Aspects of Phonological Fusion James E

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Aspects of Phonological Fusion James E Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1975, Vol. 104, No. 2, 105-120 Aspects of Phonological Fusion James E. Cutting Yale University and Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut Phonological fusion occurs when the phonemes of two different speech stimuli are combined into a new percept that is longer and linguistically more complex than either of the two inputs. For example, when PAY is presented to one ear and LAY to the other, the subject often perceives PLAY. The present article is an investigation of the conditions necessary and sufficient for fusion to occur. The rules governing phonological fusion appear to be the same for synthetic and natural speech, but synthetic stimuli fuse more readily. Fusion occurs considerably more often in di- chotic stimulus presentation than in binaural presentation. The phenomenon is remarkably tolerant of differences in relative onset time between the to-be-fused stimuli and of relative differences in fundamental frequency, intensity, and vocal tract configuration. Although phonological fusion is insensitive to such nonlinguistic stimulus parameters, it is sensitive to lin- guistic variations at the semantic, phonemic, and acoustic levels. Most of the dichotic listening literature ments of both stimuli are combined to form has dealt with the phenomenon of perceptual a new percept that is longer and linguistically rivalry. Given a different stimulus presented more complex than either of the two inputs. to each ear at the same time, the subject This phenomenon is called phonological typically reports hearing one or both of fusion because it conforms to phonological them. The different information contained rules of English: Given BANKET/LAN- in each stimulus is not combined into a sin- KET, the subject reports hearing BLAN- gle percept. Thus, for example, given the KET, not LBANKET (Day, Note 1). dichotic digits FOUR/SIX, the subject Nonword fusions also occur; for example, never reports hearing SORE or FIX. Per- GAB/LAB yields GLAB, not LGAB (Day, ceptual fusion does occur when certain vari- 1968). According to phonological rules of ables are taken in account. In several types cluster information in English, initial stop of fusion phenomena, the stimulus variables consonant + liquid clusters are allowed, but that facilitate fusion appear to be psycho- initial liquid + stop clusters are not. Day linguistic in nature. For example, given the (1970) found that when stimuli are pre- dichotic pair PAHDUCT/RAHDUCT, the sented dichotically with these constraints re- subject often reports hearing PRODUCT moved, fusion occurs in both directions. (Day, 1968). In this type of fusion, seg- Given the stimuli TASS/TACK, for exam- This research was supported by National Insti- ple, the subject reports hearing TASK on tute of Child Health and Human Development some trials and TACKS on others. Both Grant HD-01994 to the Haskins Laboratories, and /sk/ and /ks/ clusters are permissible in was based in part on a PhD thesis submitted to final position in English. the Department of Psychology of Yale University. Thanks go to Ruth S. Day, with whom I have Phonological fusion cannot be explained as never had a colorless conversation; Franklin S. a response bias for acceptable English words. Cooper, with whom I have never had a fruitless Day (1968) found that when different pro- conversation; and the members of the New Haven ductions of PAHDUCT are presented to Dance Ensemble, with whom I never talked about this at all. each ear, subjects report hearing PAH- The author is now at Wesleyan University, DUCT. That is, they do not report hearing Middletown, Connecticut and Haskins Laborato- the acceptable English word that corre- ries. Requests for reprints should be sent to James E. Cutting, Haskins Laboratories, 270 sponds most closely to the nonword inputs. Crown Street, New Haven, Connecticut 06510. Likewise, RAHDUCT/RAHDUCT yields 105 106 JAMES E. CUTTING RAHDUCT. Only when the stimuli are each formant gliding to the resting frequency of PAHDUCT/RAHDUCT does fusion occur, the following vowel. Stop consonants began with 50 msec of formant transitions gliding into the and it occurs regardless of which stimulus following vowel. Voice onset time was 0 msec is presented to which ear. In the present for voiced consonants and +50 msec for voiceless article, I will consider a fusion response to consonants. Natural speech versions of the same occur when a stimulus pair, each item of items were spoken by the author and recorded on audio tape. An effort was made to match utter- which has n phonemes, yields a percept of ances in terms of pitch, intensity, and duration, n + 1 phonemes (following Day, 1968). but some variation was unavoidable. Both syn- The studies presented here are an anal- thetic and natural speech stimuli were digitized ysis of phonological fusion and the condi- and