on p. 149. Finnish: 15 cases Rumanian: 2 cases Hungarian: 29 cases.
Recommended publications
  • Analyzing Words with Roots and Affixes Essential Question: How Can I Determine the Meaning of a Word Using Its Roots And/Or Affixes? TEK
    Analyzing Words with Roots and Affixes Essential Question: How can I determine the meaning of a word using its roots and/or affixes? TEK (2) Reading/Vocabulary Development. Students understand new vocabulary and use it when reading and writing. Students are expected to: (A) determine the meaning of grade-level academic English words derived from Latin, Greek, or other linguistic roots and affixes Vocabulary Affix – word part Root - word part added either to that gives the the beginning or word its primary end of a root (or meaning base word) to change its root meaning Vocabulary root prefix suffix Vocabulary incredible cred in- -ible not + believe + able to Vocabulary biography graph bio- -y life + write + condition of Root & Affix Flipbooks O Use the lists of prefixes, roots, and suffixes on the slides that follow to create vocabulary flip books. We will be using our flip books to practice analyzing and building different words. O On the front side of the card (hole in the top left-hand corner), write the root or affix. Try to add a picture or graphic, if can think of one, to help you remember the meaning. O On the back side of the card, write the definition of the word part, and at least two examples within-a-word. On my website, go to “STAAR Review Centers” on the right-side. Prefixes Root, Prefix or Meaning Examples Suffix antisocial, antiseptic, antithesis, antibody, anti, ant against, opposite antinomies, antifreeze, antipathy from, down, away, to do detach, deploy, derange, decrease, deodorize, de- the opposite, reverse, devoid, deflate, degenerate against out of, away from, exit, exhale, exclusive, exceed, explosion, ex- ex- lacking, former mayor exter-, extra-, external, extrinsic, extraordinary, extrapolate, outside of, beyond extro- extraneous, extrovert in, im into, on, near, towards instead, import illegible, irresolute, inaction, inviolate, in, im, il, ir not innocuous, intractable, innocent, impregnable, impossible, imposter Notice that some affixes have multiple meanings.
    [Show full text]
  • Download Download
    The Genesis of Michif: A First Hypothesis PETER BAKKER University of Amsterdam Michif as a Mixed Language Since Richard Rhodes read the first paper on Michif at the Eighth Algon­ quian Conference in 1977, much work has been done on the language. A number of additional papers have been read at the Algonquian Conference (Weaver 1983; Rhodes 1985, 1987; Papen 1987) and others have been pub­ lished elsewhere (see Bakker 1989c for a bibliography). Most of this work focussed either on description or on the problem of genetic classification that this language poses. Michif remains an unusual language, with its verb phrase from Cree and its noun phrase from French. Does the language have two half grammars, and two phonological systems? Is it a Romance or an Algonquian language, or a Creole, a pidgin, a mixed language, or whatever? These are the questions that people have tried to answer. It is now probably agreed that Michif is a mixed language. It is a convincing case, even for those linguists who maintain that mixed languages do not exist. Its mixture is also unique: Michif has (Plains) Cree verbs and French nouns. No other language has a similar distribution of elements from two different languages. Nobody thus far has attempted to explain its genesis, although this is one of the more intriguing problems this language poses. In this paper1 I will present a preliminary answer to this question. The problem can be phrased as consisting of two sub-questions: first, why is Michif a mixed language? Second, why is the language mixed in this particular way? To Fie dwork on Cree and Michif were made possible with a Canadian Studies Graduate Student Award in 1987-1988.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Latin Nouns 1. Noun Entries – Chapter 3, LFCA Example
