Valency-Augmented Dependency Parsing

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Valency-Augmented Dependency Parsing Valency-Augmented Dependency Parsing Tianze Shi Lillian Lee Cornell University Cornell University [email protected] [email protected] In Proceedings of the 2018 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing ccomp Abstract mark xcomp We present a complete, automated, and effi- nsubj nsubj mark cient approach for utilizing valency analysis in making dependency parsing decisions. It in- He says that you like to swim . cludes extraction of valency patterns, a proba- Figure 1: Sample annotation in UD, encoding the bilistic model for tagging these patterns, and core valency pattern nsubj ˛ ccomp for says, a joint decoding process that explicitly con- nsubj ˛ xcomp for like, and so on (see §2-4) siders the number and types of each token’s syntactic dependents. On 53 treebanks rep- resenting 41 languages in the Universal De- Fig. 1 for examples), and operationalize this no- pendencies data, we find that incorporating va- tion as extracted supertags. An important dis- lency information yields higher precision and F1 scores on the core arguments (subjects and tinction from prior work is that our definition of complements) and functional relations (e.g., valency-pattern supertags is relativized to a user- auxiliaries) that we employ for valency anal- specified subset of all possible syntactic relations ysis. Precision on core arguments improves (see §3). We train supertaggers that assign proba- from 80:87 to 85:43. We further show that bilities of potential valency patterns to each token, our approach can be applied to an ostensibly and leverages these probabilities during decoding different formalism and dataset, Tree Adjoin- to guide our parsers so that they favor more lin- ing Grammar as extracted from the Penn Tree- bank; there, we outperform the previous state- guistically plausible output structures. of-the-art labeled attachment score by 0:7. Fi- We mainly focus on two subsets of relations in nally, we explore the potential of extending va- our analysis, those involving core arguments and lency patterns beyond their traditional domain those that represent functional relations, and per- by confirming their helpfulness in improving form experiments over a collection of 53 treebanks 1 PP attachment decisions. in 41 languages from the Universal Dependencies 1 Introduction dataset (UD; Nivre et al., 2017). Our valency- aware parsers improve upon strong baseline sys- Many dependency parsers treat attachment deci- tems in terms of output linguistic validity, mea- sions and syntactic relation labeling as two in- sured as the accuracy of the assigned valency pat- dependent tasks, despite the fact that relation la- terns. They also have higher precision and F1 bels carry important subcategorization informa- scores on the subsets of relations under analysis, tion. For example, the number and types of the suggesting a potentially controlled way to balance syntactic arguments that a predicate may take is precision-recall trade-offs. rather restricted for natural languages — it is not We further show that our approach is not limited common for an English verb to have more than one to a particular treebank annotation style. We apply syntactic subject or more than two objects. our method to parsing another grammar formal- In this work, we present a parsing approach ism, Tree Adjoining Grammar, where dependency that explicitly models subcategorization of (some) and valency also play an important role in both syntactic dependents as valency patterns (see theory and parser evaluation. Our parser reaches 1Our implementation is available at https:// a new state-of-the-art LAS score of 92:59, with github.com/tzshi/valency-parser-emnlp18 more than 0:6 core-argument F1-score improve- ment over our strong baseline parser. Dataset Subset Syntactic Relations Finally, we demonstrate the applicability of our nsubj, obj, iobj, valency analysis approach to other syntactic phe- Core csubj, ccomp, xcomp nomena less associated with valency in its tradi- UD aux, cop, mark, tional linguistic sense. In a case study of PP at- Func. det, clf, case tachment, we analyze the patterns of two syntac- PP (§8) nmod, obl tic relations commonly used in PP attachment, and include them in the joint decoding process. Preci- 0 (subject), 1 (object), Core sion of the parsers improves by an absolute 3:30% TAG 2 (indirect object) on these two relation types. Co-head CO 2 Syntactic Dependencies and Valencies Table 1: Sets of syntactic relations we used for va- lency analysis. UD subsets come from the official According to Nivre (2005), the modern depen- categorization in the annotation guidelines. dency grammar can be traced back to Tesnière (1959), with its roots reaching back several cen- turies before the Common Era. The theory is cen- tered on the notion of dependency, an asymmet- a´j ¨ ¨ ¨ a´1 ˛ a1 ¨ ¨ ¨ ak: the ˛ symbol denotes the rical relation between words of a sentence. Tes- center word wi, and each al asserts the existence nière distinguishes three node types when analyz- of a word w dominated by wi via relation al P R, al ing simple predicates: verb equivalents that de- wi ÝÑ w. For al and am, when l ă m, the syn- scribe actions and events, noun equivalents as the tactic dependent for al linearly precedes the syn- arguments of the events, and adverb equivalents tactic dependent for am. As an example, consider for detailing the (temporal, spatial, etc.) circum- the UD-annotated sentence in Fig. 1. The token stances. There are two types of relations: (1) says has a core-relation3 valency pattern nsubj ˛ verbs dominate nouns and adverbs through a de- ccomp, and like has the pattern nsubj ˛ xcomp. pendency relation; (2) verbs and nouns are linked If we consider only functional relations, both like through a valency relation. Tesnière compares a and swim have the pattern mark ˛.4 We sometimes verb to an atom: a verb can attract a certain num- employ the abbreviated notation αL ˛αR, where α ber of arguments, just as the valency of an atom indicates a sequence and the letters L and R distin- determines the number of bonds it can engage in guish left dependencies from right dependencies. (Ágel and Fischer, 2015). In many descriptive We make our definition of valency patterns de- lexicographic works (Helbig and Schenkel, 1959; pendent on choice of R not only because some de- Herbst et al., 2004), valency is not limited to verbs, pendency relations are more often obligatory and but also includes nouns and adjectives. For more closer to the original theoretical definition of va- on the linguistic theory, see Ágel et al. (2003, lency, but also because the utility of different types 2006). of syntactic relations can depend on the down- Strictly following the original notion of va- stream task. For example, purely functional de- lency requires distinguishing between arguments pendency labels are semantically vacuous, so they and adjuncts, as well as obligatory and optional are often omitted in the semantic representations dependents. However, there is a lack of consen- extracted from dependency trees for question an- sus as to how these categorizations may be dis- swering (Reddy et al., 2016, 2017). There are also tinguished (Tutunjian and Boland, 2008), and thus recent proposals for parser evaluation that down- we adopt a more practical definition in this paper. play the importance of functional syntactic rela- 3 Computational Representation tions (Nivre and Fang, 2017). Formally, we fix a set of syntactic relations R, and define the valency pattern of a token wi with dependents. We choose to encode linearity since it appears respect to R as the linearly-ordered2 sequence that most languages empirically exhibit word order prefer- ences even if they allow for relatively free word order. 2Our approach, whose full description is in §5, can be 3UD core and functional relations are listed in Table 1. adapted to cases where linear ordering is de-emphasized. The 4The (possibly counterintuitive) direction for that and to algorithm merely requires a distinction between left and right is a result of UD’s choice of a content-word-oriented design. 4 Pilot Study: Sanity Checks system outputs and the number of those not ob- served in training.7 All 5 systems are remarkably We consider two questions that need to be ad- “hallucinatory” in inventing valency relations, in- dressed at the outset:5 troducing 16:8 to 35:5 new valency patterns, sig- 1. How well do the extracted patterns generalize nificantly larger than the actual number of unseen to unseen data? patterns. Below we show an error committed by the state-of-the-art Dozat et al. (2017) parser (up- 2. Do state-of-the-art parsers already capture per half) as compared to the gold-standard annota- the notion of valency implicitly, though they tion (lower half), and we highlight the core argu- are not explicitly optimized for it? ment valency relations of the verb bothers in bold. The system incorrectly predicts how come to be a The first question checks the feasibility of learning clausal subject. valency patterns from a limited amount of data; csubj ccomp the second probes the potential for any valency- advmod nsubj informed parsing approach to improve over cur- rent state-of-the-art systems. How come no one bothers to ask ... To answer these questions, we use the UD 2.0 fixed nsubj dataset for the CoNLL 2017 shared task (Zeman advmod xcomp et al., 2017) and the system outputs6 of the top Each such non-existent new pattern implies at five performing submissions (Dozat et al., 2017; least some (potentially small) parsing error that Shi et al., 2017b; Björkelund et al., 2017; Che can contribute to the degradation of downstream et al., 2017; Lim and Poibeau, 2017). Selection task performance. of treebanks is the same as in §6.
