The Neural and Cognitive Foundations of Human Planning

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Neural and Cognitive Foundations of Human Planning The Neural and Cognitive Foundations of Human Planning Inaugural-Dissertation zur Erlangung des Doktorgrades der Wirtschafts- und Verhaltenswissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg im Breisgau Vorgelegt von Kai Nitschke geboren am 07.11.1988 in Erfurt 03. April 2017 Freiburg im Breisgau Gutachter Prof. Dr. Heinrichs Prof. Dr. Tuschen-Caffier Dekan Prof. Dr. Renkl Tag der Promotion 28. September 2017 0 List of Contents Page Acknowledgments..................................... iv Abstract...........................................v Zusammenfassung.................................... vii 1 Theoretical and Methodological Background1 1.1 Planning and Executive Functions.......................1 1.2 The Neural Foundation of Planning.......................5 1.2.1 Insights from Lesion Studies.......................5 1.2.2 A Literature Research on Lesion Data.................6 1.2.3 The Influence of Structuredness.................... 12 1.2.4 Overviews on Hemispheric Lateralization.............. 14 1.2.5 The Present Approach on Examining Neural Lateralization... 15 1.3 The Cognitive Foundation of Planning..................... 16 1.3.1 A Sequential Model of Planning.................... 16 1.3.2 The Tower of London and its Structural Parameters........ 18 1.3.3 The Segmentation of Planning..................... 21 1.3.3.1 Cognitive Architectures for Modeling Sub-processes.. 21 1.3.3.2 Dissociating Components of Planning on Tower Tasks. 25 1.3.3.3 Insights from Eye Movement Studies........... 27 1.3.4 The Present Approach on Cognitive Processes of Planning.... 34 1.4 Methodological Background........................... 35 1.4.1 Activation Likelihood Estimation.................... 35 1.4.2 Eye Movement Measurement...................... 36 1.4.3 Pupillometry................................ 37 1.5 Aims and Objectives of the Thesis........................ 39 i 2 A Meta-Analysis on the Neural Basis of Planning: Activation Likelihood Estimation of Functional Brain Imaging Results in the Tower of London Task 41 2.1 Theoretical Background.............................. 41 2.2 Methods........................................ 43 2.2.1 Study Selection............................... 43 2.2.2 Meta-Analyses on Overall Planning and Planning Complexity.. 45 2.2.3 ALE Approach................................ 47 2.3 Results......................................... 48 2.3.1 The Functional Anatomy of Planning Processes........... 48 2.3.1.1 Meta-analysis of neural activation patterns for overall planning............................. 48 2.3.1.2 Meta-analysis of neural activation patterns for planning complexity............................ 48 2.3.1 The Anatomical Localization of Planning Processes in the dlPFC..................................... 51 2.3.2 The Hemispheric Lateralization of Planning Processes in the PFC and beyond................................. 52 2.3.2.1 ALE contrast analysis..................... 52 2.3.2.2 Exploration of Systematic Differences between Studies 52 2.3.2.3 Whole-brain lateralization analyses............ 55 2.4 Discussion...................................... 55 2.4.1 The Localization of Planning Processes within the Mid-dlPFC.. 56 2.4.2 The Role of the Left and Right Mid-dlPFC in Planning...... 57 2.4.3 The Functional Anatomy of Planning Processes beyond (Mid)- dlPFC..................................... 62 2.4.4 Conclusion.................................. 63 2.5 Additional Data................................... 64 3 Dissociating task-demand-specific differences in cognitive processing during planning and problem solving: A validation approach using pupillometry 71 3.1 Introduction..................................... 71 3.2 Methods........................................ 75 3.2.1 Sample Description............................ 75 3.2.2 The Tower of London Task and Problem Parameters........ 75 ii 3.2.3 Experimental Groups........................... 76 3.2.4 Procedure, Apparatuses, and Processing of Data.......... 77 3.2.5 Statistical Analysis............................. 81 3.3 Results......................................... 81 3.3.1 Main Analysis................................ 81 3.3.2 Post-hoc Analyses............................. 83 3.3.3 Control Analysis.............................. 85 3.4 Discussion...................................... 86 3.4.1 General effects on pupil dilation.................... 87 3.4.2 Parameter-specific effects of Tower Configuration on pupil dilation and processes of internalization ............... 88 3.4.3 Parameter-specific effects of Search Depth on pupil dilation and processes of core planning ........................ 90 3.4.4 Limitations................................. 92 3.4.5 Conclusion.................................. 92 4 General Discussion 95 4.1 Summary of the Studies.............................. 95 4.2 The Neural Lateralization of Planning..................... 97 4.2.1 Different Perspectives on the Neural Functional Lateralization. 97 4.2.2 An Approach on a Unified Neural Theory on Planning...... 103 4.2.3 Limitations of the Meta-analysis on Neural Correlates of Planning................................... 