<<

ECCUMENT RESUME

ED 036 515 24 TE 001 706 AUTHOR BLOUNT, NATHAN S.; AND OTHERS TITLE THE EERECT OF A STUDY OF GRAMMAR ON THE WRITING OE EIGHTH-GRADE STUTENTS. REPOFT FEOM THE INDIVIDUALLY GUIDED INSTRUCTION II ENGLISH LANGUAGE, COMPOSTTION, AND LITERATURE EllCjECT. iNSTITUTICN WISCONSIN UNIV., MADISON. RESEARCH AND DEVELLEMENT CENTER FOR. COGNITIVE LEARNING. SPCNS AGENCY OFFICE CI -EDUCATION (JHEw), WASHINGTON, D.C.BUREAU OF RESEARCH. EEEOET NO 1E-69 EUEEAU NO BR-5-0'416 PUE LATE DEC 68 CONTRACT OEC-5-10-154 NOTE 51F.

LEES PEICE ELES PRICE MF-s0.25 DESCRIPTORS ABLE STUDENTS, APPLIED LINGUiSliCS,*COMPOSITION SKILLS (LITERARY), *ENGLISH INSTFUCTION,GRADE 8, *GRAMMAR, LINEAR PROGRAMING, *PROGRAMEDINSTRUCTICN, PEOGEAMEE, EATIELIALS, SENTINCE S1RUCTUrE,STRUCTURAL GRAMLAR, *STUDENT IMPROVEMENT, TRANSFORMATION GENERATIVE GRAMMAR AESTRALT THIS STUDY PROPOSED (1) IC VERIFY THAI STUDENTS CF NORMAL ABILITY COULD LEAFN STRUCTURAL ANDTRANSFORMATIONAL GRAMMAR CONCEPTS PRESENTED IN THE FORMAT OF LINEARPROGRAMING, AND (2)TO TEST WHETHER THE STUDENTS WCULD APPLY THECONCEPTS IN COMPOSITIONS SO THAT THEIR WRITING SHOWED PROGRESS TOWARDMATURITY. PARTICIPATING IN THE EXPERIMENT WERE 207 EIGHTH-GRADE STUDENTSIN TWO SCHOOLS. CNE HUNDRED STUDENTS STUDIED CNE PROGRAMEDLESSON PLR LAY FOR 22 DAYS; THE ilEST OF THE STUDENTS SERVED AS THE CCNTRCLGROUP. CCMFAEISON OF SCORES ON OBJECTIVE PRE- AND POST-TESTS SHOWEDEXPERIMENTAL STUDENTS DEMONSTRATING A SIGNIFICANT GiRASP OF THE CONCEPTSAND AN INCREASE IN SENTENCE-COMBINING SKILLS..CTHE.r RESULTS SHOWED E EEALES SCORING HIGrIER THAN MALES, ABLE STUDENTS LEARNINGMORE THAN THE LESS AE I5 AND STUDENTS LEARNING MORE IN ONE SCHOOL(UPPER-M_ IDLLE ECONOMIC LEVEL) THAN IN THE LOWEE-MILDLE SCHCOL.FOE: THE SECCND PART OF THE STUDY, 1000-WCFD WRITING SAMPLES WEREOBTAINED FROM ALL STUDENTS BEFORE AND AFTER THE EXPERIMENT, AND THEPAPERS OF 114 STUDENTS WERT RANDOMLY SELECTED FOE EXAMINATION. RESULTSSHOWED THAT LXPLEIMENTAL STUDENTS INCREASED THEIR USE CI SUECFLINATICNAND, FURTHERMORE, THAT STUDENTS IN ThE UPPER-MIDDLE SCHOOL ADVANCEDMORE THAN THE OTHER SCHOOL'S STUDENTS IN THEIR USE OF SUBORDINATION.(THIRTY-THREE STATISTICAL TABLES ARE INCLUDED.) (AUTHCF/LH) WELFARE U.S. DEPARTMENT OfHEALTH, EDUCATION & OFFICE OF EDUCATION REPRODUCED EXACTLY ASRECEIVED FROM THE THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN POINTS OF VIEW OROPINIONS PERSON OR ORGANIZATIONORIGINATING IT. REPRESENT OFFICIAL. OFFICEOF EDUCATION STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY

POSITION OR POLICY.

Technical Report No. 69

THE EFFECT OF A STUDY OF GRAMMAR ON THE WRITING

OF EIGHTH-GRADE STUDENTS

By Nathan S. Blount, Wayne C. Fredrick, andShelby L. Johnson

Report from the Individually Guided Instruction in English Language, Composition, and Literature Project Nathan S. Blount and Lester S. Golub, Principal Investigators

Wisconsin Research and Development Center for Cognitive Learning The University of Wisconsin Madison, Wisconsin

December 1968

O

4ZI The research reported herein was performed pursuant to a contractwith the United States Office of Education, Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, underthe provisions of the Cooperative Research Program.

Center No. C-03 / Contract OE 5-10-154 STATEMENT OF FOCUS

The Wisconsin Research and Development Center for CognitiveLearning focuses on contributing to a better understanding of cognitivelearning by children and youth and to the improvement of related educationalpractices. The strategy for research and development iscomprehensive.It includes basic research to generate new knowledge about theconditions and processes of learning and about the processes of instruction, and thesubsequent de,elop- ment of research-based instructional materials, manyof which are designed for use by teachers and others for use bystudents. These materials are tested and refined in school settings.Throughout these operations behavioral scientists, curriculum experts, academic scholars, and school peopleinteract, insuring that the results of Center activities are based soundly onknowledge of subject matter and cognitive learning and that they areapplied to the improvement of educational practice. This Technical Report is from the Individually GuidedInstruction in English Language; Composition, and Literature Project in Program2.General objectives of the Program are to establish rationale and strategy fordeveloping instructional systems, to identify sequences of concepts and cognitiveskills, to develop assessment procedures for those concepts and skills, toidentify or develop in- structional materials associated with the concepts and cognitiveskills, and to generate new knowledge about instructional procedures.Contributing to these Program objectives, the long-range objective of the English Projectis to install and test materials for individually guided instruction inlanguage, composition, and literature.Prerequisite activities include formulating behavioral objectives for students and teachers, based on a content and conceptsoutline, and develop- ing measurement instruments related to the behavioralobjectives. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Grateful acknowledgement from the authors to the following people who assisted in some aspect of this project: Research AssistantsRobert Trezevant, Russell L. Carey, James G. Ramsay, Louis Pingel, Paula Miner, Lesley Northup, and Sherry Lane; and the following people in the Manitowoc Public Schools: Teachers--Lora Froehlich and David Olson; PrincipalsRobert Kupper and Dayton Love lien; Directors of Research Norris M. Sanders and Burton L. Grover; Assistant AdminstratorVernon Childs; District AdministratorCharles E. Jones; and, ofcourse, the students.

iv I

CONTENTS

page

List of Tables vii

Abstract ix

I. Introduction 1 Statement of the Problem 1 Related Research 1

II. Procedures 4 Subjects, Teachers, and Schools 4 Treatment 4 Classroom Methods 5 Format of Programed Lessons 5 Content of Programed Lessons 5 Format of Worksheet 6 Format of Summary Sheet 6 Program Measures and Questionnaire 6 Collection of Writing Samples 6

III. Objective Tests and Program Measures 8 Objective Tests 8 Description 8 Analysis 8 Pretest Results 9 Posttest Results 9 Posttest I 10 Posttest II 11 Posttests I and II Combined 11 Posttest III 12 Test Item Analysis 12 Program Measures 13 Errors 13 Time 15 Questionnaire 15

IV. Writing Samples 18 Procedures 18 Specifications for Writing Samples 18 Exclusion of Certain Kinds of Sentences 18 Working Definitions 18 Identifying and Tabulating T-units and Clauses 19 Measures of Syntactic Growth 21 Training of Raters 21 page

Analysis 22 Results 22 Adjective Clauses 22 Adverb Clauses 23 Noun Clauses 23 Clauses of Comparison 25 Other Tabulated Structures 25 Clause Length 25 Clauses per T-unit 26 T-unit Length 26 T-units per Sentence 27 Sentence Length 28 Total of All Subordinate Clauses 29

V. Discussion and Summary 31 Summary 31 Conclusions 33 Appendix A: General Outline of Concepts in Program 37

Appendix B:Example Worksheet 39 Appendix C: Summary Sheet for Lesson 1 40

Appendix D:Student Questionnaire 41

Bibliography 43

vi LIST OF TABLES

Table page

1 Kuhlmann-Anderson 1Q Scores and Number of Ss for Each Group 5

2 The Observed Mean Number of Items Correct and the Standard Deviations on the Pretests and Posttest, 9

3 Score on Posttest I Using Pretest I as the Covariate 10

4 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for Posttest I Using Pretest I as the Covariate

5 Score on Posttest II Using Pretest II as the Covariate

6 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for Posttest II Using Pretest II as the Covariate 12

