Dementia 101: Know the DiErent Types of Dementia Not All Dementia Is Alzheimer's Disease

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dementia 101: Know the Di�Erent Types of Dementia Not All Dementia Is Alzheimer's Disease DECEMBER 2015/JANUARY 2016 BY MARISA COHEN Dementia 101: Know the Dioerent Types of Dementia Not all dementia is Alzheimer's disease. Knowing the dioerence aoects planning, management, and prognosis. Our primer breaks down the four most common types, as well as four not-so-common ones. Forgetting a name, misplacing keys, becoming momentarily confused—these can be byproducts of normal aging or the early symptoms of dementia. If it's normal aging, the names will be recalled, the keys will be found, and the confusion will lift. But for the 14 percent of Americans over age 70 who have some type of dementia, those names may never come back, and the memory loss and confusion become so debilitating that daily tasks like driving, shopping, and paying bills become increasingly difficult. As Americans continue to live longer, dementia is poised to become the biggest health challenge of the next few decades. The US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) expects the number of people living with Alzheimer's disease, the leading cause of dementia, to more than double from 5 million to 14 million by 2050. Still, about half of all cases of Alzheimer's—not to mention other types of debilitating brain disease, including non-Alzheimer's dementia—go undiagnosed, says John C. Morris, MD, FAAN, director of the Knight Alzheimer's Disease ILLUSTRATION BY BRIAN Research Center at Washington University in St. Louis. STAUFFER It's Be}er to Know Because no cure exists for most types of dementia, many patients and their families hesitate to have their suspicions confirmed. But experts recommend speaking to a neurologist as soon as memory or behavior problems occur. "I've found that people deal with disease better if they have more information about it," says Paul Barton Rosenberg, MD, associate director of the Memory and Alzheimer's Treatment Center at Johns Hopkins University. "Getting an accurate prognosis, which is different for different types of dementia, helps patients and caregivers plan better and allays their anxiety." And the earlier people get a diagnosis, the more likely they are to be eligible for research trials, Dr. Rosenberg says. (For more information about enrolling in trials, check out the Alzheimer's Association TrialMatch tool.) Follow this primer to understand the differences between Alzheimer's disease and other dementias, and to learn ways to manage the symptoms. Alzheimer's Disease CAUSES: The brain changes characteristic of Alzheimer's disease, which affects between 60 and 80 percent of all people with dementia, usually start years before any symptoms appear, when protein deposits start to form in the brain. Plaques, which are made up of a protein called beta-amyloid, clog the spaces between nerve cells; a little later in the process, tangles—twisted fibers of the tau protein— accumulate inside the cells. Both interfere with the ability of neurons to send and receive information and eventually cause brain cells to die. "Clinical problems typically begin with hippocampus-related short-term memory and learning, but gradually involve temporal, frontal, and occipital lobe functions, including language, executive function, and vision," says Dena Dubal, MD, PhD, an assistant professor of neurology and an endowed chair of aging and neurodegenerative disease at the University of California, San Francisco. In Alzheimer's disease, the cortex, the outer layer of the cerebrum, shrinks, and the ventricles, hollow cavities that hold cerebrospinal fluid, become enlarged and misshapen. ILLUSTRATIONS BY BIODIGITAL SYMPTOMS: The most noticeable early symptom is short-term memory loss. People may remember childhood stories but will forget conversations that happened five minutes earlier. They may also have difficulty retaining new information, asking the same questions repeatedly. Depression and apathy are also common at this point, as is confusion about dates and times. As the disease progresses, people start using poor judgment, have trouble communicating, or are confused and disoriented. By the end, people have trouble speaking, following a conversation, swallowing, and walking. CURRENT TREATMENTS: The disease cannot be stopped or reversed, but the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has approved two types of medications to treat symptoms: acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, which inhibit the breakdown of acetylcholine, a brain chemical involved in learning and memory; and memantine, which blocks the activity of the neurotransmitter glutamate, which at high levels can destroy nerve cells. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors are often prescribed in the earlier stages of the disease and include donepezil (Aricept), galantamine (Razadyne), and rivastigmine (Exelon). Memantine (Namenda) is used more in the later stages and may help with language, attention, and the ability to perform simple tasks. However, these drugs work for only about half the people who take them, and their effects last only six to 12 months. NEW TREATMENTS: Eli Lilly's phase 3 trials of a drug called solanezumab show that this antibody may slow the progress of very mild Alzheimer's dementia by binding the beta-amyloid protein and helping the body flush it out before it can develop into sticky plaque, although more research is needed. Trials are also underway with BACE inhibitors, which block beta-secretase 1, an enzyme that contributes to the production of beta-amyloid, and with other antibodies designed to clear accumulated toxic proteins from the brain. ONGOING RESEARCH: Breakthroughs in genetics, biomarkers, and brain imaging should soon make it easier to diagnose conditions like Alzheimer's in the earliest stages, even before symptoms begin, when these drugs may be most effective. Scientists are looking at ways to detect the earliest hints of Alzheimer's by measuring the level of tau and beta-amyloid proteins in the blood, urine, cerebrospinal fluid, and even in the lens and retina of the eye. They are also examining how positron emission tomography (PET) scans might be used to analyze brain volume and glucose levels to detect early changes. Meanwhile, geneticists are trying to identify genes that indicate a predisposition to Alzheimer's. AGE OF FIRST SYMPTOMS: Although changes in the brain can begin decades earlier, in the stage known as pre-clinical Alzheimer's disease, the majority of people don't exhibit symptoms until their mid-60s or later. "A small subgroup of people, less than 1 percent of all Alzheimer's patients, inherit a genetic mutation that causes the disease. For them, the disease can start in their 50s, 40s, 30s, and, rarely, in their 20s," says Dr. Morris. By age 85, there is almost a one-in-three chance that any given adult will have Alzheimer's, according to research from the Rush Institute for Healthy Aging. PROGNOSIS: The average person with Alzheimer's disease lives about eight years past his or her diagnosis. That time can be shortened to a few years or lengthened to as many as 20, depending on how old the person is at diagnosis and other health conditions. ASSOCIATION: Alzheimer's Association Lewy Body Dementia CAUSE: Described by Dr. Rosenberg as "a cross between Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease," this type of dementia occurs when protein deposits called Lewy bodies accumulate in nerve cells in the areas of the brain responsible for memory, motor control, and thinking. (When the Lewy bodies attack the brainstem first, causing stiffness, rigid muscles, and difficulty walking, the condition is diagnosed as Parkinson's disease.) "As the disease progresses and the protein bodies spread across the brain, people with Lewy body dementia begin to have Parkinsonian symptoms. And the majority of people with Parkinson's disease develop dementia," Dr. Dubal explains. Shrinkage occurs in the temporal and parietal lobes, which process sensory information; in the midbrain, which integrates sensory information with muscle movement; and in the cingulate gyrus, which regulates emotion and pain. BIODIGITAL SYMPTOMS: Visual hallucinations are common in people with Lewy body dementia. They may start talking to someone who isn't there or see specific images of animals or objects. Their moods and awareness may fluctuate rapidly. "A patient may seem coherent one moment and then be staring into space the next," says Dr. Dubal. People may also have sleep disturbances such as acting out dreams at night, says Jennifer Molano, MD, an assistant professor of neurology at the University of Cincinnati Academic Health Center. As the disease progresses, the physical symptoms of Parkinson's, such as rigid muscles and stiffness, can appear. TREATMENT: No medications have been developed specifically for Lewy body dementia, but neurologists often treat memory problems with the same acetylcholinesterase inhibitors used for Alzheimer's. They treat movement difficulties with levodopa, a standard treatment for Parkinson's disease. For people with severe, debilitating hallucinations, neurologists may prescribe antipsychotics —with caution. Antipsychotics can make cognitive symptoms worse, cause heavy sedation, or increase Parkinson's-like symptoms. In addition, the FDA has warned that both typical and atypical antipsychotics increase the risk of death in elderly people with dementia. Typical antipsychotics, including chlorpromazine (Thorazine) and haloperidol (Haldol), are not recommended for people with Lewy body dementia. Newer atypical antipsychotics, such as quetiapine (Seroquel) and clozapine (Clozaril), may be prescribed at low doses, but should be monitored very closely. AGE OF FIRST SYMPTOMS: Symptoms usually appear after age 60; the disease is more common in men than in women. PROGNOSIS: Survival is about eight years after diagnosis. ASSOCIATION: Lewy Body Dementia Association Vascular Dementias CAUSE: Vascular dementias include a number of disorders in which cerebrovascular disease prevents proper blood flow to the brain, depriving brain cells of oxygen and nutrients, which lead to cognitive decline. Damage to blood vessels can occur as a result of a major stroke, a series of tiny strokes, or chronic untreated high blood pressure. Damaged blood vessels can rupture, causing blood clots (inset) and a series of small strokes.