106 4.3 Pupillometry and the Cognitive Framework of Planning.......... 109 4.3.1 Integration of Present Results...................... 109 4.3.2 The Potentials and Limitations of Pupillometry as an Indicator for Cognitive Processes.......................... 111 4.3.3 A Critique of a Sequential Model of Planning............ 114 4.4 Outlook on Linking the Cognitive and Neural Foundations of Planning 116 4.5 Conclusion...................................... 121 5 Bibliography 123 6 Appendix 145 Curriculum Vitae..................................... 146 Publication record and scientific work........................ 149 iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Acknowledgments This thesis would not have been possible without the support of numerous people. I am most greatly indebted to Dr. Christoph Kaller for supervising my PhD thesis from the early days of planning to the final manuscript. Thank you for the opportunity to participate in your research and your support in all stages of this dissertation. Thank you to Prof. Dr. Markus Heinrichs and Prof. Dr. Cornelius Weiller; without your support this thesis would not have been possible. I am very grateful for your professional supervision and the opportunity to be able to use structures of both the university clinic as well as the university. I am greatly indebted to my colleague Dr. Lena Köstering all the discussions we had about various topics and all advice that you gave me during the PhD process. Thank you to Dr. Elisa Scheller for your support, especially in the final stages. I thank my colleagues Konrad Schumacher and Charlotte Schmidt for an enjoyable and relaxing work atmosphere. You provided a great work environment full of discussions, support, and company in my daily work life. Finally, I would like to thank Dr. Julia Asbrand for her tremendous encouragement that helped me overcome numerous obstacles in the entire course of this thesis. Freiburg im Breisgau, 03. April 2017 Kai Nitschke iv Abstract Executive functions (EF) in terms of conscious control of behavior are highly relevant for adequate functioning in everyday life. Planning is a prototypical EF numerously studied in cognitive psychology and neuroscience. However, the specific neural and cognitive correlates are still subject to debate. In detail, there is an ongoing discussion about the neural lateralization of planning in the human brain as well as about the temporal separability of different phases of planning. These two issues were addressed in the presented thesis. Concerning the issue of neural lateralization, a recently published review article on the neural involvement during human planning based on selected brain lesion studies postulated a rather strict hemispheric lateralization of different types of planning tasks. Planning in well-structured tasks such as the Tower of London (ToL) was postulated to be processed solely by the prefrontal cortex (PFC) of the left but not the right hemisphere. This proposal was however contrasted by a qualitative overview of functional neuroimaging studies showing that the right PFC was activated in a large number of functional neuroimaging studies using the well-structured ToL. To substantiate either conclusion, in the first study of this thesis a quantitative meta- analysis was conducted based on the well-established activation likelihood estimation methodology. A thorough literature search on all available ToL studies yielded 29 neuroimaging publications suitable for such an analysis. The analysis statistically confirmed a bilateral involvement of left and right PFC during well-structured planning. Furthermore, an extensive literature search was conducted on brain lesion studies that using well-structured planning tasks also yielded no indication for a unilaterality of well-structured planning. Concerning the issue of temporally separable phases of planning, previous analy- ses of eye-movement patterns during the ToL determined distinct phases of planning. In a recent eye-movement study manipulation of specific structural parameters of ToL problems allowed to isolate and segment of two temporally distinct phases of cognitive processing, an initial phase serving to create a mental representation of the problem (representation creation) and a subsequent phase serving to generate v ABSTRACT the sequence of solution steps, i.e. the actual planning phase (sequence generation). However, the eye tracking methodology entails several
Recommended publications
  • Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: a Meta-Analytic Review