7 Analysis of Covariance of the Sum of Posttests I and II Using KAT Score and Pretest I and II Scores as Covariates 12

8 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for the Sum of Posttest I and II Using KAT Score and Pretest I and II as Covariates 13

9 Score on Posttest III Using Pretest III as the Covariate 13

10 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for Posttest III Using Pretest III as the Covariate 14

11 Percent of Items with a Discrimination Index Significant a.t .025 14 12 Number of Errors, Errors per 100 Responses, and Time for Each Lesson 15 13 Responses to the Question, To what degree doyou feet that what you learned about the following things will be helpful to you in the future? 16

14 Students' Ranking of the Difficulty of Seven Concepts 16

15 Student Rank of Four Modes of Presentation 17

vii Table page

16 The Observed Means of Nine Writing Measures Obtained from the 1 000-Word Prewriting and Postwriting Samples of 72 Ss 23

17 Subordinate Adjective Clauses in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate 24

18 The Means for the Writing Measures for Ss in EachSchool 24

19 Subordinate Adverb Clauses in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate 24

20 Subordinate Noun Clauses in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by IC) 25

21 Subordinate Noun Clauses in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by School 25

22 Clauses of Comparison in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate 26

23 Clause Length in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by IQ 26

24 Clause Length in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by School 27

25 Clauses per T-unit in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by IQ 27

26 Clauses per T-unit in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment bySchool 27

27 T-unit Length in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by IQ 28

28 T-unit Length in the Postwriting Using Prewriting us the CovariateTreatment by School )8

29 T-units per Sentence in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by IQ

30 T-units per Sentence in the Postwritina Using Prewriting, as the CovariateTreatment bySchool

31 Sentence Length in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the CovariateTreatment by IQ 29

32 Sentence Length in the Postwriting Using: i'rewriting as the CovariateTreatment by School 32

33 The Sum of the Eleven Clause Structures in the PostwrItin,: Using Prewriting and IQ as the Covariates 30

viii ABSTRACT

207 eighth-grade students in two schools participated in a test of pro- gran.ed materials in structural and transformational grammar. The materials were 22 lessons that were studied by 100 of the Ss at the rate of one lesson per day. Some experimental Ss received exercises in applying the content of the lessons. The performances of the 100 experimental Ss and 107 con- trol Ss were compared on pretests and posttests .These comparisons showed significant learning of concepts by the experimental Ss and an increase in their sentence-combining skills.Other test results showed females scoring higher than males, able students learning more than the less able, and more learning in School A than in School B.The regression between pretest and posttest scores was high and significant but the regression between IQ and posttest score was not significant.The exercises were not a significant factor on the tests. 1000-word writing samples were obtained from all Ss prior to and after the experiment.Statistical comparisons were based on the tabulations of 114 randomly selected Ss. These comparisons showed that while control and experimental Ss were equal on prewriting, the treatment increased the use of subordination by the experimental Ss on the postwriting.Further analyses showed that Ss in School. A had advanced in their use of subordina- tion more than Ss in School B.No sex differences were present in the writing measures and only minimal differences due to intellectual ability were seen. Prewriting measures such as clause length, T-unit length, sentence length, clauses per T-unit, and T-units per sentence were significant covariates on postwriting while IQ and frequency data on types of clauses wore not. The Ss spent about 20 minutes on each lesson and the error rate in re- sponding to the programed frames was low. The Ss judged the difficulty of the content appropriate and thought that the lessons were interesting and helpful. They rated the clues for recognizing sentence structures especially useful. INTRODUCTION

STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM ducing errors or improving the overall quality of written themes.Blake (1964) showed that The purpose of the present study was two- structural grammar was relatively ineffective fold.The first task was to verify that students in teaching punctuation,Schuster (1961) and of normal ability in a usual classroom setting (1961) found structural grammar at least could and would learn concepts of structural as effective as traditional grammar when the and transformational grammars when these con- criteria were verbal abilities measured by ob- cepts were presented in the format of linear jective tests.But O'Donnell (1963) pointed programing. The second task was to test out that neither approach correlated very highly whether students who have learned concepts with excellence in written composition. of structural and transformational grammars The ineffectiveness of these grammars seems apply these concepts in compositions so that largely due to a narrow and incorrect view of their writing would show detectable progress the objectives. What was needed was a more toward maturity. relevant specification of the kinds of changes in writing that were desired.If the only ob- RELATED RESEARCH jective was reduction of errors, this could be achieved by a simple reduction in the amount The question of whether the study of gram- of writing, a step which some children appar- mar improves writing ability has received much ently take under pressure to write perfectly. attention (c.f. reviews of research by Braddock, The combination of the new grammars, includ- Lloyd-Jones, and Schoer, 1963; and Meckel, ing structural and transformational approaches, 1963). The question as posed, however, in- and the specification of indexes which measure volves two related issues. One must ask, maturity in writing has led to the goal that in- Which grammar does one teach? and secondly, struction in grammar can operate constructively To what end is it taught, that is, what is to develop writing behavior which becomes pro- meant by improved writing?Prior to the re- gressively more like that of mature adults. cent availability of "new" grammars, tradi- Studies based on structural grammar as a tional grammar was taught for its editorial means of measuring progressive changes toward function in reducing errors. However, research maturity showed that the use of certain sentence showed that other methods were usually supe- patterns (NP + V, NP + V + NP, NP + be + adj, rior to the usage prescriptions of the traditional and NP + be + NP) increased with increasing grammar. Among these superior methods were age (Sam and Stine, 1965); that the variety of a thought approach (Frogner, 1939), incidental sentence patterns increased with age (Strick- teaching as errors appeared (Milligan, 1939; land, 1962; Riling, 1965); that children of Low Kraus, 1959) and a direct method (Harris, intelligence never used some of the structures 1962). The implication in some of the studies available to other children. (Riling, 1965; Loban, was that the error oriented approach depressed 1963); that the number of words per "sentence" the spontaneous production of complex increased with increasing age (Riling, 1965; (Milligan, 1939; Harris, 1962). Loban, 1963); and that the use of subordinate Other experimenters tried to answer the clauses increased with both age and intelligence general question of the effect of grammar study (Loban, 1963).Studies based on transforma- by using other grammars. Johnson (1960), tional grammar revealed that the use of certain however, found that a structural approach was transforms increased with age, while nongram- no different from a traditional approach in re- matical transformations decreased (Menyuk,