stored on disk file for preparation of diotic tions necessary and sufficient for fusion to and dichotic tapes (Cooper & Mattingly, 1969). Subjects. Sixteen Yale University undergradu- occur. Of primary interest are the modes ates participated in identification and fusion tasks. of presentation that optimize the occurrence Each received course credit for his or her efforts. of fusion responses and the stimulus vari- As in all experiments in this paper, subjects were ables under those conditions that enhance right-handed native American English speakers with no history of hearing trouble and with no and detract from the fused percept. Both experience listening to synthetic speech. They linguistic and physical (nonlinguistic) vari- listened in groups of four to tapes played on an ables are of interest. Ampex AG-500 dual-track tape recorder. Audi- To undertake such a project, one must be tory signals were sent through a listening sta- tion to matched Telephonies earphones (Model able to control the stimuli in a precise man- TDH39). Gains on the tape recorder and listen- ner, and such control is only available with ing station were adjusted so that stimuli were pre- a computer-driven speech synthesizer. How- sented at approximately 80 db. (re 20 /u,N/m2). ever, synthetic speech has a characteristic These criteria and procedures were used for all experiments in the present article. No subject "cold-in-the-nose" quality which may effect served in more than one experiment. The fusion phonological fusion. To insure that the task in the present study preceded the identification present studies are not concerned with arti- task, but it is more instructive to consider them facts inherent to synthetic speech, fusion was in reverse order. first observed for synthetic and natural Task 1: Identification of Individual Stimuli speech tokens of the same items. All stimuli, natural and synthetic, were recorded on a diotic identification tape. The tape consisted SYNTHETIC VERSUS NATURAL SPEECH of a random sequence of 120 trials: (4 sets of Experiment 1: Baseline Data stimuli) X (2 versions of each set: natural and synthetic) X (3 stimuli per set) X (5 observa- General Method tions per stimulus). Subjects wrote down the en- tire word that they heard presented. There was a Tape preparation. Four sets of stimuli of the 3-sec interval between items. same general pattern were selected: the PAY set (PAY, LAY, RAY) ; the BED set (BED, LED, Results RED) ; the CAM set (CAM, LAMB, RAM) ; and the GO set (GO, LOW, ROW). Two ver- All stimuli were highly identifiable. Each sions of each stimulus were prepared, one in syn- synthetic and natural speech item was cor- thetic speech and one in natural speech. Synthetic stimuli were prepared on the Haskins Laborato- rectly identified on better than 93% of all ries' parallel-resonance synthesizer. All stimuli trials. Cluster responses (such as PLAY) were identical in pitch contour, rising from 116 to were reported on less than 2% of all trials. 124 Hz and then falling to 82 Hz. Within each Identification tasks were a part of each ex- set all stimuli were identical in duration (3SO, 450, 400, and 350 msec for the four sets, respectively), periment presented in this article. Since the and all had the same acoustic structure except for stimuli and results generally did not differ the initial ISO msec. Liquid stimuli within each from those in Experiment 1, the identifica- set (those beginning with /!/ and /r/) differed tions are not discussed in all experiments. only in the direction and extent of the third- formant transition. This acoustic cue distinguishes Task 2: Identification of Fusible Pairs the two liquids (O'Connor, Gerstman, Liberman, D&lattre, & Cooper, 1957). Each liquid stimulus All stimuli used in the identification task were began with 50-msec steady state prerelease formant also used in the fusion task. However, instead of resonances, followed by 100-msec transitions in presenting one stimulus at a time, two stimuli PHONOLOGICAL FUSION 107 were presented, one to each ear. The dichotic pair TABLE 1 consisted of a stop stimulus and a liquid stimulus PERCENT FUSION RESPONSES FOR EACH PAIR IN from the same set; for example, PAY/LAY or EXPERIMENT 1, IN BOTH SYNTHETIC SPEECH PAY/RAY. All stimuli and possible fusions were AND NATURAL SPEECH VERSIONS monosyllabic, high-frequency English words: PAY/ LAY -» (yields) PLAY, PAY/RAY -» PRAY, Percent fusion BED/LED -» BLED, BED/RED -» BREAD, Fusible pair CAM/LAMB -» CLAM, CAM/RAM -> CRAM, Synthetic Natural M GO/LOW -» GLOW, and GO/ROW -» GROW. Most of these items and fusions have Thorndike- PAY/LAY 78 41 59 Lorge (1944) word frequencies of at least 100 per PAY/RAY 70 33 51 million. No alveolar stop-consonant stimuli were (55) chosen because /tl/ and /dl/ clusters do not ap- BED/LED 48 33 40 pear in initial position in English. BED/RED 46 19 33 (37) Two dichotic tapes were prepared, one using CAM/RAM 63 29 46 synthetic stimuli and one using natural speech CAM/LAMB 57 17 37 stimuli.