Recommended publications
  • Sleep, Memory, and Aging: Effects of Pre- and Post- Sleep Delays and Interference on Memory in Younger and Older Adults Michael Scullin Washington University in St
    Washington University in St. Louis Washington University Open Scholarship All Theses and Dissertations (ETDs) 1-1-2011 Sleep, Memory, and Aging: Effects of Pre- and Post- Sleep Delays and Interference on Memory in Younger and Older Adults Michael Scullin Washington University in St. Louis Follow this and additional works at: https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/etd Recommended Citation Scullin, Michael, "Sleep, Memory, and Aging: Effects of Pre- and Post-Sleep Delays and Interference on Memory in Younger and Older Adults" (2011). All Theses and Dissertations (ETDs). 640. https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/etd/640 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by Washington University Open Scholarship. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Theses and Dissertations (ETDs) by an authorized administrator of Washington University Open Scholarship. For more information, please contact [email protected]. WASHINGTON UNIVERSITY IN ST. LOUIS Department of Psychology Dissertation Examination Committee: Mark McDaniel, Chair Sandy Hale Larry Jacoby Henry Roediger, III Paul Shaw James Wertsch Sleep, Memory, and Aging: Effects of Pre- and Post-Sleep Delays and Interference on Memory in Younger and Older Adults by Michael K. Scullin A dissertation presented to the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Washington University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy December 2011 Saint Louis, Missouri Abstract The present research investigated the relationship between sleep and memory in younger and older adults. Previous research has demonstrated that during the deep sleep stage (i.e., slow wave sleep), recently learned memories are reactivated and consolidated in younger adults.
    [Show full text]
  • 2 Weeks to a Younger Brain Book Publishing
    2 WEEKS TO A YOUNGER BRAIN 2 WEEKS TO A YOUNGER BRAIN GARY SMALL, MD AND GIGI VORGAN www.humanixbooks.com Boca Raton, FL, USA Two Weeks to a Younger Brain © 2015 Humanix Books All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any other infor- mation storage and retrieval system, without written permission from the publisher. Interior: Ben Davis Index: Yvette M. Chin For information, contact: Humanix Books P.O. Box 20989 West Palm Beach, FL 33416 USA www.humanixbooks.com email: [email protected] Humanix Books is a division of Humanix Publishing, LLC. Its trademark, consisting of the words “Humanix Books” is registered with the US Patent and Trademark Of- fice, and in other countries. Disclaimer: The information presented in this book is meant to be used for general resource purposes only; it is not intended as specific medical advice for any individ- ual and should not substitute medical advice from a healthcare professional. If you have (or think you may have) a medical problem, speak to your doctor or a healthcare practitioner immediately about your risk and possible treatments. Do not engage in any therapy or treatment without consulting a medical professional. Printed in the United States of America and the United Kingdom. ISBN (Hardcover) 978-1-63006-030-5 ISBN (E-book) 978-1-63006-031-2 Library of Congress Control Number: 2014958068 Acknowledgments E ARE GRATEFUL TO the many volunteers and patients who Wparticipated in the research studies that inspired this book.
    [Show full text]
  • A CROSS-CULTURAL EXAMINATION of EMOTIONAL MEMORY PROCESSING: US Vs
    Journal of Integrated Social Sciences www.jiss.org, 2014 - 4(1): 1-17 Original Article: A CROSS-CULTURAL EXAMINATION OF EMOTIONAL MEMORY PROCESSING: US vs. AFGHANISTAN Frishta Sharifi, M.S. Mills College, USA Christie Chung, Ph.D. Mills College, USA Abstract A positivity effect where older adults remember positive information better than young adults has been documented widely in the memory literature. However, research on the generalizability of the positivity effect to non-western cultures is still in its infancy. The present study is the first in the field of memory to explore the differences in emotional memory patterns between the US and Afghan cultures. Due to the war in Afghanistan, we hypothesized that the Afghan older adults’ negative life experiences may reduce the effect of the positivity effect usually found in the US. In the present study, we tested young and older US and Afghan adults on an emotional picture