1 1963, 1964), and that there appeared to be ages Bateman and Zidonis (1966) reported that at which transform rules were first overgeneral- knowledge of generative-transformational ized before eventually being applied properly grammar increased the proportion of well formed (Menyuk, 1964). sentences , decreased the number of malformed A significant mapping of the developmental sentences and errors in applying transformation use of grammatical structures was undertaken rules, and resulted in increased structural by hunt (1965), who attempted to find the most complexity of sentences with no loss in gram- objective, systematic measure of writing char- maticality. These conclusions were based acteristics at various grade levels.Hunt was upon the writing samples of 41 ninth and tenth dissatisfied with sentence length because young graders in a university-affiliated experimental children tend to use many "ands" and not enough high school. Though Lhe :,ample was small periods, tendencies which result in long "sen- and probably atypical, the data on error reduc- tences" that are not equal to the long sentences tion and proportion of well formed sentences of mature writers.hunt developed other meas- seemed impressive.l'or example, after the ures which he rightly felt were much superior two-year treatment the experimental Ss reduced to sentence length, namely T-unit length, num- their error rate by 81compared to a 47 / re- ber of short T-units, and clauses per T- -unit. duction for the control group, which studied (T-units were defined by Hunt as the shortest no grammar. The experimental group produced word groups grammatically capable of being structurally correct sentences 88/., of the time terminated with a capital letter and a period.) compared to 63/, for the control group. How- T-unit length, for example, increased from 8.6 ever, other data obtainable from the report words at Grade 4 to 11.5 words at Grade 8 and were negative. The experimental group, which 14.4 words in Grade 1 2.Adult writers averaged prior to the experiment produced a writing sam- 20.3 words per T-unit.hunt showed that the ple of 20,209 words, dropped to 17,1 83 words increase in T-unit length resulted from two fac- following the study of generative grammar. Ihe tors, an increase in subordinate clause length, control group during the same time went from which develops early, and an increase in the 15,1 29 words to 18,302 words.Since the writ- number of subordinate clauses, which comes ing samples were obtained under '.he same con- later.The increase in frequency came espe- ditions, the decline in quantity of writing was cially in the use of adjective clauses (They a noteworthy effect attributable to the experi- double in frequency from Grade 4 to Grade 12.), mental treatment.Although the sentences of although the use of movable adverb clauses and experimental Ss were transformationally com- certain noun clauses also increased. The in- plex (in the case of one obviously precocious crease in clause length was from such factors experimental S, 70 transformational operations as use of adjectives, genitives, prepositional were detected in an average sentence, while phrases,infinitives,participles to modify nouns, prior to the treatment his average complexity auxiliary verbs, near-clause nominals, and level was six such operations) ,the frequency coordination within T-units. of transformations increased only one for the The measures developed by Hunt have been experimental Ss (from 5.39 to 6.30 transforma- tested on other groups of eighth and twelfth tions per sentence) and more than two for the graders( Blount, Johnson, and Fredrick, in control Ss (from 4.80 to 7.47) .Thus, it ap- press) and have been extended to samples of pears that well formed, errorless, complex children in Grades 1<,1,2, 3, 5, and 7 for transformations were obtained at the cost of a both oral and written discourse (O'Donnell, reduction in quantity of writing and a decrease Griffin, and Norris, 1967) .['hese studies each in the use of transformations within a sentence. verified thatI' -unit length was a revealing and As in the studies of Milligan (1939) and Harris objective measure of writing maturity.The (19 62), the emphasis upon correctness scorns latter study demonstrated that the numiier of to have suppressed spontaneous writing. sentence-combining transformations increased Blount., Klatismoier, Johnson, Fredrick, and significantly with grade level in both speech Ramsay (1967) showed that eighth-grade stu- and writing and was closely -ipproximoted dents would, if paid, learn a significant amount the more easily computedI' -unit length. of structural and transformational grammar from The waft of Hunt, which was based on trans- series of lessons prepared in programed formational gramAnar, was instrumental in pro- format.The study suggested that writing ex- viding the present authors with suitable criteria ercises, which allowed Ss to apply the gram- for detecting changes in writing behavior re- mars in the production of nov,.1 sentences, sulting tron'. ;:nowh,dge of structurn1 and trans- would facilitate nerformance in productive formational grammar.()Lher studies have shown situations classroom composition. that these grammars could be taught under The 1967 led directly to the present ex- 1 UF)rutory-schry)1 e.ind if ions . periment which tests the effect of the programed from sets lessons, with and without writingexercises, in of fully formed, complex sentences maturity is de- of kernel-like statements ,were apparently producing mature writing where elaborate fined on the basis of Hunt's workwith T-units. successful in causing students to after the data and diversify the structureswithin T-units, Two studies, which appeared rela- for the present research hadbeen collected, though the gain in length of T-units was concepts of struc- tively moderate. The second study,(Gale ,19681 suggested that teaching the between a tural and transformational grammaris feasible reported no significant differences and will transfer to writing behavior.Mellon traditional and structural-transformational ap- (1967) showed that a series of lessonsand proach on measures of T-unitlength and num- sentence-combining exercises increased ber of malformed sentences.However, the relative experimental group increasedsignificantly the markedly the number of nominal and Gale clauses and phrases occurring inthe written complexity of its well formed sentences. The further reported that the fifthgraders of her compositions of seventh-grade students. relatively easily. exercises, which allowed thepseudoproduction study understood the concepts PROCEDURES

Students and teachers in eight eighth-grade (KAT), weresupplied by the schools.For some English classes participated in this study dur- statistical analyses the Ss were divided into ing the first semester of the school year 1966- three ability levelshigh, medium, and low. 1967. There were two experimental groups and This was done by ranking the students on the one control group.Four kinds of data were basis of their KAT scores and then assigning collected from the experimental Ss;2000 the top one-third to the high ability group, the word writing samples from each student, scores middle one-third to the medium ability group, on objective tests, tabulations of errorsand and the bottom one-third to the low ability group. time on the 22 program lessons, and the re- The ranges of IQ for low, medium, and high sponses to a questionnaire of opinionstoward were: 90-106, 107-118, and 119-145.The the treatments. The control group wrote the mean IQ for all students was 113. 2000-word writing samples and took the objec- The data from 35 students were discarded for tive tests. the following reasons:22 had no KAT score on record or had taken an intelligence test other SUIJECTS, TEACHERS, than the KAT; 11 had KAT scores below 90, ex- AND SCHOOLS ceeding the lower limit of the range set by the investigators; and 2 had incomplete experimental The Ss were 242 students in the eighth test data.Of the remaining 207 Ss, 100 were grade from two junior high schools of a Mid- in the experimental groups and 107 were in the western city which had a population of about_ control groups. The mcan IQ scores of the 35,000.Four intact English classes from each various groups and the numbers of Ss in each school, two classes in the morning and two in group are shown in Table 1. the afternoon, provided a cross section of students representative of the entire schools. TREATMENT Since the two schools were located in areas of the city corresponding to different economic The experimental treatment (W and WO) con- levels, (i.e., School A was upper-middle, sisted of 22 programed lessons in English syntax School B lower-middle) school was considered and morphology. A summary sheet, reviewing a factor in some statistical analyses.The two selected concepts, followed each lesson. One teachers participating in this study, Teacher hundred Ss studied these lessons at the rate A in School A and Teacher B in School B, each of one lesson per day for 22 days. The 52 Ss conducted two experimental classes and two receiving Treatment W studied the 22 programed control classes. lessons and, in addition, completed a worksheet The experimental classes received programed at the end each lesson. This worksheet con- instruction in structural and transformational sisted of e'..,rcises which were handed in, cor- grammar. One-half of each experimentalclass rected, and returned to the student. Croup WO received Treatment W, which was programed did the lessons and summary sheet but did not lessons plus writing exercises , and one-half receive the worksheet. received Treatment WO, which was the same The control croup ((::) received no instruction programed lessons but without writing exer- in grammar or composition; the teachers agreed cises. The control groups (C) did not study to delay ciny teaching of grammar or composii ion grammar during the experiment. until the completion of the experiment. The Ss' IQ scores on the Kuhlmann-Anderson School A, control classes studied litc,r,1,:re; Intelligence Test, Seventh Edition, Booklet EF in School B, they studied speaking. Table 1 Kuhlmann-Anderson IQ Scores and :\,:umber of Ss for Each riroup

School A School B Group T.\.. (11(? I'C1:-:alc All Ss

Treatment VV IQ 109 117 11'3 115 114 No. of Ss 11 15 13 13 52 Treatment WO IQ 103 117 115 112 11? No. of Ss 12 11 10 48 Control IQ 109 116 114 114 113 No. of Ss 24 25 31 27 107

The experiment required a total of 26 school difficulty of the frames was obtained since stu- days, including time for the lessons and re- dents did riot erase incorrect answers but were lated tests.Four separate tests were given: asked to draw a line through them and write the a pretest before Lesson 1,a posttest after correct answer immediately next to the one Lesson 10, another pretest before Lt -:sson 11, crossed out. and a final posttest after T.esson 22. Students who were in the group receiving worksheets (W) completed these in class and Classroom Methods handed them to the teacher at the end of the hour. Those students who did not have the At the beginning of each class period, the worksheets (WO) were allowed to spend the clay's lesson was distributed to the experimental remaining time studying. Teachers read the group.In addition to the programed material, worksheets from each class as soon as possible, one-half of each class also received the work- wrote favorable and encouraging comments on sheet at this time.Lessons were stored in the worksheets, and helped the student under- cumulative folders which were available to the stand his errors.Before working through each students for reference and review.Students lesson, Croup W students had the opportunity who were absent made up work when they re- to look over the worksheets from the preceding turned and were given school time to work in day. study halls if needed. These conditions per- mitted nearly simultaneous testing for all Format of Programed Lessons classes in both schools. The programing technique used in the lessons Students proceeded at their individual rates. was essentially "linear," with information pre- Teachers were advised not to offer help with sented in a series of small, carefully sequenced concepts and processes taught in the lessons. frames.Each frame offered the student a chance However, teachers could help students with to respond either to a question, a blank, or a possible difficulties in reading or pronouncing multiple-choice format.Correct responses words. Teachers were told, "If the word is in appeared in a separate column immediately to a sample sentence, you may help the student the right of the frames.Students wrote their with meaning.If the word pertains to one of own responses on a separate answer sheet, the concepts in the program, and if the stu- which served as a shield for covering the printed dents are unclear about its meaning, you may responses. The average number of frames per refer them back to the frame or lesson where lesson was 60, although the exact length of a it was introduced." Teachers were free to give single lesson varied from 50 to 70 frames. No as much help as necessary with the mechanics lesson, however, demanded more than one class of the programed approach. period of the student's time In working with the programed lessons, stu- dents were instructed to cover the correct Content of Programed Lessons printed response with their answer sheet and The 22 programed lessons had been developed to reveal this response as soon as they had and tested previously. The pilot-testing of the written their own.Information on the relative lessons was by 8 eighth graders who worked