    Neuropsychology Review, Vol. 16, No. 1, March 2006 (C 2006) DOI: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: A Meta-Analytic Review Julie A. Alvarez1 and Eugene Emory2 Published online: 1 June 2006 Currently, there is debate among scholars regarding how to operationalize and measure executive functions. These functions generally are referred to as “supervisory” cognitive processes because they involve higher level organization and execution of complex thoughts and behavior. Although conceptualizations vary regarding what mental processes actually constitute the “executive function” construct, there has been a historical linkage of these “higher-level” processes with the frontal lobes. In fact, many investigators have used the term “frontal functions” synonymously with “executive functions” despite evidence that contradicts this synonymous usage. The current review provides a critical analysis of lesion and neuroimaging studies using three popular executive function measures (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Phonemic Verbal Fluency, and Stroop Color Word Interference Test) in order to examine the validity of the executive function construct in terms of its relation to activation and damage to the frontal lobes. Empirical lesion data are examined via meta-analysis procedures along with formula derivatives. Results reveal mixed evidence that does not support a one-to-one relationship between executive functions and frontal lobe activity. The paper concludes with a discussion of the implications of construing the validity of these neuropsychological tests in anatomical, rather than cognitive and behavioral, terms. KEY WORDS: Executive function; Frontal lobe; Neuropsychology; Meta-analysis. Executive functions generally refer to “higher-level” ropsychological processes involving (a) cognitive flexi- cognitive functions involved in the control and regulation bility, (b) problem-solving, and (c) response maintenance of “lower-level” cognitive processes and goal-directed, (Greve et al., 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • FEMA Developing and Maintaining Emergency Operations Plans
    Developing and Maintaining Emergency Operations Plans Comprehensive Preparedness Guide (CPG) 101 Version 2.0 November 2010 I am pleased to announce the release of Version 2.0 of Comprehensive Preparedness Guide 101: Developing and Maintaining Emergency Operations Plans. Comprehensive Preparedness Guide (CPG) 101 provides guidance for developing emergency operations plans. It promotes a common understanding of the fundamentals of risk-informed planning and decision making to help planners examine a hazard or threat and produce integrated, coordinated, and synchronized plans. The goal of CPG 101 is to assist in making the planning process routine across all phases of emergency management and for all homeland security mission areas. This Guide helps planners at all levels of government in their efforts to develop and maintain viable, all-hazards, all-threats emergency plans. Based on input from state, territorial, tribal, and local officials from across the United States, this update of CPG 101 expands on the fundamentals contained in the first version. With this edition, greater emphasis is placed on representing and engaging the whole community—to include those with access and functional needs, children, and those with household pets and service animals. Residents and all sectors of the community have a critical role and shared responsibility to take appropriate actions to protect themselves, their families and organizations, and their properties. Planning that engages and includes the whole community serves as the focal point for building a collaborative and resilient community. CPG 101 is the foundation for state, territorial, tribal, and local emergency planning in the United States. Planners in other disciplines, organizations, and the private sector, as well as other levels of government, may find this Guide useful in the development of their emergency operations plans.
    [Show full text]
  • Frontal Stroke: Problem Solving, Decision Making, Impulsiveness
    PSYCHOLOGY Psychology & Neuroscience, 2011, 4, 2, 267 - 278 NEUROSCIENCE DOI: 10.3922/j.psns.2011.2.012 Frontal stroke: problem solving, decision making, impulsiveness, and depressive symptoms in men and women Morgana Scheffer1, Janine Kieling Monteiro1 and Rosa Maria Martins de Almeida2 1 - Universidade do Vale do Rio dos Sinos, RS, Brazil 2 - Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil Abstract The present study compared men and women who suffered a frontal lobe stroke with regard to problem solving, decision making, impulsive behavior and depressive symptoms and also correlated these variables between groups. The sample was composed of 10 males and nine females. The study period was 6 months after the stroke. The following instruments were used: Wisconsin Card Sort Test (WCST), Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), Barrat Impulsiveness Scale (BIS11), and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). For the exclusion criteria of the sample, the Mini International Psychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I Plus) and Mini Mental Stage Examination (MMSE) were used. To measure functional severity post-stroke, the Rankin Scale was used. The average age was 60.90 ± 8.93 years for males and 60.44 ± 11.57 years for females. In females, total impulsiveness (p = .013) and lack of planning caused by impulsiveness (p = .028) were significantly higher compared with males, assessed by the BIS11. These data indicate that females in the present sample who suffered a chronic frontal lesion were more impulsive and presented more planning difficulties in situations without demanding cognitive processing. These results that show gender differences should be considered when planning psychotherapy and cognitive rehabilitation for patients who present these characteristics.