Recommended publications
  • Vowel Quality and Phonological Projection
    i Vowel Quality and Phonological Pro jection Marc van Oostendorp PhD Thesis Tilburg University September Acknowledgements The following p eople have help ed me prepare and write this dissertation John Alderete Elena Anagnostop oulou Sjef Barbiers Outi BatEl Dorothee Beermann Clemens Bennink Adams Bo domo Geert Bo oij Hans Bro ekhuis Norb ert Corver Martine Dhondt Ruud and Henny Dhondt Jo e Emonds Dicky Gilb ers Janet Grijzenhout Carlos Gussenhoven Gert jan Hakkenb erg Marco Haverkort Lars Hellan Ben Hermans Bart Holle brandse Hannekevan Ho of Angeliek van Hout Ro eland van Hout Harry van der Hulst Riny Huybregts Rene Kager HansPeter Kolb Emiel Krah mer David Leblanc Winnie Lechner Klarien van der Linde John Mc Carthy Dominique Nouveau Rolf Noyer Jaap and Hannyvan Oosten dorp Paola Monachesi Krisztina Polgardi Alan Prince Curt Rice Henk van Riemsdijk Iggy Ro ca Sam Rosenthall Grazyna Rowicka Lisa Selkirk Chris Sijtsma Craig Thiersch MiekeTrommelen Rub en van der Vijver Janneke Visser Riet Vos Jero en van de Weijer Wim Zonneveld Iwant to thank them all They have made the past four years for what it was the most interesting and happiest p erio d in mylife until now ii Contents Intro duction The Headedness of Syllables The Headedness Hyp othesis HH Theoretical Background Syllable Structure Feature geometry Sp ecication and Undersp ecicati on Skeletal tier Mo del of the grammar Optimality Theory Data Organisation of the thesis Chapter Chapter
    [Show full text]
  • Sound Change, Priming, Salience
    Sound change, priming, salience Producing and perceiving variation in Liverpool English Marten Juskan language Language Variation 3 science press Language Variation Editors: John Nerbonne, Dirk Geeraerts In this series: 1. Côté, Marie-Hélène, Remco Knooihuizen and John Nerbonne (eds.). The future of dialects. 2. Schäfer, Lea. Sprachliche Imitation: Jiddisch in der deutschsprachigen Literatur (18.–20. Jahrhundert). 3. Juskan, Marten. Sound change, priming, salience: Producing and perceiving variation in Liverpool English. ISSN: 2366-7818 Sound change, priming, salience Producing and perceiving variation in Liverpool English Marten Juskan language science press Marten Juskan. 2018. Sound change, priming, salience: Producing and perceiving variation in Liverpool English (Language Variation 3). Berlin: Language Science Press. This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/210 © 2018, Marten Juskan Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-96110-119-1 (Digital) 978-3-96110-120-7 (Hardcover) ISSN: 2366-7818 DOI:10.5281/zenodo.1451308 Source code available from www.github.com/langsci/210 Collaborative reading: paperhive.org/documents/remote?type=langsci&id=210 Cover and concept of design: Ulrike Harbort Typesetting: Marten Juskan Proofreading: Alec Shaw, Amir Ghorbanpour, Andreas Hölzl, Felix Hoberg, Havenol Schrenk, Ivica Jeđud, Jeffrey Pheiff, Jeroen van de Weijer, Kate Bellamy Fonts: Linux Libertine, Libertinus Math, Arimo, DejaVu Sans Mono Typesetting software:Ǝ X LATEX Language Science Press Unter den Linden 6 10099 Berlin, Germany langsci-press.org Storage and cataloguing done by FU Berlin To Daniela Contents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Intentions – what this study is about ..............