5 through the lessons in individual sessions with the concepts and processes learned in each the investigators and assistants. Wherever programed lesson.Labeling and parsing of these students experienced difficulty in learning sentence parts were avoided.Instead, some the materials, frames and entire lessons were exercises asked the student to create his own revised.In the summer of 1966, the lessons sentences or parts of sentences.Other exer- were again tested with 59 eighth graders who cises required the combination of two or n.ore participated in an experiment for 2 hours each sentences using a specified transformation, or morning for 5 weeks (Blount: et al., 1967). the completion of a sentence using a partie:lar From the 1966 field-testing, the investigators structure.In the last ten lessons the work- learned that the length of the lessons was suit- sheets included explanations of how the stu- able for practical classroom use and that the dent could apply his knowledge to his own lessons held the interest of both the fast and writing and showed that choices were open to slow student.Also, 93`7, of the students indi- him when writing.Examples of the kinds of cated that they did not consider the vocabulary exey .:1ses included in the worksheets are shown of the lessons difficult; 86A said they preferred in Appendix B. working with the programed approach rather than with a standard textbook format.The average Format of Summary Sheet amount of time required for each lesson was 21 Following each lesson Groups W and WC) minutes; times for individuals ranged from 12 received a summary sheet reviewing the main to 35 minutes. Of the responses made to the concepts in the lesson.Grammatical terms frames, 7.3A were incorrect.This error rate introduced in the lesson were summarized and was acceptable for a linear program, although additional examples of concepts and structures particularly difficult frames were again revised. were also given. The summary sheets some- A calculation of the average error rate for each times included sentence tree diagrams to re- lesson showed that the error rate was relatively view graphically the relationships among sen- consistent across lessons, neither increasing tence structures. Appendix C contains the or decreasing systematically. summary sheet for Lesson 1. Lessons 1 thrcugh 10 presented nine basic sentence patterns and the main structures of a Program Measures basic sentence: noun phrase, verb phrase, and Questionnaire noun, noun marker, forms of be, verb, verb The Ss wrote their responses to each frame markers, completer, adjective, adverb, and on a separate annmor sheet.In addition to prepositional phrase. The basic patterns were instructional fun,Jtions, this answer sheet had as follows and were introduced in this order: two administrative purposes.It contained a record of the time required to work through each 1.NP be+NP lesson and of the errors made on each individual 2.NP be+adj frame. The Ss recorded time at the beginning 3.NP be+adv and end of each lesson.Teachers emphasized 4.NP V that time was recorded only to give information 5.NP V +NP on the length of the lessons and not to measure 6.NP Vs+adj the speed of the students. Teachers asked the 7.NP Vb+adj/NP students to cross out incorrect responses in- 8.NP V +NP + NP stead of erasing them.Tabulating these errors 9.NP V +NP + adj/NP gave the investigators some indication of the Lessons 11 through 22 introduced the concept difficult frames which might require revision. of transform and explained the processes for A 20-item questionnaire (Appendix D) was constructing relative clauses, possessive noun given to all treatment groups after completion phrases, passive sentences, phrases to expand of the 22 programed lessons. These questions a noun phrase, prenoun modifiers, appositives, asked for opinions on the lessons, worksheets, noun clauses, and two kinds of verb-form nomi- and sun.n.ary sheets. nals. The labels given to the nine transforms were abbreviated in the lessons, and lengthy Collection of Writing Samples algebraic transform rules were avoided in pref- During a period of five weeks immediately erence to verbal explanations and examples. before the treatment, and again immediately Appendix A contains a general outline of the 22 after treatment, each student in both the treat- lessons. ment and control groups wrote a 1000-word prose writing sample.Usually, three or four Format of Worksheet themes were required to obtain 1000 words. The worksheets for Treatment W contained Although all 207 students wrote 1000-word sam- exercises designed to help the student apply ples,the themes used in the analysis were from

6 114 students randomly selected from stratified Xeroxed and returned intact to the teachers for groups.Neither the teachers nor the students classroom use after the experiment. thr were told which Ss had been selected.The Xerox duplicate, typists made legible cow,' for original manuscripts from each student were analysis. III OBJECTIVE TESTS AND PROGRAM MEASURES

Because Lessons I. through 10 consisted of of structures to include was left to the student. material on sentence patterns and Lessons 11 The basic sentences which the students were through 22 were on sentence transforms, sep- asked to combine were: arate tests were constructed for patterns and transforms.Test 1tested the knowledge of Problem One patterns and Test II tested transforms. Test The truck eased around the corner. II contained two sentence-combining problems The truck was carrying logs. which were analyzed separately as Test III. The logs were pine. Tests were used both as pretests and as posttests. The corner was sharp. The pretests were analyzed for significant Problem Two effects due to sex and treatment group.The The bear snarled at the hunters. posttests were analyzed several ways to deter- The bear was black. mine the effects of sex, treatment, ability, and The bear was protecting her cubs. school, using KAT score and/or pretest score The hunters were unfriendly. as covariates. The hunters were walking toward her. As shown by Hunt (19651, T-unit length and OBJECTIVE TESTS clause length are the two most reliable indexes Description of growth in sentence maturity.An increase in tests were given by the teachers to all clause length is attributable to an increase in groHps. The first test was used as a pretest less-than-clause structures placed in the sen- before Lesson 1 and as a posttest after Lesson tence through deletion and embedding. T-unit 1n.The second test was used as a pretest length reflects these processes, too, but is before Lesson 11 and as a posttest after Lesson also affected by an increase or decrease in the number of subordinate clauses.Since the pur- 22.Most of the items were reliable and dis- pose of the sentence-combining tasks was to criminated well when used on previous occa- test deletion and embedding, clause length was sions (Blount et al., 19671.Test I contained chosen to evaluate performance more directly. 3')four-option multiple-choice items and 15 If a student used all of the possibilities for completion items.rest ft had 35 four-option deletion and embedding in the two sets of kernel pie-choice items, 10 completion items, and two problen-is in sentence-comnining. sentences, he might write these two transform Inese latter two problems were treated as a sentences: separate test in this report although they were The truck carrying pine logs eased around (:P.,en Ss as part of Test II. the sharp corner. In each sentence-combining problem, the S The black bear, protecting her cubs, \vas asked to combine a series of short basic snarled at the unfriendly hunters walk- sen!ences intolonger sentence retaining the ing toward her. sa::.e information and adding no new information. st,dents were not told specifically to make Thus, the maximum clause length would be 10 simple sen'ence although the short sentences and 14 words, respectively. co!-:.Prisin: each item could easily have been Analysis r(,vorked r)nn longer sentence through dole 'i-on, and substitution. The choice The pretests posttests were analyzed of !Howl:An.; sentences to write and what kinds as follows: fi a.An analysis of covariance of the pretests Each of the three pretests (I,II, and III) was and posttests employed a 2 x 3 factorial design analyzed for significant effects in a2 x 3 with 23 Ss per cell (total N = 138) .The cover- analysis of covariance with 23 Ss per cell iate was KAT score. The factors were sex- (N = 138). The covariate was KAT score and male or female; and treatment-W, WO, or (.. the factors were sex and treatment.Table 2 b.An analysis of covariance of the post- contains the means and standard deviations of tests using a 2x 2x 3x 2 factorial c I.esid!: 1: the pretest scores. No significant differences chided all 207 Ss and, necessaril'..', had were :-:,)ti:!11-ietween the WO, and (' groups equal numbers in each cell.The covaria!! )1 the pretes.!.en-!ales score,: higher pretest score.The factors were sex -;;. !I! 1 (Ind 11 t :(,111 (2 .01); and fel-1.1,11e; school-A or 13; ability-low, ii.e(11 they also scored higher than males o!: i'retest or high (90-106, 107-118, or 119-1:15 on the Ill(p .051.Thus, in initial knowledge of KAT); and treatment-experimental or co!.!rol. grammar and in ability to combine sentences, c.Another analysis of covariance of the females were more advanced than males, posttests employed a 2 x 2 x 3 factorial design but no differences existed between the control with 12 Ss per cell (total N = 1441.The covar- and experimental groups. iates were KAT score and pretest score. The factors were sex-male or female; school-A Posttest Results or B; and treatment-W, WO, or (:.An analysis The three posttests (I,II, and III) were ana- of the sum of Posttests I and II was made with 1..-zed with the same design and the same Ss as this same design, using the KAT score and the that used for the pretests.In addition, two sum of Pretest I and II as the covariales. other designs were used so that an adequate picture of the results could be obtained.In Pretest Results almost every case, the posttests revealed that

For purposes of analysis, the two sentence- groups WO and \V learned a great deal more combining problems were analyzed as Test Ill. than the control groups.