    Session A3: Introduction to Latin Nouns 1. Noun entries – Chapter 3, LFCA When a Latin noun is listed in a dictionary it provides three pieces of information: The nominative singular, the genitive singular, and the gender. The first form, called nominative (from Latin nömen, name) is the means used to list, or name, words in a dictionary. The second form, the genitive (from Latin genus, origin, kind or family), is used to find the stem of the noun and to determine the declension, or noun family to which it belongs. To find the stem of a noun, simply look at the genitive singular form and remove the ending –ae. The final abbreviation is a reference to the noun’s gender, since it is not always evident by the noun’s endings. Example: fëmina, fëminae, f. woman stem = fëmin/ae 2. Declensions – Chapters 3 – 10, LFCA Just as verbs are divided up into families or groups called conjugations, so also nouns are divided up into groups that share similar characteristics and behavior patterns. A declension is a group of nouns that share a common set of inflected endings, which we call case endings (more on case later). The genitive reveals the declension or family of nouns from which a word originates. Just as the infinitive is different for each conjugation, the genitive singular is unique to each declension. 1 st declension mënsa, mënsae 2 nd declension lüdus, lüdï ager, agrï dönum, dönï 3 rd declension vöx, vöcis nübës, nübis corpus, corporis 4 th declension adventus, adventüs cornü, cornüs 5 th declension fidës, fideï Practice: 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • Types and Functions of Reduplication in Palembang
    Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society JSEALS 12.1 (2019): 113-142 ISSN: 1836-6821, DOI: http://hdl.handle.net/10524/52447 University of Hawaiʼi Press TYPES AND FUNCTIONS OF REDUPLICATION IN PALEMBANG Mardheya Alsamadani & Samar Taibah Wayne State University [email protected] & [email protected] Abstract In this paper, we study the morphosemantic aspects of reduplication in Palembang (also known as Musi). In Palembang, both content and function words undergo reduplication, generating a wide variety of semantic functions, such as pluralization, iteration, distribution, and nominalization. Productive reduplication includes full reduplication and reduplication plus affixation, while fossilized reduplication includes partial reduplication and rhyming reduplication. We employed the Distributed Morphology theory (DM) (Halle and Marantz 1993, 1994) to account for these different patterns of reduplication. Moreover, we compared the functions of Palembang reduplication to those of Malay and Indonesian reduplication. Some instances of function word reduplication in Palembang were not found in these languages, such as reduplication of question words and reduplication of negators. In addition, Palembang partial reduplication is fossilized, with only a few examples collected. In contrast, Malay partial reduplication is productive and utilized to create new words, especially words borrowed from English (Ahmad 2005). Keywords: Reduplication, affixation, Palembang/Musi, morphosemantics ISO 639-3 codes: mui 1 Introduction This paper has three purposes. The first is to document the reduplication patterns found in Palembang based on the data collected from three Palembang native speakers. Second, we aim to illustrate some shared features of Palembang reduplication with those found in other Malayic languages such as Indonesian and Malay. The third purpose is to provide a formal analysis of Palembang reduplication based on the Distributed Morphology Theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Adjective in Old English
    Adjective in Old English Adjective in Old English had five grammatical categories: three dependent grammatical categories, i.e forms of agreement of the adjective with the noun it modified – number, gender and case; definiteness – indefiniteness and degrees of comparison. Adjectives had three genders and two numbers. The category of case in adjectives differed from that of nouns: in addition to the four cases of nouns they had one more case, Instrumental. It was used when the adjective served as an attribute to a noun in the Dat. case expressing an instrumental meaning. Weak and Strong Declension Most adjectives in OE could be declined in two ways: according to the weak and to the strong declension. The formal differences between the declensions, as well as their origin, were similar to those of the noun declensions. The strong and weak declensions arose due to the use of several stem-forming suffixes in PG: vocalic a-, o-, u- and i- and consonantal n-. Accordingly, there developed sets of endings of the strong declension mainly coinciding with the endings of a-stems of nouns for adjectives in the Masc. and Neut. and of o-stems – in the Fem. Some endings in the strong declension of adjectives have no parallels in the noun paradigms; they are similar to the endings of pronouns: -um for Dat. sg, -ne for Acc. Sg Masc., [r] in some Fem. and pl endings. Therefore the strong declension of adjectives is sometimes called the ‘pronominal’ declension. As for the weak declension, it uses the same markers as n-stems of nouns except that in the Gen.