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. UMI University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information C om pany 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Order Number 9401204 Phonetics and phonology of Nantong Chinese Ac, Benjamin Xiaoping, Ph.D. The Ohio State University, 1993 Copyri^t ©1993 by Ao, Benjamin Xiaoping.
    [Show full text]
  • Feet and Fusion: the Case of Malay,’ Appears in the Proceedings of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society's 13Th Annual Conference
    Ann Delilkan Head-dependent Asymmetry: Feet and Fusion in Malay1 Abstract The variegated data relating to fusion in Malay have long escaped unified analysis. Past analyses assume that the conditioning environment is morphological. I suggest the question to ask is not why fusion occurs where it does but why it does not occur elsewhere. I show, too, that the answer to this question lies unquestionably in the consideration of prosody, not morphology. Crucially, I claim that the demand for feature recoverability interacts with the prosodic specifications of the language. While seeking independent support in the language for this claim, I find evidence that, far from demanding special treatment, fusion is more appropriately seen as a natural impulse in the language, one of many segmental changes that occur to effect greater prosodic unmarkedness. Within my description of the parameters of fusion, all data relevant to the process that have hitherto been cited as exceptions are, instead, predictable. Fusion in Malay is, in fact, learnable. 1. Introduction: Background and data The complex data relating to fusion (or ‘nasal substation’) in Malay have long escaped unified analysis. Most past analyses assume that the crucial conditioning environment for the process is purely morphological. Thus, it is claimed that root-initial voiceless obstruents trigger fusion when preceded by nasal-final prefixes (Teoh (1994), Onn (1980), Zaharani (1998), and, on related data in Indonesian, Pater (1996)). All analyses, however, suffer from incomplete coverage of the relevant data, numerous instances set aside as ‘exceptions’, if mentioned at all. In (1), which derives from Delilkan (1999, 2000), ‘•’ denotes data previously considered exceptional.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Phonetic Bias in Sound Change
    i i “Garrett-Johnson” — 2011/11/26 — 15:25 — page 1 — #1 i i 1 Phonetic bias in sound change ANDREW GARRETT AND KEITH JOHNSON 1.1 Introduction Interest in the phonetics of sound change is as old as scientific linguistics (Osthoff and Brugman 1878).1 The prevalent view is that a key component of sound change is what Hyman (1977) dubbed phonologization: the process or processes by which automatic phonetic patterns give rise to a language’s phonological patterns. Sound patterns have a variety of other sources, including analogical change, but we focus here on their phonetic 1 For helpful discussion we thank audiences at UC Berkeley and UC Davis, and seminar students in 2008 (Garrett) and 2010 (Johnson). We are also very grateful to Juliette Blevins, Joan Bybee, Larry Hyman, John Ohala, and Alan Yu, whose detailed comments on an earlier version of this chapter have saved us from many errors and injudicious choices, though we know they will not all agree with what they find here. i i i i i i “Garrett-Johnson” — 2011/11/26 — 15:25 — page 2 — #2 i i 2 Andrew Garrett and Keith Johnson grounding.2 In the study of phonologization and sound change, the three long-standing questions in (1) are especially important. (1) a. Typology: Why are some sound changes common while others are rare or nonexistent? b. Conditioning: What role do lexical and morphological factors play in sound change? c. Actuation: What triggers a particular sound change at a particular time and place? In this chapter we will address the typology and actuation questions in some detail; the conditioning question, though significant and controver- sial, will be discussed only briefly (in §1.5.3).