Taiile 2 The Observed Mean Number of Items Correct and the Standard Deviations on the Pretests and Posttests (N = 138)

Grand N1ale Female WO Test (138) (69) (69) (46) (46) (46)

Pretest I

X 25.1 22.4 27.8 24.3 23.6 27.4 9.1 8.8 8.6 8.6 8.4 9.9 Pretest II

X 20.9 18.4 23.5 21.0 21.1 20.8 7.3 6.7 7.0 7.4 7.2 7.4 Pretest III

X 15.8 11.4 17.9 14.5 16.0 17.0

a L. ), 6.9 5.3 6.9 6.5 5.1

Posttesti

3(1.8 27.6 34.0 .2. 1 31.1 29.0 9.9 9.7 9.2 9.9 10.1 Posttest II

24.3 )1.t) 27.5 ) . ).1 29.1 8.6 A,0 7.9 8.8 9.5 Posttest Ill 17.5 1 0,1, 18.5 18. A 1 6.; U 6.2 U.9 5.4 U. 4.9 6. A

9 Table 3 Posttest 0.The initial analysis showed both sex and treatment to be significant.Females Score on Posttest I Using Pretest I were above males on mean score (2. < .01), as the Covariate and Groups W and WO were each significantly higher than the control group (2. < .05 by Newman-Keuls analysis) ,Table 2 shows the 2 x 3 x 2 x 2 Analysis of Covariance Note that the relevant means of this analysis. Sum of improvement from Pretest I to Posttest I for the Source two Groups W and WO was about 8 points as Squares df compared to less than 2 points for Group C. In a second analysis of Posttest 1, all 207 Within Cells 3840.08182 -- Ss were used. This analysis tested not only Regression 5373.39 1 254.67.001 the effects of sex and treatment but also pos- Treatment (T) 1799.77 1 85.30.001 320.15 2 7.59.001 sible differences due to ability and school.The IQ results are summarized in Tables 3 and 4. The Sex 46.41 1 2.20 ns score on Pretest I was used as a covariate, and, School (S) 156.24 1 7.40.007 as a result, effects attributed to sex in the T x IQ 60.22 2 1.43 ns previous analysis disappeared. Though females T x Sex 21.20 1 1.00 ns 1 7.18.008 were at a higher level when entering the experi- T x S 151.53 ment than males, there were not differential IQ x Sex 2.98 2 0.07 ns learning increases.Both sexes retained the IQ x S 40.78 2 0.97 ns same relative standing at the close of the ex- Sex x S 4.42 1 0.21 ns 2 1.71 periment as had been noted at the start.Groups T xIQxSex 72.29 ns W and WO were considered as one group in the T x IQ x S 37.53 2 0.89 ns comparisons with Group C. The 100 Ss in the T x Sex x S 6.51 1 0.31 ns experimental groups had a mean score of 31.9 IQ x Sex x S 5.34 2 0.13 ns on Posttest I, while the control group scored T x IQ x Sex x S 83.22 2 1.97 ns 26.9, a difference that was significant (2. +.79 .001). Raw Regression Coefficient:

Table 4 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for Posttest I Using Pretest I as the Covariate

Means ContraFi Factor Level Pre Post (x' X')

Treatment 100 Experimental 24.2 31.9 + 3.18 107 Control 25.9 26.9 3.18 IQ 65 Low 18.4 22.7 2.12 75 Medium 24.8 28.6 + .08 67 High 31.8 37.2 + 2.04 Sex 102 1\la le 23.0 27.0 .36 10 ['ornate 27.1 32.0 + .36 School 102 A 24.3 29.9 + 1.02 105 25.8 29.1 1.02 interaction Treatment x School Exp. A 23.8 33.1 + 5.24

47 Exp. 13 24.6 30.6 + 1.12 49 Control A 24.8 26.5 3.20 58 Control B 26.7 28.0 3.16

10 Though males and females showed equal males and that the experimental gro..ips were gains, the different ability levels experienced higher than the control groups. The medium differential learning increases.As shown in and high ability groups learned more than the Table 4, the high group was superior to the low ability group (p-.01 by :\:ewman-heals medium group, which in turn scored higher comparison). The regression of pretest score than the low group. Newman- Keels compari- to posttest score was significant (n .00P. son of means showed that the differences be- tween all groups were significant.Pretest I Fable 5 score was used as a covariate, and thus the significant differences represent increases -:core on Posttest II1. sing Pretest 11 over and above the linear effect of prior knowl- as the Covariate edge. The high group increased an average of 5.4 points from Pretest I to Posttest I compared 2 x 3 x 2 x 2 Analysis of covariance to an increase of 3.8 for the medium group and 4.3 for the low group (p.05). SlIm Another significant effect in the analysis of Source Squares df Posttest I was the school variable. The stu- dents in School A were superior (p< .01) on Within Fell s 3275.68182 Posttest Ito the School B students.Since the Regression 3461.39 1 192.32.001 linear effect of pre'st score was removed, the Treatment (T) 662.73 1 36.82.001 difference reflects)re gain for School. i than 10 189.32 2 5.26.006 for School B.Table 4 shows these pretest and Sex 161.60 1 8.98.003 posttest differences. The factor of school in- School (S) 47.69 1 2.65.105 teracted significantly with treatment .01), x 12.13 2 0.34 ns as shown by the contrasts in Table 4.The T x Sex 9.37 1 0.52 ns experimental Ss in School A advanced to a T x S 46.01 1 2.56 ns higher level of knowledge than the experimental IC) x Sex 86.95 2 2.42.092 Ss in School B, while both control groups showed IQ x S 61.00 2 1.70 ns only small advances. Newman -Keels comparison Sex x 0.81 1 0.04 ns of means showed that the experimental. group in T x I() x Sex 10.09 2 0.28 ns School A was higher than the experimental group TxIo x S 53.51 2 1.49 ns in School B (Exp. B) and the control groups Tx Sex x 31.10 1 1.73 ns (p < .01), and that Exp. B was higher than both IQxSexx S 8.45 2 0.24 ns control groups (p < .01). TxIQxSexxS 1.38 2 0.01 ns Whether or not the experimental groups them- selves differed from one another on test scores Raw Regression Coefficient: +.85 was answered by the third analysis of covari- ance of Posttest I.With KAT score and pre- test score as covariates and considering all The third analysis, which used 144 Ss in a three groups, W, WO, and C', the treatment 3 x 2 x 2 design that tested Groups W, WO, effect was significant at the .001 level. and C, showed that, with the linear effect of Newman-Keels comparison of the three adjusted both pretest score and RAT score removed, means for Groups W, WC), and C (32.5, 32.4, Groups W and \VO were significantly higher and 26.6, respectively) showed that Groups \V than Group C (p .01 by Newman-Keels com- and WO were significantly higher (p. < .01) than parison).No significant differences existed Group C, but that there were no differences be- between Groups W and WO. With KAT and tween Groups W and WO. pretest as covariates, sex (p. .05) and schoo1 Posttest H.The pattern of differences between (p_ .01) were signiiicant; females were higher groups was much the same on Posttest II as it than males, and School A Ss higher than School had been on Posttest I.The first analysis, in B Ss. which KAT score was the covariate and sex and Posttest 1 and I1 CombinedThe scores of the treatments were the factors, showed that fe- two posttests were combined and analyzed males scored higher than males (p < .01) and using the 3 x 2 x 2 covariance design which that Group;; W and WO scored higher than Group employed two covariates (KAT and combined C (o <.01 by Newman-Keuts comparison). The pretest scores) ,144 Ss, and the factors treat- means from this analysis are presented in Table 2.ment, school, and sex.The summary of this In Table 5 the summary of the second analysis analysis is described in Table 7, and the rele- of covariance is presented. The effects of treat- vant means and contrasts are in Table 8. ment, ability, and sex were significant (2 < .01). Posttests I and II in combination provided Table 6 shows that females were higher than a good indication of the total amount of concept

11 Table 6 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for Posttest iI Using Pretest II as the Covariate

Means Contrast Factor Level Pre Post (x' Treatment 100 Experimental 21,1 25.4 + 1.91 107 Control 19.9 20.8 1.91 IQ 65 Low 15.8 17.7 - 1.83 75 Medium 19.7 22.6 + .90 67 High 25.8 28.7 + .94 Sex 102 Male 18.7 20. 3 .81 105 Female 22. 2 25.7 + .81 School 102 A 20.4 23.5 + .62 105 20.5 22. 6 .62

Table 7 Posttest 111.The preliminary analysis of the two sentence-combining tasks showed no sig- Analysis of Covariance of the Sum of Posttests nificant effects for sex or treatment. However, I and II Using KAT Score and Pretest I and II when the Pretest III score was used as a covarl- Scores as Covariates ate, several significant effects were revealed, as shown in Table 9.Ss in the experimental 3 x 2 x 2 Analysis of Covariance groups combined sentences more maturely than those in the control group (2. < .05).Ss in Sum of School A were significantly above Ss in School Source Squares df P_ B in sentence combining (2. < .001). Table 10 shows the relevant means for the main effects Within Cells 5060.8130 and for the two significant interactions. The Regression 25083.8 2 322.17.001 contrasts and means show that in each inter- Treatment (T) 3004.9 2 38.60.001 action one group of Ss scored quite low in School (SC) 578.6 1 14.86.001 relation to the other groups. Sex (5) 209.0 1 5.37.022 T x SC 434.4 2 5.58.005 Test Rem Analysis T x S 26.1 2 0.34 ns SC x S 10.5 1 0.27 ns Test I contained 35 multiple-choice items T x SC x S 41.9 2 0.54 ns and 15 completion items. Test II contained 35 multiple-choice items and 10 completion Raw Regression Coefficients: KAT +.18 items. Test III consisted of two sentence- Pretest +.94 combining problems. The responses from the 100 students in Groups W and WO were used for an analysis of items.All multiple-choice learning achieved by Ss. The items tested all and completion items on Tests I and II were the major concepts in the program. The results analyzed by the simplified test analysis pro- indicated a highly significant treatment effect cedure described by Stanley (1964). The nature in favor of Groups W and WO over Group C. of the sentence-combining problems of Test III Also significant were the effects of sex (2. < did not permit item analysis.In the Stanley .05, with females higher than males) and school procedure, the top 27A of the scores are com- (2. < .001, with A higher than B). Treatment pared to the bottom 27% to obtain a measure of and school showed a significant interaction discrimination for each item. The discrimina- < .01) which, as the contrasts in Table 8 tion index of an item is the number of times it show, could be interpreted as due to the gains was answered incorrectly by the low 27% minus of the Ss in School A in using the experimental the number of times it was missed by the high treatments. 27%. Thus, an index of 1 2 would imply that 12 Table 8 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for the Sum of Posttest I and II Using KAT Score and Pretest I and II as Covariates