memory task consisted of 30 pictures (10 positive, 10 negative, 10 neutral). They were later asked to recall these pictures using brief verbal descriptions. US older adults remembered significantly fewer negative pictures compared to US young adults, demonstrating a positivity effect in old age. Afghan adults, however, did not show an age-related positivity effect in memory as predicted. We discussed our results with the framework of current socio-emotional theories. Keywords: Aging, emotional memory, positivity effect, cross-cultural __________________ AUTHOR NOTE: Please address all correspondence to: Dr. Christie Chung, Mills College Psychology Department, 5000 MacArthur Blvd., Oakland, CA 94613. Email: [email protected] © 2014 Journal of Integrated Social Sciences Sharifi & Chung Emotional Memory: US vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive and Neuropsychological Aspects of Age-Associated Memory Dysfunction
    COGNITIVE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF AGE-ASSOCIATED MEMORY DYSFUNCTION A k a d e m is k a v h a n d l in g som för avläggande av filosofie doktorsexamen med vederbörligt tillstånd av rektorsämbetet vid Umeå universitet framlägges för offentlig granskning vid Psykologiska institutionen, Umeå Universitet, Seminarierum 2, fredagen den 24 januari 1992, klockan 10.15 AV Th o m a s K a r l s s o n Psykologiska institutionen, Umeå Universitet, Umeå COGNITIVE AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASPECTS OF AGE- ASSOCIATED MEMORY DYSFUNCTION BY Thom as K arlsson Doctoral Dissertation Department of Psychology, University of Umeå, Umeå, Sweden ABSTRACT Memory dysfunction is common in association with the course of normal aging. Memory dysfunction is also obligatory in age-associated neurological disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease. However, despite the ubiquitousness of age-related memory decline, several basic questions regarding this entity remain unanswered. The present investigation addressed two such questions: (1) Can individuals suffering from memory dysfunction due to aging and amnesia due to Alzheimer’s disease improve memory performance if contextual support is provided at the time of acquisition of to-be- remembered material or reproduction of to-be-remembered material? (2) Are memory deficits observed in ‘younger’ older adults similar to the deficits observed in ‘older’ elderly subjects, Alzheimer’s disease, and memory dysfunction in younger subjects? The outcome of this investigation suggests an affirmative answer to the first question. Given appropriate support at encoding and retrieval, even densely amnesic patients can improve their memory performance. As to the second question, a more complex pattern emerges.
    [Show full text]
  • Viewing Instructions Impact Emotional Memory Differently in Older and Younger Adults
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by The University of North Carolina at Greensboro Emery, L., & Hess, T.M. (2008). Viewing instructions impact emotional memory differently in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 2-12. (Mar 2008) Published by the American Psychological Association (ISSN: 1939-1498). Doi:10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.2 Viewing Instructions Impact Emotional Memory Differently in Older and Young Adults Lisa Emery, Thomas M. Hess ABSTRACT The current study examines how the instructions given during picture viewing impact age differences in incidental emotional memory. Previous research has suggested that older adults' memory may be better when they make emotional rather than perceptual evaluations of stimuli and that their memory may show a positivity bias in tasks with open-ended viewing instructions. Across two experiments, participants viewing photographs either received open-ended instructions or were asked to make emotionally focused (Experiment 1) or perceptually focused (Experiment 2) evaluations. Emotional evaluations had no impact on older adults' memory, whereas perceptual evaluations reduced older adults' recall of emotional, but not of neutral, pictures. Evidence for the positivity effect was sporadic and was not easier to detect with open- ended viewing instructions. These results suggest that older adults' memory is best when the material to be remembered is emotionally evocative and they are allowed to process it as such. The traditional focus in the study of memory and aging has been on determining which basic cognitive factors may account for age-related declines or differences in memory performance.
