<<

WARHOL CREDIT HERE

by Scott O. Lilienfeld, James M. Wood and Howard N. Garb

What’s Wrong with This

PICTURE PICTURE?

PHOTOGRAPHS BY JELLE WAGAANER

PSYCHOLOGISTS OFTEN USE THE FAMOUS RORSCHACH INKBLOT TEST AND RELATED TOOLS TO ASSESS AND MENTAL ILLNESS. BUT RESEARCH SHOWS THAT INSTRUMENTS ARE FREQUENTLY INEFFECTIVE FOR THOSE PURPOSES

www.sciam.com SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN 41 What if you were asked to describe images you saw in an inkblot or to invent a story for an ambiguous illustration— say, of a middle-aged man looking away from a woman who was grabbing his arm? To comply, you would draw on your own emotions, experiences, memories and imagination. You would, in short, project yourself into the images. Once you did that, many practicing psychologists would assert, trained evaluators could mine your musings to reach conclusions about your personality traits, unconscious needs and overall mental health.

But how correct would they be? The answer is important Standardization is needed because seemingly trivial differences because psychologists frequently apply such “projective” in- in the way an instrument is administered can a person’s struments (presenting people with ambiguous images, words responses. Norms provide a reference point for determining or objects) as components of mental assessments, and because when someone’s responses fall outside an acceptable range. the outcomes can profoundly affect the lives of the respondents. In the 1970s John E. Exner, Jr., then at Long Island Uni- The tools often serve, for instance, as aids in diagnosing men- versity, ostensibly corrected those problems in the early tal illness, in predicting whether convicts are likely to become Rorschach test by introducing what he called the Comprehen- violent after being paroled, in assessing the mental stability of sive System. This set of instructions established detailed rules parents engaged in custody battles, and in discerning whether for delivering the inkblot exam and for interpreting the re- children have been sexually molested. sponses, and it provided norms for children and adults. We recently reviewed a large body of research into how well In spite of the Comprehensive System’s current popularity, the projective methods work, concentrating on three of the most Rorschach generally falls short on two additional, and critical, cri- extensively used and best-studied instruments. Overall our find- teria: scoring reliability and validity. A tool possessing scoring re- ings are unsettling. liability yields similar results regardless of who tabulates and in- terprets the responses. A valid technique measures what it aims to Butterflies or Bison? measure: its results are consistent with those produced by other THE FAMOUS RORSCHACH inkblot test—which asks people to trustworthy instruments or are able to predict behavior, or both. describe what they see in a series of 10 inkblots—is by far the To understand the Rorschach’s scoring reliability problems, most popular of the projective methods, given to hundreds of it helps to know something about how reactions to the inkblots thousands, or perhaps millions, of people every year. The com- are interpreted. First, a psychologist rates the collected reactions ments that follow refer to the modern, rehabilitated version, on more than 100 characteristics, or variables. The evaluator not to the original construction, introduced in the 1920s by records, for instance, whether the person looked at whole blots Swiss psychiatrist . or just parts, notes whether the detected images were unusual The initial tool came under severe attack in the 1950s and or typical of most test takers, and indicates the aspects of the 1960s, in part because it lacked standardized procedures and inky swirls (such as form or color) that most determined what a set of norms (averaged results from the general population). the respondent saw.