Recommended publications
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • Causative in Sasak: a Mechanism of Valency Increase
    ISSN 1798-4769 Journal of Language Teaching and Research, Vol. 11, No. 3, pp. 491-496, May 2020 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.17507/jltr.1103.19 Causative in Sasak: A Mechanism of Valency Increase Nurul Azizah Udayana University, Bali, Indonesia Ketut Artawa Udayana University, Bali, Indonesia Ni Luh Ketut Mas Indrawati Udayana University, Bali, Indonesia Made Sri Satyawati Udayana University, Bali, Indonesia Abstract—This study discusses the formation of causatives based on the criteria proposed by Dixon (2012). Pertain to the mechanism of valency increasing, it is found that causative construction in Sasak Kuto-Kute dialect covers two forms: morphological and lexical. Morphological causative involves the process of adding confix N -ang/in and affix -in to the non-causative base verb, whilst, the lexical causative uses the causative verb mate 'died/killed'. Furthermore, it was found that the formation of causative from intransitive verbs can be done by moving the original S to O position in the causative construction, which is morphologically marked on the verb, and also using different verbs with the same meaning to form the causative construction which is followed by the transfer of the original S to O position. For transitive verbs, the formation can be done through the use of affixes with the original A and original O both becoming O1 and O2. Last, the causative construction in the dialect semantically involves five semantic parameters, namely transparency, naturalness, intention, involvement, and control. Index Terms—Sasak, causative, valency increase I. Introduction Causative is known as a way that can be used to add an argument on sentence structures (Dixon and Aikhenvald, 2000, p.
    [Show full text]
  • Flexible Valency in Chintang.∗
    Flexible valency in Chintang.∗ Robert Schikowskia , Netra Paudyalb , and Balthasar Bickela a University of Zürich b University of Leipzig 1 e Chintang language Chintang [ˈts̻ ʰiɳʈaŋ]̻ (ISO639.3: ctn) is a Sino-Tibetan language of Nepal. It is named aer the village where it is mainly spoken. e village lies in the hills of Eastern Nepal, bigger cities within day’s reach being Dhankuṭā and Dharān. ere are no official data on the number of speakers, but we estimate there to be around 4,000 - 5,000 speakers. Most speakers are bi- or trilingual, with Nepali (the Indo-Aryan lingua franca of Nepal) as one and Bantawa (a related Sino-Tibetan language) as the other additional language. Monolingual speakers are still to be found mainly among elderly women, whereas a considerable portion of the younger generation is rapidly shiing to Nepali. Genealogically, Chintang belongs to the Kiranti group. e Kiranti languages are generally accepted to belong to the large Sino-Tibetan (or Tibeto-Burman) family, although their position within this family is controversial (cf. e.g. urgood 2003, Ebert 2003). Based on phonological evidence, Chintang belongs to the Eastern subgroup of Kiranti (Bickel et al. 2010). ere are two major dialects (Mulgaũ and Sambugaũ ) named aer the areas where they are spoken. e differences between them concern morphology and the lexicon but, as far as we know, not syntax, and so we will not distinguish between dialects in this chapter. For all examples the source has been marked behind the translation. Wherever possible, we take data from the Chintang Language Corpus (Bickel et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Valency Patterns Topics in English Linguistics 71
    Verb Valency Patterns Topics in English Linguistics 71 Editors Bernd Kortmann Elizabeth Closs Traugott De Gruyter Mouton Verb Valency Patterns A Challenge for Semantics-Based Accounts by Susen Faulhaber De Gruyter Mouton ISBN 978-3-11-024071-9 e-ISBN 978-3-11-024078-8 ISSN 1434-3452 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Faulhaber, Susen, 1978Ϫ Verb valency patterns : a challenge for semantics-based accounts / by Susen Faulhaber. p. cm. Ϫ (Topics in English linguistics ; 71) Originally presented as the author’s thesis (doctoral Ϫ University of Erlangen and Nuremberg, Germany, 2009) under the title: Semantic aspects of verb valency. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-3-11-024071-9 (alk. paper) 1. Grammar, Comparative and general Ϫ Verb phrase. 2. Seman- tics. 3. Dependency grammar. I. Title. P281.F38 2011 4251.6Ϫdc22 2011009773 Bibliographic information published by the Deutsche Nationalbibliothek The Deutsche Nationalbibliothek lists this publication in the Deutsche Nationalbibliografie; detailed bibliographic data are available in the Internet at http://dnb.d-nb.de. ” 2011 Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co. KG, 10785 Berlin/New York Cover image: Brian Stablyk/Photographer’s Choice RF/Getty Images Printing: Hubert & Co. GmbH & Co. KG, Göttingen ϱ Printed on acid-free paper Printed in Germany www.degruyter.com Preface This book, which is a revised version of my doctoral thesis, submitted to and accepted by the Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg in 2009 under the title Semantic Aspects of Verb Valency – The Relationship between Meaning and Form, would not have been possible without the help of so many people who supported me in various ways.