    [Show full text]
  • Mayan Phonology Ryan Bennett∗ Yale University
    Language and Linguistics Compass 10/10 (2016): 469–514, 10.1111/lnc3.12148 Mayan phonology Ryan Bennett∗ Yale University Abstract This paper presents an overview of the phonetics and phonology of Mayan languages. The focus is primarily descriptive, but the article also attempts to frame the sound patterns of Mayan languages within a larger typological and theoretical context. Special attention is given to areas which are ripe for further descriptive or theoretical investigation. 1. Introduction This article provides an overview of the phonology of Mayan languages. The principal aim is to describe the commonalities and differences between phonological systems in the Mayan family. I have also tried to highlight some phonological patterns which are interesting from a typological or theoretical perspective. There are many intriguing aspects of Mayan phonology which remain understudied; I’ve noted a handful of such phenomena here, with the hope that other researchers will take up these fascinating and difficult questions. Apart from phonology proper, this article also touches on the phonetics and morpho- syntax of Mayan languages (see also Coon this volume). Most work on Mayan phonology has focused on basic phonetic description and phonemic analysis. A much smaller portion of the literature deals directly with phonotactics, alternations, prosody, morpho-phonology, or quantitative phonetics. Since phonology connects with all of these topics, I have tried to be as thematically inclusive as possible given the current state of research on Mayan sound patterns. The phonemic systems of Mayan languages are comparatively well-studied, thanks to the efforts of numerous Mayan and non-Mayan scholars. This article has drawn heavily on grammatical descriptions published by the Proyecto Lingüístico Francisco Marroquín (PLFM), Oxlajuuj Keej Maya’ Ajtz’ib’ (OKMA), and the Academia de Lenguas Mayas de Guatemala (ALMG); many of these works were written by native-speaker linguists.
    [Show full text]
  • Turkish Vowel Epenthesis
    Turkish vowel epenthesis Jorge Hankamer Introduction One of the striking features of Turkish morphophonology is the system of C~0 and V~0 alternations at morpheme boundaries, which seem to conspire to prevent the occurrence of a stem-final V and a suffix-initial V next to each other. The classic analysis of this phenomenon was provided by Lees (1961), and has been generally accepted since. Zimmer (1965, 1970), however, noticed a flaw in the Lees analysis, and proposed a correction, which will be discussed in section 2. The present paper argues that there is a second flaw in the Lees analysis, and one that weakens the case for Zimmer’s proposed correction. Instead, I propose a more radical reanalysis of the phenomenon, in which most of the V~0 alternations are due not to vowel deletions, as in the Lees analysis, but to vowel epenthesis.This raises some theoretically interesting questions. 1 The V~0 alternation Turkish,1 with certain exceptions,2 does not permit VV sequences within words. Thus, while both V-initial and V-final syllables are permitted, VV sequences are avoided both _________________ 1 The variety of Turkish investigated here is “Modern Standard Turkish”, cf. Lees 1961, Lewis 1967. 2 There are four classes of exceptions: (i) some borrowed roots which originally had a glottal stop either between vowels or at the end of the root (the glottal stop being lost in modern Turkish) have VV root-internally or V+V with a V-initial suffix: saat ‘hour,clock’ <Arabic sa?at şair ‘poet’ <Arabic şa?ir camii ‘mosque-poss’ <cami? + (s)I (ii) Turkish words with syllable-initial ğ (“yumuşak ge”) have VV superficially due to the zero realization of the ğ: kağıt ‘paper’ dağı < dağ + (y)I ‘mountain-acc’ geleceği < gel + AcAg+I ‘come-fut-poss’ Commonly, the first vowel is just pronounced long.