42 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MAY 2001 Then he or she compiles the findings representative of the U.S. population into a psychological profile of the indi- and mistakenly make many adults and vidual. As part of that interpretive children seem maladjusted. For in- process, psychologists might conclude stance, in a 1999 study of 123 adult that focusing on minor details (such as volunteers at a California blood bank, stray splotches) in the blots, instead of on one in six had scores supposedly in- whole images, signals obsessiveness in a dicative of . patient and that seeing things in the white The inkblot results may be even spaces within the larger blots, instead of more misleading for minorities. Sever- in the inked areas, reveals a negative, al investigations have shown that RORSCHACH TEST contrary streak. scores for African-Americans, Native For the scoring of any variable to be Americans, Native Alaskans, Hispan- Wasted Ink? considered highly reliable, two different ics, and Central and South Americans assessors should be very likely to produce differ markedly from the norms. To- “It looks like two dinosaurs with huge heads similar ratings when examining any giv- gether the collected research raises se- and tiny bodies. They’re moving away from en person’s responses. Recent studies rious doubts about the use of the each other, looking over their shoulders. The demonstrate, however, that strong agree- Rorschach in the psychotherapy office black blob in the middle reminds me of a ment is achieved for only about half the and in the courtroom. spaceship.” characteristics examined by those who score Rorschach responses; evaluators Doubts about TAT Once deemed an “x-ray of the mind,” the might well come up with quite different ANOTHER PROJECTIVE TOOL—the Rorschach inkblot test remains the most ratings for the other variables. Thematic Apperception Test (TAT)— famous—and infamous—projective Equally troubling, analyses of the may be as problematic as the psychological technique. An examiner hands Rorschach’s validity indicate that it is Rorschach. This method asks respon- 10 symmetrical inkblots, one at a time in a set poorly equipped to identify most psychi- dents to formulate a story based on order, to a respondent, who says what each atric conditions—with the notable excep- ambiguous scenes in drawings on blot resembles. A few blots include colored tions of schizophrenia and other distur- cards. Among the 31 cards available to shapes, but most are black and gray—like bances marked by disordered thoughts, psychologists are ones depicting a boy artist ’s rendering above (the such as (manic-depres- contemplating a violin, a distraught actual blots cannot be published). sion). Despite claims by some Rorschach woman clutching an open door, and Responses to the inkblots purportedly proponents, the method does not consis- the man and woman who were men- reveal aspects of a person’s personality and tently detect depression, anxiety disorders tioned at the start of this article. One mental health. Advocates believe, for or psychopathic personality (a condition card, the epitome of ambiguity, is to- instance, that references to moving characterized by dishonesty, callousness tally blank. animals—such as the dinosaurs mentioned and lack of guilt). The TAT has been called “a clini- above—often indicate impulsiveness, Moreover, although psychologists cian’s delight and a statistician’s night- whereas allusions to a blot’s “blackness”—as frequently administer the Rorschach to mare,” in part because its administra- in a spaceship—often indicate depression. assess propensities toward violence, im- tion usually is not standardized: Swiss psychiatrist Hermann Rorschach pulsiveness and criminal behavior, most different clinicians present different probably got the idea of showing inkblots from research suggests it is not valid for these numbers and selections of cards to re- a European parlor game. The test debuted in purposes either. Similarly, no compelling spondents. Also, most clinicians inter- 1921 and reached high status by 1945. But a evidence supports its use for detecting pret the stories intuitively instead of critical backlash began taking shape in the sexual abuse in children. following a well-tested scoring proce- 1950s, as researchers found that Other problems have surfaced as dure. Indeed, a recent survey of nearly psychologists often interpreted the same well. Some evidence suggests that the 100 North American psychologists responses differently and that particular Rorschach norms meant to distinguish practicing in juvenile and family courts responses did not correlate well with specific mental health from mental illness are un- found that only 3 percent relied on a mental illnesses or personality traits. Today the methodological response to SCOTT O. LILIENFELD, JAMES M. WOOD and HOWARD N. GARB all conduct research on those weaknesses—the Comprehensive psychological assessment tools and recently collaborated on an extensive review of re- System (CS)—is used widely to score and search into projective instruments that was published by the American Psychological So- interpret Rorschach responses. But it has ciety (see “More to Explore,” on page 47). Lilienfeld and Wood are associate professors in been criticized on similar grounds. Moreover, the departments of at Emory University and the University of Texas at El Paso, respectively. Garb is a clinical psychologist at the Pittsburgh Veterans Administra- several recent findings indicate that the

THE AUTHORS tion Health Care system and the University of Pittsburgh and author of the book Studying Comprehensive System incorrectly labels

PHOTO CREDIT HERE the Clinician: Judgment Research and Psychological Assessment. many normal respondents as pathological.