Means Contrast Factor Level Pre Post (x' x')

Treatment 48 Without 44.65 56.25 2.86 48 With 46.00 58.90 + 3.58 48 Control 45.15 47.62 6.45 School 72 A 43.58 54.38 + 2.03 72 46.95 54.18 2.03 Sex 72 Male 40.40 48.00 1.30 72 Female 50.13 60.58 + 1.30 Treatment x School 24 Without A 43.00 58.38 * 7.04 24 Without B 46.30 54.12 1.32

24 With A 45.90 60.58 4 5.59 24 With B 46.10 57.20 4 1.57 24 Control A 41.8.5 44.21 6.55 24 Control B 48.45 51.25 - 6.35

Table 9 The multiple-choice items of Test I, when used in the pretest, had a mean discrimination Score on Posttest III Using Pretest III index of 13.7; the completion items had a as the Covariate mean discrimination index of 19.9. Asa post- test, these same items showed discrimination 2 x 3 x 2 x 2 Analysis of Covariance indexes of 18.3 for the multiple-choice items and 20.0 for the completion items. Sum of The multiple-choice items of Test Ti, when Source Squares df F used in the pretest, had a mean discrimination index of 12.3; the completion items, 19.9. Within Cells 5384.52182 -- On the posttest these same items had mean Regression 741.51 1 25.06.001 discrimination indexes of 15.9 and 21.7, re- Treatment (T) 129.99 1 4.39.037 spectively. IQ 163.78 2 2.77.065 E. V. Piers (Stanley, 1964) calculated that Sex 12.81 1 0.43 ns for four options and 100 Ss a discrimination School (S) 323.65 1 10.94.001 index of 8 or higher on an individual item is T x IQ 183.20 2 3.10.048 significant at the .025 level.The percent of T x Sex 12.72 1 0.43 ns items reaching or exceeding this criterion of T x S 24.75 1 0.84 ns 8 is summarized in Table 11.A very high per- IQ x Sex 30.67 2 0.52 ns centage of the items discriminated significantly IQ x S 210.29 2 3.55.031 between high and low scorers. Sex x S 19.99 1 0.68 ns T x IQ x Sex 29.18 2 0.49 ns PROGRAM MEASURES T x IC) x S 1.73 2 0.03 ns TxSexxS 12.55 1 0.42 ns Errors IQ x Sex x S 13.51 2 0.23 ns T x x Sex x S 3.72 2 0.06 ns To assess the difficulty of the lessons and to identify ElMbig11011s or otherwise poor fralles, Raw Regression Coefficient +.35 assistants recorded and compiled the errors found on each sHdent's response sheet. Tahle 12 gives the total number of errors and the twelve more low scorers than high scorers mean error rale for two ability groups ,those got an item wrong. Ss above the median score (1. 13) on KAT and 13 Table 10 Means and Adjusted Contrasts for Posttest III Using Pretest III as the Covariate

Means Contrast [actor Level Pre Post (x' X')

Treatment 100 Experimental 15.3 18.0 .87 107 Control 15.9 16.6 .87 IQ 65 Low 11.8 14.6 2.09 75 Medium 16.3 17.7 + .83 67 High 18.6 19.3 + 1.26 Sex 102 Male 14.7 16.5 .91 ].05 Female 16.5 18.0 + .91 School 102 A 14.1 17.9 + 1.31 105 B 17.1 16.6 1.31 Interaction Treatment x IQ 36 Exp. Low 11.1 16.4 + .06 28 Exp. Med. 17.3 18.3 + 1.18 36 Exp. High 17.9 19.2 + 1.37 29 Control Low 12.6 12.3 4.24 47 Control Med. 15.7 17.4 + .48 3]. Control High 19.3 19.4 + 1.15 IQ x School 41 Low A 11.0 16.5 + .67 24 Low B 13.1 11.3 4.85 26 Med. A 14.1 18.0 1.76 49 Med. B 17.5 17.6 .10 35 High A 17.8 19.5 + 1.50 32 High B 19.5 19.1 + 1.02

Table 11 Percent of Items with a Discrimination Index Significant at .025

Test I Test II Item Type Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest

Multiple-choice 86 100 80 90 Completion 100 100 100 90 those below 113 on KAT. The mean error rate, All frames answered incorrectly by :3ix errors per 100 responses, was generally at a or more Ss were marked for revision.From low level. The overall error rate for the higher a total of 1,328 frames, 14.5/, eJ the :rames ability group was 1.13, for the lower ability elicited incorrect responses from 6 or more group, 2.34. The highest error rate for both the students and were subsequently revised; high and low Ss occurred on Les son1. -1, concerning60.5 elicited incorrect responses from1 to the related clause with whose, a two-transform 5students; and 25 1.elicited no incorrect process.There was no consistent relationship responses. between lesson length, content, and error rate.

14 Table 12 Number of Errors, Errors per 100 Responses, and Time forEach Lesson

Number Number Errors per Average Time of of 100 Responses in Minutes Lesson Number and Content Frames Errors High Low Ability AbilityHigh Low Ss Ss

Lesson1:A Look at Basic Sentences 60 75 .61 1.30 22 27 Lesson 2:Basic Sentence Patterns 1 and 2 60 124 1.36 1.92 22 26 Lesson3:Basic Sentence Pattern 3 and Review 65 148 .69 2.66 22 27 Lesson4:Basic Sentence Pattern 4 60 96 .65 1.99 20 22 Lesson5:More About Nouns 65 119 .94 1.88 20 23 Lesson6:Recognizing Verbs 87 206 .82 2.69 26 28 Lesson7:Recognizing Adverbs 52 116 1.37 2.13 16 19 Lesson8:Adjectives with Patterns 5 and 6 75 209 1.50 2.81 22 23 Lesson9:Basic Sentence Patterns 7 and 8 64 131 1.21 1.99 19 19 Lesson 10:Basic Sentence Pattern 9 and General Review 55 142 1.52 2.52 19 20 Lesson 11:Introduction to Transform Sentences and Related Clauses 52 90 .78 1.84 16 16 Les son 1 2:T-rel (related clause) 55 110 1.11 1.99 18 18 Lesson 13:T-rel Again 65 136 1.13 2.11 22 22 Les son 14:T-rel after T-pos (possessive construction) 55 338 2.75 6.58 18 18 Lesson 15:T-pass (passive construc- tion) 50 106 1.10 2.16 16 18 Lesson 16:T-ph (participial phrases) 55 123 .93 2.44 17 19 Lesson 17:T-BN (single words before the noun) 60 131 .95 2.35 21 Lesson 18:T-NP and Double Nouns (appositives, noun adjuncts) 62 112 .86 1.90 19 ?r, Lesson 19:T-NC (noun clause) 70 172 1.31 2.49 20 20 Les son 20:T-Vto (Vto nominals) 65 110 .85 1.75 18 19 Les son 21:T-Ving (-ing nominals) 50 110 .98 2.36 14 16 Les son 22:Adj and Adv Nominals and Review of Nominals 46 102 1.82 1.82 14 15 TOTAL 1328 3006 421 454 MEAN 60.4 136.6 1.13 2.34 19.1 20.6

Time QUESTIONNAIRE The overall average time required tocom- The questionnaire concerned the general plete a lesson was 19.1 and 20.6 minutes for suitability of the lessons for practical class- the upper and lower ability groups, respectively. room use.Over one-half of the students The range extended from 13 to 26 minutes for the thought the length of the lessons was just. right. high group and from 13 to 30 minutes for the low. Most of the remaining students considered The Pearson product-moment correlation between them too long, despite the low average time of length of lesson and average timewas +.79. 20 minutes.Very few students thought the pace The time required to work through each lesson of one lesson per day too fast; most considered is shown in Table 12. it a comfortable pace. About two-thirds of the

15 students reported quite candidly that they revealed Although interest in the lessons generally correct responses from1 to 3 times a lesson, 20A was moderate, 90% indicated that the new said they did so more than 3 times, and 13A" said approach was more interesting than the tra- they never revealed the correct response before ditional, and 96% thought the new approach writing it.Very few found the vocabulary difficult; would be more helpful to them.Table 13 62% thought it very easy. Most students thought shows the responses to the question,To the summary sheet was helpful, and the majority what degree do you feel that what you learned of students referred to the previous lessons for about the following things will be helpful to review. Almost all students receiving the work- you in thy: future?Students were asked to sheets after each lesson reported that they found rank seven concepts and transforms in order of the worksheets helpful in understanding the lesson decreasing difficulty.Table 14 summarizes . better and in realizing how they could apply what the responses of the students who answered was taught in that lesson. this item. One can conclude from the Table 13 Responses to the Question, To what degree do you feel that what you learned about the following things will be helpful to you in the future?