    [Show full text]
  • Stop the Morphological Cycle, I Want to Get Off: Modeling the Development of Fusion
    Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics Volume 3 Article 40 2020 Stop the Morphological Cycle, I Want to Get Off: Modeling the Development of Fusion Micha Elsner The Ohio State University, [email protected] Martha B. Johnson The Ohio State University, [email protected] Stephanie Antetomaso The Ohio State University, [email protected] Andrea D. Sims The Ohio State University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/scil Part of the Computational Linguistics Commons, and the Morphology Commons Recommended Citation Elsner, Micha; Johnson, Martha B.; Antetomaso, Stephanie; and Sims, Andrea D. (2020) "Stop the Morphological Cycle, I Want to Get Off: Modeling the Development of Fusion," Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics: Vol. 3 , Article 40. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7275/33wn-nb03 Available at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/scil/vol3/iss1/40 This Paper is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Stop the Morphological Cycle, I Want to Get Off: Modeling the Development of Fusion Micha Elsner, Martha Booker Johnson, Stephanie Antetomaso, Andrea D. Sims Department of Linguistics The Ohio State University elsner.14,johnson.6713,antetomaso.2,[email protected] Abstract Along with this taxonomic distinction comes a historical origin story, sometimes called the mor- Historical linguists observe that many fusional phological cycle (Hock and Joseph, 1996)[183].
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in African Linguistics Volume 23, Number 2, 1992-1994
    Studies in African Linguistics Volume 23, Number 2, 1992-1994 EPENTHESIS, MUTATION, AND STRUCTURE PRESERVATION IN THE SHONA CAUSATIVE Scott Myers University of Texas In Shona (Bantu: Zimbabwe), the causative fonn of a verb is fonned in one of two ways: either with the suffix -is-/-es- or by changing the root final consonant to a corresponding coronal continuant. The author argues for an analysis in which both fonns are derived from a common underlying suffix /sl. The suffixal fonn is the result of an independently motivated process of epenthesis. The mutation, on the other hand, comes about by fusion of the /s/ with the preceding consonant. This fusion leads in some cases to feature combinations disallowed in Shona. The effects of mutation in these cases can be captured exploiting an active version of Kiparsky's Structure Preservation in tenns of "persistent rules". 1. Introduction In Shona, a Bantu language of Zimbabwe, the causative of a verb is formed in one of two ways. The more productive way is to add the verbal suffix -is- ~ -es-, as illustrated in (1) and (2), respectively) (1) a. -Bika 'cook' -Bikisa 'make (so.) cook' b. -Bata 'hold' -Batisa 'cause (so.) to hold' 1 The final -a in these and all other verbs cited is the unmarked inflectional suffix. It is replaced by -e in some marked inflections such as negative or subjunctive [Fortune 1982]. All forms are from the Zezuru dialect, spoken around Harare. Unless otherwise noted, examples and glosses are from Hannan [1984] and have been checked with Ms. Drusilla Chambati, a speaker ofZezuru Shona.
    [Show full text]
  • A Study of Korean Phonology and Morphology in a Derivational Approach
    Ecology of PF: A Study of Korean Phonology and Morphology in a Derivational Approach Inkie Chung, Ph.D. University of Connecticut, 2007 This dissertation studies the component of PF, i.e., morphology, phonology and phonetics, in Korean in a consistently derivational approach. Using phonological arguments and an acoustic phonetic study, chapter 2 identifies the surface syllable structure. It also recognizes the two syllable structure constraints: the complex onset constraint and the branching nucleus constraint. Using this syllable structure and these constraints, chapter 3 provides a comprehensive analysis of hiatus in verbal morphology and related issues within Calabrese’s (1995, 2002a) Dynamic Phonology framework. A limited number of actual outcomes for similar hiatus configurations show that the constraint-induced repairs are not finite but limited, and subject to the general principle of economy. It is shown that economy chooses shorter derivations over longer ones, when more than one derivation is possible and yield the same final outcome. It also show's that the hiatus constraint is a non-surface, cyclic constraint, operating once per morphophonological cycle. Furthermore, it argues that certain phonological operations must be described with the traditional rule-like formalism. It is also pointed out that the syllable structure constraints identified in chapter 2 are surface constraints. Chapters 4 and 5 provide a morphological study in Distributed Morphology (Bobaljik 2000, Halle and Marantz 1993, Marantz 2006) With verbal and adjectival roots exhibiting negative and honorific suppletion, these chapters identify the morphological structure of the inflected predicates (and hence phrase structure to a certain extent) in Korean. It identifies the paradoxical situation arising from the interaction of negative Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner.