www.sciam.com SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN 43 standardized TAT scoring system. Un- ty is frequently questionable as well; person’s of others (a proper- fortunately, some evidence suggests that studies that find positive results are often ty called “object relations”). But many clinicians who interpret the TAT intu- contradicted by other investigations. For times individuals who display a high itively are likely to overdiagnose psycho- example, several scoring systems have need to achieve do not score well on mea- logical disturbance. proved unable to differentiate normal in- sures of actual achievement, so the abili- Many standardized scoring systems dividuals from those who are psychotic ty of that variable to predict behavior are available for the TAT, but some of or depressed. may be limited. These scoring systems the more popular ones display weak A few standardized scoring systems currently lack norms and so are not yet “test-retest” reliability: they tend to yield for the TAT do appear to do a good job ready for use outside of research settings, inconsistent scores from one picture- of discerning certain aspects of person- but they merit further investigation. viewing session to the next. Their validi- ality—notably the need to achieve and a THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST Picture Imperfect The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), created by Harvard University psychiatrist Henry Murray and his student Christiana Morgan in the 1930s, is among the most commonly used projective measures. Examiners present individuals with a subset (typically five to 12) of 31 cards displaying pictures of ambiguous situations, mostly featuring people. Respondents then construct a story about each picture, describing the events that are occurring, what led up to them, what the characters are thinking and feeling, and what will happen later. Many variations of the TAT are in use, such as the Children’s Apperception Test, featuring animals interacting in ambiguous situations, and the Blacky Test, featuring the adventures of a black dog and its family. Psychologists have several ways of interpreting responses to the TAT. One promising approach—developed by Boston University psychologist Drew Westen—relies on a specific scoring system to assess people’s perceptions of others (“object relations”). According to that approach, if someone wove a story about an older woman plotting against a younger person in response to the image visible in the photograph at the right, the story would imply that the respondent tends to see malevolence in others—but only if similar themes turned up in stories told about other cards. Surveys show, however, that most practitioners do not use systematic scoring systems to interpret TAT stories, relying instead on their intuitions. Unfortunately, research indicates that such “impressionistic” interpretations of the TAT are of doubtful validity and may make the TAT a projective

exercise for both examiner and examinee. PHOTO CREDIT HERE

44 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MAY 2001 Faults in the Figures OTHER PROJECTIVE TOOLS: IN CONTRAST TO THE RORSCHACH and the TAT, which elicit reactions to exist- What’s the Score? ing images, a third projective approach instructs the people being evaluated to Psychologists have dozens of projective methods to choose from beyond the draw the pictures. A number of these in- Rorschach Test, the TAT and figure drawings. As the sampling below indicates, struments, such as the frequently applied some stand up well to the scrutiny of research, but many do not. Draw-a-Person Test, have people depict a human being; others have them draw houses or trees as well. Clinicians com- Hand Test monly interpret the sketches by relating Subjects say what hands pictured in various positions might be doing. This method is specific “signs”—such as features of the used to assess aggression, anxiety and other personality traits, but it has not been body or clothing—to facets of personali- well studied. ty or to particular psychological disor- ders. They might associate large eyes with paranoia, long ties with sexual ag- Handwriting Analysis Graphology gression, missing facial features with de- Interpreters rely on specific “signs” in a person’s handwriting to assess personality pression, and so on. characteristics. Though useless, the method is still used to screen prospective As is true of the other methods, the re- employees. search on drawing instruments gives rea- son for serious concern. In some studies, raters agree well on scoring, yet in others Lüscher Color Test the agreement is poor. What is worse, no People rank colored cards in order of preference to reveal personality traits. strong evidence supports the validity of Most studies find the technique to lack merit. the sign approach to interpretation; in other words, clinicians apparently have no grounds for linking specific signs to Play with Anatomically Correct Dolls particular personality traits or psychiatric Research finds that sexually abused children often play with the dolls’ genitalia; diagnoses. Nor is there consistent evi- however, that behavior is not diagnostic, because many nonabused children dence thats signs purportedly linked to do the same thing. child sexual abuse (such as tongues or genitalia) actually reveal a history of mo- lestation. The only positive result found Rosenzweig Picture Frustration Study repeatedly is that, as a group, people who After one cartoon character makes a provocative remark to another, a viewer draw human figures poorly have some- decides how the second character should respond. This instrument, featured in the what elevated rates of psychological dis- movie A Clockwork Orange, successfully predicts aggression in children. orders. On the other hand, studies show that clinicians are likely to attribute men- tal illness to many normal individuals Sentence Completion Test who lack artistic ability. Test takers finish a sentence such as, “If only I could . . .” Most versions are poorly Certain proponents argue that sign studied, but one developed by Jane Loevinger of Washington University is valid for approaches can be valid in the hands of measuring aspects of ego development, such as morality and empathy. seasoned experts. Yet one group of re- searchers reported that experts who ad- ministered the Draw-a-Person Test were Szondi Test less accurate than graduate students at From photographs of patients with various psychiatric disorders, viewers select distinguishing psychological normality the ones they like most and least. This technique assumes that the selections reveal from abnormality. something about the choosers’ needs, but research has discredited it.