    [Show full text]
  • Designing Cities, Planning for People
    Designing cities, planning for people The guide books of Otto-Iivari Meurman and Edmund Bacon Minna Chudoba Tampere University of Technology School of Architecture [email protected] Abstract Urban theorists and critics write with an individual knowledge of the good urban life. Recently, writing about such life has boldly called for smart cities or even happy cities, stressing the importance of social connections and nearness to nature, or social and environmental capital. Although modernist planning has often been blamed for many current urban problems, the social and the environmental dimensions were not completely absent from earlier 20th century approaches to urban planning. Links can be found between the urban utopia of today and the mid-20th century ideas about good urban life. Changes in the ideas of what constitutes good urban life are investigated in this paper through two texts by two different 20th century planners: Otto-Iivari Meurman and Edmund Bacon. Both were taught by the Finnish planner Eliel Saarinen, and according to their teacher’s example, also wrote about their planning ideas. Meurman’s guide book for planners was published in 1947, and was a major influence on Finnish post-war planning. In Meurman’s case, the book answered a pedagogical need, as planners were trained to meet the demands of the structural changes of society and the needs of rapidly growing Finnish cities. Bacon, in a different context, stressed the importance of an urban design attitude even when planning the movement systems of a modern metropolis. Bacon’s book from 1967 was meant for both designers and city dwellers, exploring the dynamic nature of modern urbanity.
    [Show full text]
  • 2020 Edmund N. Bacon Urban Design Awards
    2020 Edmund N. Bacon Urban Design Awards Student Design Competition Brief $25 | per entry at the time of submission $5,000 | First Prize Award Sign up now for more information www.PhiladelphiaCFA.org IMPORTANT DATES August 1, 2019: Full Competition Packet released + Competition opens November 22, 2019: Final date to submit entries February 2020 (date TBD): Awards Ceremony in Philadelphia EDMUND N. BACON URBAN DESIGN AWARDS Founded in 2006 in memory of Philadelphia’s iconic 20th century city planner, Edmund N. Bacon [1910-2005], this annual program honors both professionals and students whose work epitomize excellence in urban design. Each year, a professional who has made significant contributions to the field of urban planning is selected to receive our Edmund N. Bacon Award. In addition, the winners of an international student urban design competition, envisioning a better Philadelphia, are honored with our Edmund N. Bacon Student Awards. The combined awards ceremony is hosted in Philadelphia each February. 2020 STUDENT AWARDS COMPETITION TOPIC THE BIG PICTURE: REVEALING GERMANTOWN’S ASSETS Chelten Avenue is the heart of the Germantown business district in northwest Philadelphia. The most economically diverse neighborhood in the city, Germantown is an African American community which bridges the economically disadvantaged neighborhoods of North Philadelphia to the east with the wealthier Mount Airy and Chestnut Hill neighborhoods to the west. The Chelten Avenue shopping district benefits from two regional rail stations (along different train lines) and one of the busiest bus stops in the city, located midway between the stations. In addition, the southern end of the shopping district is just steps from the expansive Wissahickon Valley Park, one of the most wild places in Philadelphia, visited by over 1 million people each year.
    [Show full text]
  • The Challenge of Recreation Planning: Methodology and Factors to Consider
    THE CHALLENGE OF RECREATION PLANNING: METHODOLOGY AND FACTORS TO CONSIDER by RONALD B. ULECK, Research Associate, Department of For- estry, University of Illinois, Urbana, Ill. This paper is adapted from a Ph.D. dissertation, "Guidelines for preparing development plans for public resource-based outdoor recreation areas." submitted to New York State University College of Forestry at Syracuse Uni- versity, Syracuse, N. Y. ABSTRACT. The proposed methodology of planning is a descrip- tion, explanation, and justification of the methods or techniques that a planner should use in preparing outdoor recreation development plans. The sequence of steps required is described. INTRODUCTION AND SCOPE In this paper, public outdoor recreation development planning is defined as a ra- UTDOOR RECREATION planning tional and systematic process, integrated 0 is a broad and complex activity. This with all the important social and physical paper deals with one aspect of that factors, for determining appropriate action activity: development planning - that is, in developing the natural resource to pro- planning of the natural resource base to vide outdoor recreational opportunities.* change its potential capacity for providing recreational opportunities. The primary The guidelines were developed from orientation is toward providing guidelines several sources. General planning theory, for planning an individual outdoor recrea- methodology, methods, and techniques for tion area or complex such as a state, county, various kinds of planning (e.g., urban, en- or regional
    [Show full text]
  • Planning Commission Handbook
    Routt County Planning Department Box 773749 Steamboat Springs, CO 80477 970-879-2704 Routt County Planning Commission Handbook March, 2015 PI2014-007 Introduction Welcome to the Planning Commission The Planning Commission is one of the most This handbook will help you understand the legal visible groups responsible for planning in and policy framework for planning. It will also unincorporated Routt County including the provide you with a reference for the powers and communities of Milner, Phippsburg, Toponas and duties of the planning commission. As a planning Hahns Peak. commissioner you must have some understanding of the following topics: The Planning Commission is an appointed board of residents with commissioners selected from 1. Comprehensive planning. four different districts and four municipalities in 2. Zoning and platting. Routt County (North Routt – District 1, Land 3. How the planning commission operates and around Steamboat Springs – District 2, West the role and responsibilities of each individual Routt - District 3, and South Routt – District 4, the member. Towns of Yampa, Hayden, Oak Creek, and the City 4. The authority and duties of the commission. of Steamboat Springs) established as an advisory 5. Legal aspects of commission conduct. group for the Board of County Commissioners on 6. Standards for commission decision-making. planning matters and to perform other duties authorized by state statute. Even though you may This Planning Commission Handbook covers all of have been appointed from a certain area, your these topics and you will be given copies of the job is to represent the whole community. The Routt County Zoning and Subdivision Regulations Planning Commission’s principle responsibility is and the Routt County Master Plan.