    [Show full text]
  • VALENCY SENTENCE PATTERNS and MEANING INTERPRETATION - Case Study of the Verb CONSIDER –
    VALENCY SENTENCE PATTERNS AND MEANING INTERPRETATION - Case study of the verb CONSIDER – by Renate Reichardt A thesis submitted to the for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of English School of English, Drama and American & Canadian Studies The University of Birmingham October 2013 University of Birmingham Research Archive e-theses repository This unpublished thesis/dissertation is copyright of the author and/or third parties. The intellectual property rights of the author or third parties in respect of this work are as defined by The Copyright Designs and Patents Act 1988 or as modified by any successor legislation. Any use made of information contained in this thesis/dissertation must be in accordance with that legislation and must be properly acknowledged. Further distribution or reproduction in any format is prohibited without the permission of the copyright holder. Dedicated to Marie and Otto Reichardt Page ii ABSTRACT This thesis explores the interrelationship of local grammar, meaning, and translation equivalence, using a case study of the English verb CONSIDER, compared in a monolingual study with its near-synonyms BELIEVE, FEEL and THINK, and in a contrastive analysis with their German translation equivalents. The methodology fuses corpus linguistics and valency grammar, analysing and comparing monolingual and parallel corpora. Corpus investigation is found to be a reliable tool in identifying key translation equivalents and in verifying sentence patterns. Valency theory is argued to be more successful than related approaches in distinguishing between different levels of language analysis. Its flexibility regarding complement categorisation types make it possible to define categories that can be applied to both German and English appropriately in a contrastive study, in spite of the surface differences between the two languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Valency and Transitivity in a Contact Variety: the Evidence from Cameroon Pidgin English
    journal of language contact 12 (2019) 52-88 brill.com/jlc Valency and Transitivity in a Contact Variety: The Evidence from Cameroon Pidgin English Melanie Green University of Sussex [email protected] Gabriel Ozón University of Sheffield [email protected] Abstract We explore valency and transitivity patterns in Cameroon Pidgin English (cpe) from a language contact perspective, with particular focus on (a) lexical and (b) construc- tional phenomena. With respect to (a), many verbs of English origin surface in cpe with additional senses and valency properties to those they display in the lexifier, il- lustrating the drive towards polysemy in a language with a relatively small lexicon. We also describe category change, whereby English non-verbal expressions (typically adjectives) emerge as verbs in cpe. In terms of (b), verbs undergo valency changes as a consequence of participation in productive serial verb constructions. These construc- tions are built around a small set of high-frequency verbs, some of which also occur in the light verb construction, which represents another strategy for the creation of complex predicates. We review the evidence for constructional substrate influence. The data under discussion are drawn from two small corpora of spoken cpe. Keywords valency – transitivity – serial verb – light verb – Cameroon Pidgin English – corpus linguistics © melanie green and gabriel ozón, 2019 | doi 10.1163/19552629-01201003 This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the prevailing CC-BY-NC License at the time of publication. Downloaded from Brill.com10/07/2021 10:58:02AM via free access Valency and transitivity in a contact variety 53 1 Introduction Cameroon Pidgin English (cpe) is an Atlantic expanded pidgin/creole spoken in some form by an estimated 50% of Cameroon’s 22,000,000 population, pri- marily in the Anglophone west regions, but also in urban centres throughout the country (Lewis et al., 2014).