    [Show full text]
  • Animacy Encoding in English: Why and How
    Animacy Encoding in English: why and how Annie Zaenen Jean Carletta Gregory Garretson PARC & Stanford University HCRC-University of Edinburgh Boston University 3333 Coyote Hill Road 2, Buccleuch Place Program in Applied Linguistics Palo Alto, CA 94304] Edinburgh EH8LW, UK 96 Cummington St., [email protected] [email protected] Boston, MA 02215 [email protected] Joan Bresnan Andrew Koontz-Garboden Tatiana Nikitina CSLI-Stanford University CSLI-Stanford University CSLI-Stanford University 220, Panama Street 220, Panama Street 220, Panama Street Stanford CA 94305 Stanford CA 94305 Stanford CA 94305 [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] M. Catherine O’Connor Tom Wasow Boston University CSLI-Stanford University Program in Applied Linguistics 220, Panama Street 96 Cummington St., Stanford CA 94305 Boston, MA 02215 [email protected] [email protected] Abstract of entity representation within language: the definiteness dimension is linked to the status of the We report on two recent medium-scale initiatives entity at a particular point in the discourse, the annotating present day English corpora for animacy person hierarchy depends on the participants distinctions. We discuss the relevance of animacy for within the discourse, and the animacy status is an computational linguistics, specifically generation, the annotation categories used in the two studies and the inherent characteristic of the entities referred to. interannotator reliability for one of the studies. Each of these aspects, however, orders entities on a scale that makes them more or less salient or 1 Introduction ‘accessible’ when humans use their language. It has long been known that animacy is an The importance of accessibility scales is not important category in syntactic and morphological widely recognized in computational treatments of natural language analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • 4.1 Inflection
    4.1 Inflection Within a lexeme-based theory of morphology, the difference between derivation and inflection is very simple. Derivation gives you new lexemes, and inflection gives you the forms of a lexeme that are determined by syntactic environment (cf. 2.1.2). But what exactly does this mean? Is there really a need for such a distinction? This section explores the answers to these questions, and in the process, goes deeper into the relation between morphology and syntax. 4.1.1 Inflection vs. derivation The first question we can ask about the distinction between inflection and derivation is whether there is any formal basis for distinguishing the two: can we tell them apart because they do different things to words? One generalization that has been made is that derivational affixes tend to occur closer to the root or stem than inflectional affixes. For example, (1) shows that the English third person singular present inflectional suffix -s occurs outside of derivational suffixes like the deadjectival -ize, and the plural ending -s follows derivational affixes including the deverbal -al: (1) a. popular-ize-s commercial-ize-s b. upheav-al-s arriv-al-s Similarly, Japanese derivational suffixes like passive -rare or causative -sase precede inflectional suffixes marking tense and aspect:1 (2) a. tabe-ru tabe-ta eat- IMP eat- PERF INFLECTION 113 ‘eats’ ‘ate’ b. tabe-rare- ru tabe-rare- ta eat - PASS-IMP eat- PASS-PERF ‘is eaten’ ‘was eaten’ c. tabe-sase- ru tabe-sase- ta eat- CAUS-IMP eat- CAUS-PERF ‘makes eat’ ‘made eat’ It is also the case that inflectional morphology does not change the meaning or grammatical category of the word that it applies to.
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistics 1A Morphology 2 Complex Words
    Linguistics 1A Morphology 2 Complex words In the previous lecture we noted that words can be classified into different categories, such as verbs, nouns, adjectives, prepositions, determiners, and so on. We can make another distinction between word types as well, a distinction that cuts across these categories. Consider the verbs, nouns and adjectives in (1)-(3), respectively. It will probably be intuitively clear that the words in the (b) examples are complex in a way that the words in the (a) examples are not, and not just because the words in the (b) examples are, on the whole, longer. (1) a. to walk, to dance, to laugh, to kiss b. to purify, to enlarge, to industrialize, to head-hunt (2) a. house, corner, zebra b. collection, builder, sea horse (3) c. green, old, sick d. regional, washable, honey-sweet The words in the (a) examples in (1)-(3) do not have any internal structure. It does not seem to make much sense to say that walk , for example, consists of the smaller parts wa and lk . But for the words in the (b) examples this is different. These are built up from smaller parts that each contribute their own distinct bit of meaning to the whole. For example, builder consists of the verbal part build with its associated meaning, and the part –er that contributes a ‘doer’ reading, just as it does in kill-er , sell-er , doubt-er , and so on. Similarly, washable consists of wash and a part –able that contributes a meaning aspect that might be described loosely as ‘can be done’, as it does in refundable , testable , verifiable etc.