Even when projective methods assess what they claim to measure, they RARELY ADD MUCH to information that can be obtained in other, more practical ways

www.sciam.com SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN 45 HUMAN FIGURE DRAWINGS Misleading Signs Psychologists have many projective drawing instruments at their disposal, but the Draw-a-Person Test is among the most popular—especially for assessing children and adolescents. A clinician asks the child to draw someone of the same sex and then someone of the opposite sex in any way that he or she wishes. (A variation involves asking the child to draw a person, house and tree.) Those who employ the test believe that the drawings reveal meaningful information about the child’s personality or mental health. In a sketch of a man, for example, small feet would supposedly indicate insecurity or instability—a small head, inadequacy. Large hands or teeth would be considered signs of aggression; short arms, a sign of shyness. And feminine features—such as eyelashes or darkly colored lips—would allegedly suggest sex-role confusion. Yet research consistently shows that such “signs” bear virtually no relation to personality or mental illness. Scientists have denounced these sign interpretations as “phrenology for the 20th century,” recalling the 19th-century of inferring people’s from the pattern of bumps on their skulls. Still, the sign approach remains widely used. Some psychologists even claim they can identify sexual abuse from certain key signs. For instance, in the child’s drawing at the right, alleged signs of abuse include a person older than the child, a partially unclothed body, a hand near the genitals, a hand hidden in a pocket, a large nose and a moustache. In reality, the connection between these signs and sexual abuse remains dubious, at best.

A few global scoring systems, which report, global interpretation of the OUR LITERATURE REVIEW, then, indicates are not based on signs, might be useful. In- Draw-a-Person Test correctly differenti- that, as usually administered, the stead of assuming a one-to-one correspon- ated 54 normal children and adolescents Rorschach, TAT and human figure draw- dence between a feature of a drawing and from those who were aggressive or ex- ings are useful in only very limited cir- a personality trait, psychologists who ap- tremely disobedient. The global ap- cumstances. The same is true for many ply such methods combine many aspects proach may work better than the sign other projective techniques, some of of the pictures to come up with a general approach because the act of aggregating which are described in the box on the pre- impression of a person’s adjustment. information can cancel out “noise” from ceding page. In a study of 52 children, a global variables that provide misleading or in- We have also found that even when scoring approach helped to distinguish complete information. the methods assess what they claim to normal individuals from those with measure, they tend to lack what psychol-

mood or anxiety disorders. In another What to Do? ogists call “incremental validity”: they PHOTO CREDIT HERE