    [Show full text]
  • Appendix A: Some Verbs and Their Valencies
    Appendix A: Some Verbs and Their Valencies In this Appendix I give a small sample of the entries that compose a valency dictionary of Portuguese verbs. Notation Used in the List The diatheses are indicated in full, that is, after the eventual application of linking rules and coding of semantic roles by default. Thus, the list is neutral with respect to several hypotheses advanced in this book; for instance, the information that the VSubj of eat is an Agent does not depend on accepting the Agent<>VSubj rule. On the other hand, the notation does depend on the syntactic analysis proposed in Chap. 2, and on the delimitation of semantic roles as developed in Chaps. 3–9;I hope the syntactic and semantic analysis presented in this list may be acceptable to most linguists. Eventually, as explained previously, assignments that depend on linking rules will be marked with [LR], on transparency with [Transp], and on default assignment with [byD]. Here (as in the current Valency Dictionary) these marks are not given, since they still depend on some research. It was seen in Chap. 9 that in some cases we need role-coding by direct connection with the schema, which results in highly elaborate CSRs being attached to sentence constituents; one example is he rid the house of insects, where the VSubj is Agent, the object is Patient, and the of-complement is the thing.the.ridder. rids.something.of. In the valency list these elaborate CSRs are written without capitals; thus, one diathesis of livrar ‘rid’ will be VSubj>Agent VNP>Patient de NP>thing.the.ridder.rids.something.of © Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015 227 M.A.
    [Show full text]
  • CREEK VOICE: BEYOND VALENCY Jack B. Martin Within Chemistry
    CREEK VOICE: BEYOND VALENCY Jack B. Martin Within chemistry, VALENCY refers to the capacity of an atom or group of atoms to combine in specific proportions with other atoms or groups of atoms.1 The French linguist Lucien Tesnière is generally credited with introducing this term to linguistics, where it is used metaphorically for the capacity of a verb to combine with distinct arguments or valents (Crystal 1985). A verb like rain, which has no referential noun phrases associated with it, is said to be ZERO-PLACE or AVALENT; a verb like disappear, which takes only a subject argument, is said to be ONE-PLACE or MONOVALENT; verbs like devour and give are said to be TWO-PLACE (BIVALENT) and THREE-PLACE (TRIVALENT), respectively. This chemical metaphor has had a pervasive influence in linguistics: causative and applicative morphemes are now described as ‘adding arguments,’ while passives and middles are described as ‘suppressing’ or ‘deleting’ arguments, respectively. Entire sections of grammars are devoted to ‘valency-changing,’ ‘valency-increasing,’ or ‘valency-reducing’ processes, suggesting that the primary function of these grammatical processes is to regulate the number of arguments in clauses. The chemical metaphor contrasts with an older tradition that distinguishes just two classes of predicates—TRANSITIVE and INTRANSITIVE—and a category of VOICE. Passive voice and middle voice are seen within this tradition as altering the ‘point of view’ or ‘centre of interest’ (Jesperson 1924:167) within a clause rather than applying mathematical operations to it, and causatives and applicatives are sometimes included and sometimes excluded from the traditional range of voice-related phenomena.
    [Show full text]
  • The Influence of Animacy and Context on Word Order Processing: Neurophysiological Evidence from Mandarin Chinese
    Impressum Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 2011 Diese Arbeit ist unter folgender Creative Commons-Lizenz lizenziert: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0 Druck: Sächsisches Druck- und Verlagshaus Direct World, Dresden ISBN 978-3-941504-13-4 The Influence of Animacy and Context on Word Order Processing: Neurophysiological Evidence from Mandarin Chinese Von der Philologischen Fakultät der Universität Leipzig genehmigte DISSERTATION Zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades doctor philosophiae Dr. Phil. vorgelegt von Luming Wang geboren am 16. Februar 1981 in Zhoushan, China Dekan: Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Lörscher Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Balthasar Bickel Prof. Dr. Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky Prof. Dr. Kaoru Horie For my mother tongue, one of the many languages in this world. Acknowledgements This thesis could not have been written without the support and friendship of many people. If life is like online sentence processing in the sense that one must make a decision even without being sure of where it will lead, those people are definitely “prominent” characters that have greatly influenced my decisions, especially when I was experiencing “ambiguities” during different periods. I would like to thank them in chronological order. I thank my parents for giving me the initial processing preference, which has driven me to become closer to those things and people that I like, but unexpectedly lead me farther from them. I thank them from the bottom of my heart for their continuing selfless love. I am lucky for having been a student of Prof. Kaoru Horie and Prashant Pardesh during my MA studies at Tohoku University in Japan.