    [Show full text]
  • Aging and the Seven Sins of Memory Benton H
    Advances in Cell Aging and Gerontology Aging and the seven sins of memory Benton H. Pierce1,*, Jon S. Simons2 and Daniel L. Schacter1,* 1Department of Psychology, Harvard University, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland St., Cambridge, MA 02138 2Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, Alexandra House, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK Contents 1. Sins of omission 2 1.1. Transience 2 1.2. Absent-mindedness 4 1.3. Blocking 8 2. Sins of commission 12 2.1. Misattribution 12 2.2. Suggestibility 18 2.3. Bias 21 2.4. Persistence 24 2.5. Conclusions 26 Acknowledgements 29 References 29 Memory serves many different functions in everyday life, but none is more important than providing a link between the present and the past that allows us to re-visit previously experienced events, people, and places. This link becomes increasingly significant as we age: recollections of past experiences serve as the basis for a process of life review that assumes great importance to many older adults (Coleman, 1986; Schacter, 1996). Not surprising, then, a common worry among older adults is that forgetfulness, which may seem to be increasingly pervasive as years go by, will eventually result in the loss of the precious store of memories that has been built up over a lifetime. While much evidence suggests that various aspects of memory do decline with increasing age, it is clear that performance on some memory tasks remains relatively preserved (Naveh-Benjamin et al., 2001). *Corresponding author. Tel.: 617-495-3855; fax: 617-496-3122. E-mail address: [email protected] (D.L.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief History of Memory and Aging
    7 A Brief History of Memory and Aging AIMÉE M. SURPRENANT, TAMRA J. BIRETA, and LISA A. FARLEY n addition to his empirical work, Roddy Roediger has written tutorials, commentaries, encyclopedia articles, summaries of entire areas, textbooks, I practical guides to research, career guides, and guides to professional devel- opment. Throughout all of his work there is a clear sensitivity to history. He has written and given talks on the works of Ebbinghaus, Bartlett, Ballard, Deese, Nipher, and others, and, through his many editorships, he has supported the efforts of others in this direction. Even though he is no longer the editor, Psycho- nomic Bulletin and Review, a journal Roddy reinvented, is known to welcome submissions exploring the history of experimental psychology. If that were not enough, in recent years Roddy’s research program has added a new facet, exploring age-related differences in memory, particularly as it concerns false memory. Thus, it seemed appropriate to combine those interests and delve into the history of memory and aging at the conference in his honor. The purpose of this chapter, therefore, is to summarize the history of beliefs and research that serves as the foundation of current work on memory and aging. Although there have been in-depth articles on the history of life-span develop- mental psychology (Baltes, 1983), histories and surveys of the psychology of aging (Birren, 1961a, 1961b; Birren & Schroots, 2001; Granick, 1950; Pressey, Janney, & Kuhlen, 1939), and comprehensive considerations of the history of memory (e.g., Burnham, 1888; Murray, 1976; Yates, 1966), there is no published look into the history of aging and memory specifically.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Aging: Deficits, Beliefs, and Interventions Jane M
    University of Richmond UR Scholarship Repository Psychology Faculty Publications Psychology 2009 Memory Aging: Deficits, Beliefs, and Interventions Jane M. Berry University of Richmond, [email protected] Erin Hastings Robin West Courtney Lee John C. Cavanaugh Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.richmond.edu/psychology-faculty- publications Part of the Personality and Social Contexts Commons Recommended Citation Hastings, Erin, Jane M. Berry, Robin West, Courtney Lee, and John C. Cavanaugh. Aging in America, Volume 1, Psychological Aspects of Aging. Edited by John C. Cavanaugh and Christine K. Vavanaugh. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger/ABC-CLIO, 2010. 255-299. This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology at UR Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of UR Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. CHAPTER 11 MEMORY AGING: DEFICITS, BELIEFS, AND INTERVENTIONS Jane Berry, Erin Hastings, Robin West, Courtney Lee, and John C. Cavanaugh If any one faculty of our nature may be called more wonderful than the rest, I do think it is memory. There seems something more speak­ ingly incomprehensible in the powers, the failures, the inequalities of memory, than in any other of our intelligences. Jane Austen, Manifi,eld Park Of all mental faculties, memory is unique. It defines who we are and places our lives on a narrative continuum from birth to death. It helps to structure our days, it guides our daily tasks and goals, and it provides pleasurable interludes as we anticipate the future and recall the past.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Emotion on Item and Source Memory in Young and Older Adults
    JournalCognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2006, ??6 (4), (?), 306-322 ???-??? Effects of emotion on item and source memory in young and older adults PATRICK S. R. DAVIDSON, CRAIG P. MCFARLAND, and ELIZABETH L. GLISKY University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona Emotional experiences are easier to remember than neutral ones, but whether memory for all as- pects of an experience is improved by emotion remains unclear. Some researchers have argued that the influence of emotion on memory is different for item than for source information, whereas others have argued that emotion affects both similarly. Also, whether item and source memory are affected by emotion in older people in the same way as in young people is currently unclear. We examined item and source memory for emotional and neutral materials in young and older adults. Memory for emotional items was superior to memory for neutral items, whereas there was no difference in source memory. Overall, item and source memory were poorer in older people than in young people, but emotion seemed to have a similar effect on both age groups. Although emotional content was remem- bered better than neutral content, this benefit did not apply to source memory. However, varying the emotionality of the source (i.e., the voice in Experiment 3) improved memory for the source, and this effect was greater in young than in older people. Tone of voice had no effect on item memory in older people, but the effect was variable in the young and may depend on the extent to which the tone of voice moderates the interpretation of the content.