46 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MAY 2001 rarely add much to information that can HOW OFTEN THE TO0LS ARE USED be obtained in other, more practical ways, such as by conducting interviews Popularity Poll or administering objective personality tests. (Objective tests seek answers to rel- atively clear-cut questions, such as, “I fre- In 1995 a survey of 412 randomly selected clinical psychologists in the American quently have thoughts of hurting my- Psychological Association asked how often they used various projective and self—true or false?”) This shortcoming of nonprojective assessment tools, including those listed below, in their practices. projective tools makes the costs in mon- Projective instruments present people with ambiguous pictures, words or things; ey and time hard to justify. nonprojective measures are less open-ended. The percentages who use the Some mental health professionals dis- projective methods might have declined slightly since 1995, but these techniques agree with our conclusions. They argue remain widely used. that projective tools have a long history of constructive use and, when administered PROJECTIVE USE ALWAYS USE AT LEAST and interpreted properly, can cut through TECHNIQUES OR FREQUENTLY OCCASIONALLY the veneer of respondents’ self-reports to Rorschach 43% 82% provide a picture of the deepest recesses of the mind. Critics have also asserted Human Figure Drawings 39% 80% that we have emphasized negative find- ings to the exclusion of positive ones. Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) 34% 82% Yet we remain confident in our con- clusions. In fact, as negative as our over- Sentence Completion Tests 34% 84% all findings are, they may paint an over- ly rosy picture of projective techniques CAT (Children’s version of the TAT) 6% 47% because of the so-called file drawer effect. As is well known, scientific journals are more likely to publish reports demon- NONPROJECTIVE USE ALWAYS USE AT LEAST strating that some procedure works than TECHNIQUES* OR FREQUENTLY OCCASIONALLY reports finding failure. Consequently, re- Weshler Adult Scale (WAIS) 59% 93% searchers often quietly file away their negative data, which may never again see Minnesota Multiphasic 58% 85% the light of day. Personality inventory-2 (MMPI-2) We find it troubling that psychologists commonly administer projective instru- Weschler Intelligence 69% 42% ments in situations for which their value Scale for Children (WISC) has not been well established by multiple studies; too many people can suffer if er- Beck Depression Inventory 71% 21% roneous diagnostic judgments influence therapy plans, custody rulings or criminal * Those listed are the most commonly used nonprojective tests for assessing adult IQ (WAIS), personality (MMPI-2), childhood IQ (WISC) and depression (Beck Depression Inventory). court decisions. Based on our findings, we SOURCE: “Contemporary Practice of Psychological Assessment by Clinical Psychologists,” by C.E. Watkins et al. in Professional Pscyhology: Research and Practice, strongly urge psychologists to curtail their Vo. 26, No. 1, pages 54–60; 1995 use of most projective techniques and, when they do select such instruments, to MORE TO EXPLORE limit themselves to scoring and interpret- The Rorschach: A Comprehensive System, Vol. 1: Basic Foundations. Third edition. John E. Exner. John Wiley ing the small number of variables that & Sons, 1993. have been proved trustworthy. The Comprehensive System for the Rorschach: A Critical Examination. James M. Wood, M. Teresa Nezworski Our results also offer a broader lesson and William J. Stejskal in Psychological Science, Vol. 7, No. Tk, pages 3–10; DateTK, 1996. for practicing clinicians, psychology stu- Studying the Clinician: Judgment Research and Psychological Assessment. Howard N. Garb. American Psy- dents and the public at large: even seasoned chological Association, 1998. professionals can be fooled by their intu- Evocative Images: The Thematic Apperception Test and the Art of Projection. Edited by Lon Gieser and Morris itions and their faith in tools that lack I. Stein. American Psychological Association, 1999. strong evidence of effectiveness. When a Projective Measures of Personality and : How Well Do They Work? Scott O. Lilienfeld in Skep- tical Inquirer, Vol. 23, No. TK, pages 32–39; DateTK 1999. substantial body of research demonstrates The Scientific Status of Projective Techniques. Scott O. Lilienfeld, James M. Wood and Howard N. Garb in Psy- that old intuitions are wrong, it is time to chological Science in the Public Interest, Vol. 1, No. TK, pages 27–66; DateTk, 2000. Available at www.psycho- adopt new ways of thinking. logicalscience.org/newsresearch/publications/journals/pspi1_2.html

www.sciam.com SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN 47