    [Show full text]
  • Welsh Mutation and Strict Modularity © Õìth August Óþõé Florian Breit
    §Z f§u± WELSHMUTATIONANDSTRICTMODULARITY WELSHMUTATION & STRICTMODULARITY Florian Breit esis submitted in partial fullment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Department of Linguistics University College London Õìth August óþÕÉ Florian Breit: Welsh Mutation and Strict Modularity © Õìth August óþÕÉ Florian Breit £§h£Z «¶£u§ê«§: John Harris «¶f«oZ§í «¶£u§ê«§: Andrew Nevins is document was typeset using the typographical look-and-feel classicthesis Minion Pro (version ¦.ó) for LYX by André Miede. e type faces used are by Robert Slimbach and Euler by Hermann Zapf. For the greater glory of God, and in memory of Uncle Peter, who taught me the importance of love, the meaning of sorrow, and the struggles of happiness. Declaration I, Florian Breit, conrm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I conrm that this has been indicated in the thesis. London, Õìth August óþÕÉ ............................. Florian Breit vii Abstract e generative view of the language faculty is premised on the modularity of mind. In its most restrictive form, strict modularity, this predicts that phonological processes cannot be sensitive to syntactic information and vice versa. Initial Consonant Mutation in languages such as Welsh appear to falsify the strict modularity hypothesis in relation to the morphosyn- tax–phonology interface. is is because mutation involves the triggering of a phonologically regular process in a morphosyntactically determined environment. Consider the Welsh data in (Õ): (Õ) /kaT/ ‘cat’ (a) [i XaT] ‘her cat’ (b) [i kaT] ‘his cat’ e plosive–fricative alternation in (Õ) cannot be purely phonological in nature, because the phonological environment in (Õa) and (Õb) is identical.
    [Show full text]
  • Editon Consortium Journal of Literature and Linguistic Studies (ECJLLS)
    Editon Consortium Journal of Literature and Linguistic Studies (ECJLLS) ISSN: 2663-9297 Volume: 01 Issue: 03| Sep-Dec -2019 Received: 17.09.2019; Accepted 21.09.2019; Published: 30.10.2019 at www.editoncpublishing.org Chebet, et al., Editon Cons. J. Lit. Linguist. Stud. Double –Blind Peer Reviewed Journal An Interrogation of the Phonological Similarities between Somor and Aror Sub-Dialects of the Tugen Dialect in Kenya Nancy Chebet1; Phylis Bartoo2 1, 2 Egerton University, Kenya Corresponding Author Email: [email protected] ABSTRACT: The paper sets out to find the extent of the phonological similarities of Somor and Aror sub-dialects of the Tugen dialect. Phonological and semantic structures are many and varied and cannot be studied within such a limited time. Thus the study focused on tone and length in Tugen sub-dialects. The selected structures were epenthesis, vowel deletion, fusion, demonstratives, negation, possessives and definiteness. In semantics, the study focused on meaning in general. Stratified and random sampling procedures were used to get samples of Somor and Aror speakers from the population of those who practice in the selected domains in Torongo and Kapuskei locations of Baringo County. The data for the study was a Swadesh list of one hundred and fifty words and fifty sentences. These were drawn from the fields of education, domestic life, religion, health and administration. Data was collected by use of language performance test, which were recorded, on an audiotape. These words were written in gloss and transcribed using the IPA symbols. This was in preparation for the phonological and semantic analysis, which was done by using Natural Generative Phonology and Descriptive Linguistics.
    [Show full text]