Rating Content being rated Very helpful Somewhat helpful Not helpful

Basic sentences 39.3% 55.4% 5.3% Test-sentences 47.3 45.5 7.2 Clues for recognizing a noun, verb, adj., adv. 64.3 30.4 5.3 Transform processes for combining shorter sentences to expand an NP (T-rel, T-BN, T-ph, T-NP, etc.) 41.1 40.2 18.7 Transform processes for making nominals (T-NC, T-Vto, T-Ving) 28.6 54.4 17.0

Table 14 Students' Ranking of the Difficulty of Seven Concepts

Rank of Difficulty Concept Difficult Easy Average 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 rank Subject and predicate groups 3 3 1 3 2 15 35 38 6.73 Basic sentence patterns 10 7 7 4 5 34 36 7 5.33

T-rel 7 12 19 20 30 7 5 0 3.95 T-BN (pre-noun modifiers) 8 24 24 7 21 7 8 1 3.67 T-Vto (infinitive nominals) 23 17 8 15 17 10 7 3 3.59 T-NC

(noun clause) 14 17 . 16 25 14 13 2 0 3.58

T-pass 18 18 22 22 9 6 3 2 3.23

16 students' responses that the lessonscover- A Programed lessons alone ing the basic sentence patterns andsen- B Programed lessons with teacher'shelp tence structures (Lessons 1-10)were the C Standard textbook form easiest, and that transformswere considered D Teacher lecturing more difficult. There was not enoughcon- The responses of the 78 studentsanswering census to indicate one transformas being this question are summarized inTable 15. more difficult than another. The preferred mode was the programedapproach Students were asked to rank four alternate with teacher's help; the method preferredleast modes of presenting material: was teacher lecturing.

Table 15 Student Rank of Four Modes of Presentation

Rank Mode Best Worst Average 1 2 3 4 rank

AProgramed lessons alone 17 37 13 11 2.23 B Programed lessons with teacher help 43 20 15 0 1.64 CStandard textbook 13 16 14 2.73 DTeacher lecture 5 5 15 53 3.48

17 Iv WRITING S AMBLES

PROCEDURES Exclusion of Certain Kinds of Sentences Themes totaling approximately 1000 words were written by each student before and after Hunt (n.d.), in his report on the sentence the experiment.Prewriting samples were col- structures of superior fourth- and twelfth-grade lected over a period of five weeks preceding students and superior adults, made certain the experiment. The postwriting samples were procedural recommendations as to wnat to omit collected during the five weeks following the when analyzing sentences. Relying on these experiment.Pre- and postwriting samples of recommendations, the present investigators 114 Ss were scored for the number of sentences, omitted the following items from the writing T-units, clauses, and words and for kinds of samples before totaling the 1000 words: subordinate clauses used. These scores were analyzed to test whether treatment, ability, 1.Sentence fragments and unintelligible sex, or school had significant effects upon structures. 2.All direct discourse.(If direct discourse writing. had been included, the immature writer would be credited with an abundance of Specifications for noun clauses, and his overall clause Writing Samples length would decrease due to the presence of many abnormally short clauses such as In specifying topics for the student themes, "Mary said.") the teachers were urged to avoid topics which 3.Imperatives and questions.(These occur would prompt a great deal of direct discourse too infrequently in writing to influence the since sentences of this type would be elimi- statistical analyses significantly.) nated before analysis.In some cases the Ss were given a list of possible narrative and ex- In addition, sentences containing direct quo- pository topics and were allowed to choose tations were also eliminated because direct those which appealed to them most. The topics quotations reflect the syntactic ability of some eventually used were as follows: writer other than the student.

Prewriting Working Definitions 1.Desirable changes at school The investigators used Hunt's definitions 2.Future plans and goals of sentence, clause, and 'r-unit in deciding 3.Narrative about an event what structures would be tabulated as such. 4.Description of someone A sentence was defined as "the words written 5.A typical school day between a capital letter and a period or other terminal punctuation." A clause was "a struc- Pos twriting ture containing a subject (or coordinated sub- 1.. Best time of year jects) and a finite verb phrase (or coordinated 2.Desirable chancres at home verbs or phrases) ."T-unit, or "minimal termi- 3.Description of a pet nable unit," was defined as "one main clause 4.Narrative about a trip plus the subordinate clauses attached to or 5.Defense of special programs at school embedded within it [Ilunt, 1965, p. 491."

18 A T-unit is the shortest grammatically al- instructed not to correct misspellings and lowably unit into which a sentence can be seg- punctuation, not to add words which were mented without producing sentence fragments. carelessly omitted, and not to delete words Every sentence contains at least one T-unit, which the writer himself had not deleted. The but it may have more.Every T-unit has at first process of breaking each writing sample least a main clause and may have several sub- into sentences was accomplished simultaneously ordinate clauses. T-units within sentences with the typing. The typists were told to num- are usually coordinated with and, but, or or ber each sentence consecutively and to consider so. The coordinator must be followed by a as a sentence everything that appeared between clause having a subject and finite verb. a capital letter and terminal punctuation. The word so required special attention be- Working from the typed copies, two raters cause it has two very different functions. indicated the end of each T-unit with a double Used one way the word means (and) so; used slash.Subordinate clauses were indicated by another, so (that).(And) so begins a T-unit; a single slash, and the type of clause was but so (that) signals a subordinate adverb written immediately above. The subordinate clause, as in the T-unit"...and I would clauses found in the writing samples were start on another job so people couldn't call identified as the following kinds: me a lazy man." Here the writer clearly means noun clauses so that.At other times, when the ambiguous adjective clauses so could understandably be either possibility, adverb clauses the decision was made subjectively. clauses of comparison The following sentence, written by an eighth deferred subject construction grader, contains three T-units (marked by double cleft sentence slashes) each of which could stand alone as a subjunctive word order grammatically complete structure.In addition clauses that complement an adjective to the main clauses, two of the T-units have clauses that complement a verb like "seem" a subordinate clause (marked by a single slash the more, the merrier construction and an abbreviation). special which construction If I had a milliondollaardsv/I think/that I would Most noun clauses occurred in the familiar do a lot of things// nominal positions of subject, direct object, but I would put most of the money in the bank// and object of a preposition.Another fairly a and I would start on anotherjovb/so people common position was as an appositive after a couldn't call me a lazy man// noun. Here are several examples of the ap- positive noun clause: A definition of word was also needed to avoid large discrepancies in the actual word It creates a feeling that we shall strive on. count of the writing samples. Decisions on The fact that there are good teenagers is never whether to count a structure as one word or mentioned. two (no matter how the Ss had written it) were He stated the opinion that we should continue made by referring to Webster's Seventh New to fight. Collegiate Dictionary, 1965. The investigators Although on the surface these noun clauses were advised byHunt1 to consider all contrac- seemed identical to adjective clauses in the tions as two words since they would be written same position, their deep structure, or deriva- out in formal writing.Also, all proper nouns, tion, was quite different.An easy test for an regardless of their "actual" word count, were appositive noun clause is to place it in predi- tabulated as one word.Consequently, terms cate nominal position after be: like Mr. Grimm, Joe, and Yellowstone National Park were counted as one word. A feeling is that we shalt strive on. The fact is that there are good teenagers. Identifying and Tabulating The opinion is that we should continue to fight. T-units and Clauses Adjective clauses will not function in the predicate nominal position, but appositive To facilitate the analysis of T-units and noun clauses will. clauses, the 1000-word samples were typed "Asides" such as I suppose and I think oc- exactly as S had written them. Typists were curred relatively infrequently.In the sentence "I thought the party was disastrous," the clause 1Personal interview with Kellogg W. Hunt, following I thought was identified as a subordi- Madison, Wisconsin, June, 1966. nate noun clause and I thought as the main clause.