    [Show full text]
  • Strategic Planning and Management Guidelines for Transportation Agencies
    331 NATIONAL COOPERATIVE HIGHWAY RESEARCH PROGRAM REPORT 33I STRATEGIC PLANNING AND MANAGEMENT GUIDELINES FOR TRANSPORTATION AGENCIES TRANSPORTATION RESEARCH BOARD NATIONAL RESEARCH COUNCIL TRANSPORTATION RESEARCH BOARD EXECUTIVE COMMITTEE 1990 OFFICERS Chairman: Wayne Muri, Chief Engineer, Missouri Highway & Transportation Department Vice Chairman: C. Michael Walton, Bess Harris Jones Centennial Professor and Chairman, College of Engineering, The University of Texas at Austin Executive Director: Thomas B. Deen, Transportation Research Board MEMBERS JAMES B. BUSEY IV, Federal Aviation Administrator, U.S. Department of Transportation (ex officio) GILBERT E. CARMICHAEL, Federal Railroad Administrator, U.S. Department of Transportation. (ex officio) BRIAN W. CLYMER, Urban Mass Transportation Administrator, US. Department of Transportation (ex officio) JERRY R. CURRY, National Highway Traffic Safety Administrator, US. Department of Transportation (ex officio) FRANCIS B. FRANCOIS, Executive Director, American Association of State Highway and Transportation Officials (ex officio) JOHN GRAY, President, Notional Asphalt Pavement Association (ex officio) THOMAS H. HANNA, President and Chief Executive Officer, Motor Vehicle Manufacturers Association of the United States, Inc. (ex officio) HENRY J. HATCH, Chief of Engineers and Commander, U.S. Army Corps of Engineers (ex officio) THOMAS D. LARSON, Federal Highway Administrator, U.S. Department of Transportation (ex officio) GEORGE H. WAY, JR., Vice President for Research and Test Departments, Association
    [Show full text]
  • Strategic Planning Basics for Managers
    STRATEGIC PLANNING Guide for Managers 1 Strategic Planning Basics for Managers In all UN offices, departments and missions, it is critical that managers utilize the most effective approach toward developing a strategy for their existing programmes and when creating new programmes. Managers use the strategy to communicate the direction to staff members and guide the larger department or office work. Here you will find practical techniques based on global management best practices. Strategic planning defined Strategic planning is a process of looking into the future and identifying trends and issues against which to align organizational priorities of the Department or Office. Within the Departments and Offices, it means aligning a division, section, unit or team to a higher-level strategy. In the UN, strategy is often about achieving a goal in the most effective and efficient manner possible. For a few UN offices (and many organizations outside the UN), strategy is about achieving a mission comparatively better than another organization (i.e. competition). For everyone, strategic planning is about understanding the challenges, trends and issues; understanding who are the key beneficiaries or clients and what they need; and determining the most effective and efficient way possible to achieve the mandate. A good strategy drives focus, accountability, and results. How and where to apply strategic planning UN departments, offices, missions and programmes develop strategic plans to guide the delivery of an overall mandate and direct multiple streams of work. Sub-entities create compatible strategies depending on their size and operational focus. Smaller teams within a department/office or mission may not need to create strategies; there are, however, situations in which small and medium teams may need to think strategically, in which case the following best practices can help structure the thinking.