    [Show full text]
  • CAUSATIVE CONJUGATION Lntroduction the Grammatical Category "Causative" Is Semantically Definable As a "Macrosituation" Consisting of Two "Microsituations"
    CHAPTER FOUR THE SEMITIC CAUSATIVE CONJUGATION lntroduction The grammatical category "causative" is semantically definable as a "macrosituation" consisting of two "microsituations". A microsituation consists of two constants: thema and rhema or subject and predicate. When two such microsituations are combined so that one is considered the cause of the other, we speak of a causative macrosituation.' It is obvious that there are many ways of realizing this semantic structure on the expression Ievel of a language. We are here concerned with one of these devices, viz. the causative verb. Such a verb represents two elements of the causative macrosituation: the predicates of both the causing and the caused microsituations. The causative macrosituation Iooks like this: the fiddler cause the girl dance s p S* P* A characteristic of the situation underlying the causative verb is that the predicate of the causing situation is empty of all semantic content except the feature causativity. 2 In transformational terms, the surface structure can be described as the result of predicate raising of P* and objectivization of S *. the fiddler cause-dance the girl In many languages, predicate raising does not usually occur. Instead P is realized as a separate verb 'to cause'. This is also found in Semitic although we mostly have causative verbs including both P and P*. 3 Syntactically such verbs are characterized by valency increasing: a monovalent verb ( = intransitive) is made bivalent by the addition of one actant, viz. the causator ( = S), while the former first actant (S*) is moved 1 For the following analysis see especially Nedyalkov/Silnitzky, 1 f.
    [Show full text]
  • Changing Valency Case Studies in Transitivity
    Changing valency Case studies in transitivity edited by R. M. W. DIXON and ALEXANDRA Y. AIKHENVALD Research Centre for Linguistic Typology Le Trobe University, Melbourne published by the press syndicate of the university of cambridge The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom cambridge university press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http://www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York NY 10011–4211, USA http://www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia © Cambridge University Press 2000 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2000 Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge Typeset in 10/13 Times [gc] A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress cataloguing in publication data Changing valency: case studies in transitivity / edited by R. M. W. Dixon and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0 521 66039 4 (hardback) 1. Grammar, Comparative and general – Verb. 2. Grammar, Comparative and general – Transitivity. 3. Grammar, Comparative and general – Voice. 4. Typology (Linguistics). 5. Dependency grammar. I. Dixon, Robert M.W. II. ASkhenval’d, A. |. (Aleksandra |r’evna) P281.C48 2000 415 – dc21 99–24413 CIP ISBN 0 521 66039 4 hardback Contents List of contributors page xi Preface xiii List of abbreviations xiv 1 Introduction by R.M.W. Dixon and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald 1 1 Predicate arguments and clause types 2 2 Verb classes 4 3 Changing valency 6 4 Valency reduction 7 5 Valency increase 12 6 An integrated approach 16 7 Semantic classes of verbs 19 8 Alternative construction types 21 9 The studies in this volume 22 10 Topics for further investigation 25 References 28 2 A typology of causatives: form, syntax and meaning by R.M.W.
    [Show full text]
  • Valency Change and Complex Predicates in Wolof: an Lfg Account
    VALENCY CHANGE AND COMPLEX PREDICATES IN WOLOF: AN LFG ACCOUNT Cheikh Bamba Dione University of Bergen Proceedings of the LFG13 Conference Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King (Editors) 2013 CSLI Publications http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/ Abstract This paper presents an LFG-based analysis of Wolof valency-changing suffixes found in applicative and causative constructions. The analysis ad- dresses the particular issue of applicative-causative polysemy in this lan- guage. Similar to the work for Indonesian (Arka et al., 2009), I adopt an LFG-based predicate composition approach of complex predicate formation (Alsina, 1996; Butt, 1995), and extend it to handle the Wolof data. However, the present work does not propose a unified argument-structure to handle applicative-causative polysemy. It rather postulates an a-structure for each derivation (applicative and causative) by analyzing polysemous suffixes as carrying their own PRED(ICATE) argument structure which they share with other suffixes of the same derivation type. The proposed analysis is integrated into an implementation of an existing computational grammar using the XLE grammar development environment. The relevant system components include a finite-state morphology, annotated phrase structure and sub-lexical rules. The implementation makes use of the restriction operator (Kaplan and Wedekind, 1993; Butt et al., 2003). 1 Introduction This paper presents an analysis of Wolof applicative and causative suffixes (Com- rie, 1985; Voisin-Nouguier, 2002, 2006) within the Lexical-Functional Grammar (LFG) framework (Kaplan and Bresnan, 1982). In Wolof, valency changes may be encoded by different suffixes, and the same suffix (e.g. -al and -e) may encode different valency changes, giving rise to applicative-causative polysemy (1-4).
    [Show full text]