    [Show full text]
  • Nutrition Research Reviews (2015), 28, 167–180 Doi:10.1017/S0954422415000141 © the Authors 2015
    Nutrition Research Reviews (2015), 28, 167–180 doi:10.1017/S0954422415000141 © The Authors 2015 The role of nutrition on cognition and brain health in ageing: a targeted approach Jim M. Monti1*, Christopher J. Moulton1,2 and Neal J. Cohen2,3,4,5,6 1Abbott Nutrition, Columbus, OH, USA 2Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA 3Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA 4Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA 5Beckman Institute, Urbana, IL, USA 6Interdisciplinary Health Sciences Initiative, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA Abstract Animal experiments and cross-sectional or prospective longitudinal research in human subjects suggest a role for nutrition in cognitive ageing. However, data from randomised controlled trials (RCT) that seek causal evidence for the impact of nutrients on cognitive ageing in humans often produce null results. Given that RCT test hypotheses in a rigorous fashion, one conclusion could be that the positive effects of nutrition on the aged brain observed in other study designs are spurious. On the other hand, it may be that the design of many clinical trials conducted thus far has been less than optimal. In the present review, we offer a blueprint for a more targeted approach to the design of RCT in nutrition, cognition and brain health in ageing that focuses on three key areas. First, the role of nutrition is more suited for the maintenance of health rather than the treatment of disease. Second, given that cognitive functions and brain regions vary in their susceptibility to ageing, those that especially deteriorate in senescence should be focal points in evaluating the efficacy of an intervention.
    [Show full text]
  • Attention and Ageing
    Chapter 32 Attention and ageing Theodore P. Zanto and Adam Gazzaley Introduction The term ‘attention’ has been part of the English language for centuries, and the ‘state of giving heed’ has been recognized across cultures for millennia. Th e ubiquitous nature of this phenomenon has led to the notion that ‘[o] n attention itself, it is needless to discourse at length; its nature and conditions are familiar to every thoughtful student’ ( Munsell 1873 : 11), and more succinctly, ‘everyone knows what attention is’ ( James 1890 , vol. 1: 403). Yet James (1890) discusses diff erent types of attention, just as other chapters in this volume show that attention is more multifaceted than is appreciated in common parlance. Th us, attention is a set of cognitive processes, and transcends a single defi ni- tion or overarching theory ( Parasuraman 1998 ). Th e goal of this chapter is to review the various aspects of attention in the context of how they change in normal ageing. Th ere are several critical reviews on ageing and attention (e.g. Hartley 1992 ; Kok 2000 ; McDowd and Shaw 2000 ; Rogers 2000 ), some within the last few years ( Madden 2007 ; Kramer and Madden 2008 ; Drag and Bieliauskas 2010 ). However, as mentioned, there are many aspects of attention and thus many ways to consider how attention may be aff ected by ageing. Here, we will attempt to summarize and present a broad overview of the current state of attention and ageing research. Th e chapter is organized according to the following categories of attention: selective attention, sustained attention, divided attention, task-switching, and attentional capture.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Disorders
    Memory Disorders Alzheimer’s disease and other forms of dementia Social workers touch the lives of most individuals, either through Advanced practice nurses their own family’s experience, or that of a friend ●Speech, physical and occupational therapists or neighbor. Primary care providers are often on the front lines of a dementia diagnosis, serving as Our industry-leading model of care also includes clinical the first point of contact when a patient and their trials and research studies of new and novel medications family begin to suspect a memory disorder. Each and therapies. form of dementia carries its own symptoms and patterns of decline, making an accurate diagnosis Dementia’s dramatic effects are felt not only by the individual and careful disease management imperative in with the disease, but by their family as well. Our program the care of these patients. seeks to support families and caregivers, helping them understand the disease process and coordinate their loved The UVA Memory and Aging Care Clinic offers the expertise one’s care — every step of the way. and leading-edge diagnostic tools required to provide this level of care. It begins with an initial diagnostic evaluation that includes cognitive testing, neuroimaging, biomarkers Conditions Treated and a neurological exam. From there, we tailor our approach to each individual patient, utilizing our innovative We treat Alzheimer’s disease and every type of dementia care coordination model, which provides every dementia at our Memory and Aging Care Clinic. patient and their family one-on-one counseling and support through the entire disease process. This novel approach is Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) considered a model within the commonwealth of Virginia, as We diagnose and treat patients with both amnestic MCI, evidenced by the federal funding it has received.
    [Show full text]