    [Show full text]
  • Managing Gentrification
    ULI Community Catalyst Report NUMBER 5 Managing Gentrification UrbanUrban LandLand $ InstituteInstitute COVER: A Chicago greystone; Julie Jaidenger/UIC City Design Center. ULI Community Catalyst Report NUMBER 5 Managing Gentrification The 2006 ULI/Charles H. Shaw Forum on Urban Community Issues September 21–22, 2006 Prepared by Deborah L. Myerson Urban Land $ Institute ULI Community Catalyst Report ABOUT ULI–the Urban Land Institute ULI PROJECT STAFF The mission of the Urban Land Institute is to provide leadership in Maureen McAvey the responsible use of land and in creating and sustaining thriving Executive Vice President, Initiatives communities worldwide. ULI is committed to: Michael Pawlukiewicz • Bringing together leaders from across the fields of real estate and land use policy to exchange best practices and serve community Director, Environment and needs; Sustainable Development • Fostering collaboration within and beyond ULI’s membership Deborah L. Myerson through mentoring, dialogue, and problem solving; Consultant to ULI • Exploring issues of urbanization, conservation, regeneration, land Samara Wolf use, capital formation, and sustainable development; Manager, Meetings and Events • Advancing land use policies and design practices that respect the uniqueness of both built and natural environments; Nancy H. Stewart Director, Book Program • Sharing knowledge through education, applied research, publish- ing, and electronic media; and Laura Glassman, Publications • Sustaining a diverse global network of local practice and advisory
    [Show full text]
  • A Cognitive Model of Planning*
    COGNITIVE SCIENCE 3, 275-310 (1979) A Cognitive Model of Planning* BARBARA HAYES-ROTH AND FREDERICK HAYES-ROTH The Rand Corporation, Santa Monica, California This paper presents a cognitive model of the planning process. The model gener- alizes the theoretical architecture of the Hearsay-II system. Thus, it assumes that planning comprises the activities of a variety of cognitive "specialists." Each specialist can suggest certain kinds of decisions for incorporation into the plan in progress, These include decisions about: (a) how to approach the planning problem; (b) what knowledge bears on the problem; (c) what kinds of actions to try to plan; (d) what specific actions to plan; and (e) how to allocate cognitive resources during planning, Within each of these categories, different specialists suggest decisions at different levels of abstraction. The activities of the various specialists are not coordinated in any systematic way. Instead, the specialists operate opportunistically, suggesting decisions whenever promising opportunities arise. The paper presents a detailed account of the model and illustrates its assumptions with a "thinking aloud" protocol. It also describes the performance of a computer simulation of the model. The paper contrasts the proposed model with successive refinement models and attempts to resolve apparent differences between the two points of view. 1. INTRODUCTION Planning is a familiar cognitive activity. We all have many opportunities to decide how we will behave in future situations. For example, we plan how to get to work in the morning, where and with whom to eat lunch, and how to spend our evenings. We also make longer-term plans, such as what to do on our vacations, how to celebrate Christmas, and what career path to follow.
    [Show full text]
  • Planning with Brain-Inspired AI Naoya Arakawa the Whole Brain Architecture Initiative [email protected] Abstract
    Planning with Brain-inspired AI Naoya Arakawa The Whole Brain Architecture Initiative [email protected] Abstract This article surveys engineering and neuroscientific models of planning as a cognitive function, which is regarded as a typical function of fluid intelligence in the discussion of general intelligence. It aims to present existing planning models as references for realizing the planning function in brain-inspired AI or artificial general intelligence (AGI). It also proposes themes for the research and development of brain-inspired AI from the viewpoint of tasks and architecture. Introduction This article surveys engineering and neuroscientific models of planning as a cognitive function. It aims to present existing planning models as references for realizing the planning function in brain-inspired AI or artificial general intelligence (AGI). It also proposes themes for the research and development of brain-inspired AI from the viewpoint of tasks and architecture. This survey may develop into specific requests for research. Planning is an important cognitive function for general intelligence in that it solves problems on the fly without new learning. In psychological discussions of general intelligence, planning is regarded as a typical function of fluid intelligence. In engineering, planning has been studied since the dawn of AI. While there are solutions for symbolically formulated tasks, solving problems in the real world is still difficult. Regarding non-human animals, Caledonian crows, for example, are known to solve planning tasks, and other animals may plan in order to survive in the wild. In the mammalian brain, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is known to be involved in planning.
    [Show full text]