    [Show full text]
  • The Formation of the Fifth Declension; Uses of the Ablative Case; and at the End of the Lesson We'll Review the Vocabulary Which You Should Memorize in This Chapter
    Chapter 22: Fifth Declension. Chapter 22 covers the following: the formation of the fifth declension; uses of the ablative case; and at the end of the lesson we'll review the vocabulary which you should memorize in this chapter. There is one important rule to remember in this chapter: fifth declension represents e-stem nouns which are for the most part feminine in gender. Fifth Declension. Hooray, hooray! Latin only has five declensions, so this is the last declension we'll study. Once you've mastered fifth declension, you've learned everything you need to know about Latin nouns in this class. Fifth declension consists of nouns characterized by -ē. This declension is a unique concoction which the Romans brewed up at one point in their history and which did not survive long. As Latin after classical antiquity began evolving into the various Romance languages, this e-stem category of nouns was conflated into other declensions and disappeared as a separate grammatical category. Here are the endings for fifth-declension nouns. Let's recite them together: -es, -ei, -ei, -em, -e; -es, -erum, -ebus, -es, -ebus. Note the dominance of -e- (often -ē-) which is, without doubt, the most significant feature of this declension, though the -ē- produces no mandatory long marks, meaning no forms are distinguished by macrons in this declension. A close look at the endings shows why it was easy for the post-classical Romans to subsume fifth declension into third, for instance. Fifth and third look a lot like each other. In fact, the majority of fifth-declension endings look like a long ē-stem with third-declension endings appended on to it.
    [Show full text]
  • Number Systems in Grammar Position Paper
    1 Language and Culture Research Centre: 2018 Workshop Number systems in grammar - position paper Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald I Introduction I 2 The meanings of nominal number 2 3 Special number distinctions in personal pronouns 8 4 Number on verbs 9 5 The realisation of number 12 5.1 The forms 12 5.2 The loci: where number is shown 12 5.3 Optional and obligatory number marking 14 5.4 The limits of number 15 5.4.1 Number and the meanings of nouns 15 5.4.2 'Minor' numbers 16 5.4.3 The limits of number: nouns with defective number values 16 6 Number and noun categorisation 17 7 Markedness 18 8 Split, or mixed, number systems 19 9 Number and social deixis 19 10 Expressing number through other means 20 11 Number systems in language history 20 12 Summary 21 Further readings 22 Abbreviations 23 References 23 1 Introduction Every language has some means of distinguishing reference to one individual from reference to more than one. Number reference can be coded through lexical modifiers (including quantifiers of various sorts or number words etc.), or through a grammatical system. Number is a referential property of an argument of the predicate. A grammatical system of number can be shown either • Overtly, on a noun, a pronoun, a verb, etc., directly referring to how many people or things are involved; or • Covertly, through agreement or other means. Number may be marked: • within an NP • on the head of an NP • by agreement process on a modifier (adjective, article, demonstrative, etc.) • through agreement on verbs, or special suppletive or semi-suppletive verb forms which may code the number of one or more verbal arguments, or additional marker on the verb.
    [Show full text]
  • Affix Ordering in Optimal Construction Morphology
    John Benjamins Publishing Company This is a contribution from Morphological Metatheory. Edited by Daniel Siddiqi and Heidi Harley. © 2016. John Benjamins Publishing Company This electronic file may not be altered in any way. The author(s) of this article is/are permitted to use this PDF file to generate printed copies to be used by way of offprints, for their personal use only. Permission is granted by the publishers to post this file on a closed server which is accessible to members (students and staff) only of the author’s/s’ institute, it is not permitted to post this PDF on the open internet. For any other use of this material prior written permission should be obtained from the publishers or through the Copyright Clearance Center (for USA: www.copyright.com). Please contact [email protected] or consult our website: www.benjamins.com Tables of Contents, abstracts and guidelines are available at www.benjamins.com Affix ordering in Optimal Construction Morphology Sharon Inkelas University of California, Berkeley This paper sketches an integrated approach to affix ordering within Optimal Construction Morphology, a bottom-up, competition based model of word production in which each step of affixation is the optimal choice among competing possibilities (Caballero & Inkelas 2013). Optimality-theoretic models are natural fits for affix ordering, a complex phenomenon governed by a mix of conflicting universal and language-specific factors which interact differently in every language. This study covers familiar, global cross-linguistic principles such as semantic relevance (e.g., Bybee 1985) and scope (e.g., Baker 1988; Rice 2000), integrating them with local lexical selectional restrictions (e.g., Fabb 1988); it also incorporates usage-based factors such as Complexity-Based Ordering (e.g., Hay & Plag 2004).
    [Show full text]