19 Clauses beginning with where and when may There were two kinds of clauses whose function as any of three frequently used clauses classification as an adverb clause or as a adjective, adverb, or noundepending upon clause of comparison depended on the context their sentence position and movability.As ad- of the sentence. These special. cases were verb clauses they may be moved to different clauses beginning with as far as and as long positions in the sentence without loss of mean- as. When the writer intended theliteral mean- ing. ing of far and long, expressions of distance, length, or duration, the clauses were tabulated When I woke up, I heard noises outside the tent. as clauses of comparison.But when the func- I heard noises outside the tent when I woke up. tion of the structures was analogous to insofar A when clause functioning as a nominal was most as or if, they were consideredadverb clauses. likely to occur as the direct object after verbs As far as the Indians could see,buffalo crowded like remember, imagine, and so on. the prairie.(comparison) The sheriff remembered where he lost his keys. As far as the jury is concerned, the prisoner I can imagine when they'll arrive. is guilty.(adverb) The dangling climber hung on as long as he Clauses in this position were analyzed as noun could.(comparison) clauses. However, if the writer supplied the She said I could go as long as I told her where. deletable noun of place or time before where (adverb) or when, the clause then became anadjective clause, as in: Some subordinate clauses appeared as a complement to an adjective, usually a predi- The sheriff remembered the place where he lost cate adjective relating to emotion or state of his keys. mind. These clauses could not be moved away As an adjective clause, where he lost his keys from their position after the adjective: cannot be moved away from the noun phrase it I'm sorry (that)1 didn't do better. expands without destroying the deep-structure She was sure (that) I had left home. relationships in the sentence. The deputy was convinced (that) the car was Where he lost his keys, the sheriff remembered stolen. the place. A very infrequent kind of complement clause An example of the where or when clause used occurred after a verb like seem, feel, appear, as an adjective clause can be seen in a sen- look, and so on.Here are some examples of tence such as, "The fire occurred that time this kind of clause: when they were gone." That time when they The paragraph sounds like the author is against were gone is used as an adverbialof time, but war. the entire structure can be further analyzed as She looked as if she had seen a ghost. a noun phrase plus a clause. Theclause itself The director felt as though the tempo was too functions as an adjective clause even though s low. it does appear within a larger adverbial. structure. Clauses of comparison were tabulated sepa- Occasionally a writer used the indefinite it, rately rather than with the movable adverb which had no referent, as the "grammatical" clauses.Although some clauses of comparison subject of the sentence. The "logical" subject were movable, as in "I left as soon as Icould," followed later, sometimes in the form of a clause. most were very closely connected to a noun, It was unfortunate that such an accident hap- adjective, or adverb and had a fixed sentence pened position.Here are several examples: It's the fog that keeps the dirt and fumes from It got so dark that I was going to turn on my escaping, lantern. Using the same terms which Hunt (1965) as- You do a lot more than you did at the first of signed to these structures, raters labeled the the year. first sentence as a "deferred subject" sentence The clouds seemed so low that you could catch and the second as a "cleft" sentence. The dif- one. ference between a sentence with a deferred sub- They are so crowded that you can't walk up and ject and a cleft sentence is quite simple, down the steps. theoretically. One can reposition the deferred We should have more dances than we had last subject clause as the grammatical subject, as year. in "That such an accident happened was unfor- The hell made such a noise that I couldn't hear. tunate."to make an acceptable English

20 sentence.But one cannot say That keeps the coordinators. The longest sentence found in dirt and fumes from escaping is the fog." the writing samples of this study consisted of The use of a different word order to express 136 words, which is eight times longer than the subjunctive mood was very rare.Only one the average eighth-grader's sentence.But student wrote a sentence containing the sub- this sentence contained 11 main clauses con- junctive word order. nected with coordinators, commas, or simply They would have killed each other had it not no punctuation at all.Sentence length alone been for Mr. Napoleon. as an index of maturity would place this writer far ahead of his classmates who might be more "The more, the merrier" construction was skilled at punctuation and subordination.By used by some students, though relatively in- breaking the sentence into its main clauses, frequently.Here are the sentences in which or T-units, plus any attached subordinate clauses of this type appeared. clauses, one can compute more valid measures And the fewer the days get, the more I think of maturity: T-unit length and clause length about it. (Hunt, 1965). The more he worried, the more he felt sorry Main clauses, or 'f-units, may be lengthened for himself. by adding subordinate clauses and by embedding The test shows that the more you move, the less-than-clause structures. The amount of larger the growth gets. embedding a student does is reflected in his The warmer the water, the farther down the clause length, while the addition of subordi- fish will be. nate clauses may be measured by the ratio of The more they understand each other, the more total clauses divided by total T-units. The they're able to get done. ratio of clauses to T-units, called the "sub- But no rain, no food. ordinate clause index," is an arithmetic link The more money I would have, the more things between clause length and T-unit length: I could get. words clauses words The special which construction covers x "which" clauses that modify an entire sentence clauses T-units T-units or idea. A mature writer would probably use an A subordinate clause index of 1.8 indicates adverbial clause instead. Some examples are: that 80/, of the time a suoordinate clause was I dislike school as a whole because you have included in the main clause.Because all T- to get up so early in the morning which cuts unas must contain at least one main clause, down on your sleep. this index can never be less than 1.0 and is When we come in the morning we can go to the rarely greater than 2.0. gym, which I think is nice. Another ratio, 'f-units per sentence, is the School should end when grade school ends, arithmetic link between T-unit length and sen- tence length and is known as the "main clause which is about four o'clock. coordination index."This ratio is never less than 1.0 since each sentence has at least one Measures of Syntactic Growth T-unit or main clause.T-units per sentence is the only synopsis factor which decreases For each student's 1000 words of pre- and with increasing maturity (Hunt, 1965).Mature postwriting, the raters made 15 frequency counts writers use less coordination between main which included the number of the eleven kinds clauses and more subordination and embedding. of subordinate clauses and the number of sen- tences, T-units, clauses, and words. These Training of Raters last four were used for the computation of five synopsis factors. The five synopsis factors Two raters were trained to analyze the writing were words per sentence, words per T-unit, samples. They identified and marked T-units words per clause, T-units per sentence, and and subordinate clauses and assigned a label to clauses per T-unit. each subordinate clause.In a two-week period Although mature writers produce longer sen- they were taught to recognize the various kinds tences than young children, sentence length as of clauses and to indicate the names of clauses a measure of individual growth is relatively un- with a standard notation, NT for noun clause, reliable.Immature writers may produce sen- comp for clauses of comparison, and so on. tences of extraordinary length because they are When their accuracy and speed showed that unskilled in punctuation and are more apt to further training was not required, they analyzed connect a long string of main clauses with the samples of eighth-grade writing independently.

21 To assure accuracy of the final co ,:its fti ,: of nine :.))st- classifications for the ,,vriting sarr,Hes used ts !",..ictorial de

the statistical analyses, a third person ) pared the notations on all papers alter each rater had made independent trialyses.Dis-

1( crepancies in the two raters' Lalln.,s o ), 1- . 1- clauses, T-units, and sentence:-, were tr tu.'H J:

to the specific sentences, and errors is. t:: s )! 1

rectos.Disagreements in the classifn..c l.. %vele I of types of subordinate clauses were . i It."1 11I : 1.1 (_.*( , by the third person. 1:tj di:terk )1 - 1:,:)1- eifc2Ctr.,_): !he ANALYSIS

The 1000-word writing san.phs 1,I en:nth RESULTS graders were tabulated on the basis o! ir, ari- ables which included eleven different ,Ands of Jr t:.!' structures and five synopsis scorm;. n-e- )!t:-. hint .1.s quencies of seven structures were to) inh-yi,ent Inc :ro tccordih; to provide a rigorous basis for anl7sis vari- sex, thd f,-)r IWOV/11. V, A:A ance. Thus,us, only nine measures werewe Dostwrit1m-: I able 10 Is : sd on he 7 1.Subordinate adjective clauses of analysis a.In the nine !he nre- writing sa::.nles, there were two si,:hatcant 2.Subordinate adverb clauses 3.Subordinate noun clauses effects, neither of whim in the assi-.'i'(.'lit 4.Clauses of comparison of Ss to treau.. rou: s.With Iin(ar ofteCt 1) 5.Clause length .,ore controlled by covariance, Ss below t:-.e 6.Clauses per T-unit five score wro,J :..ore 7.T-unit length synoiSIS ordi,-.ate .n (.1,t .scs than :is a} ove the median 8.T-units per sentence scores (j2 t:, ::,bers of no::; clauses 9.Sentence length (Ta*,101 6) were 1.!.c3 :he ability Ss and 1 3.6 for low.H.other si,-;nificant effect Those variables occurring too i_ntre,.;'.enh' showed that ::,ales wrote more nor sen- for statistical analysis were deferred subject, tence than fer,,i1c.,s(1. 38 c9r:.Hired to 1.28, cleft sentence, subjunctive word order, clauses p .,)')). other oftect or interaction due complementing an adjective or a verb line to sex, ability, or trea:::.ent was significant "seem," "the more the merrier" construction, in the of the nrewriting. and the special which clause. Several analyses were made of the writing Adjective Clauses measures: a.An analysis of covariance of nine pre- Thu AVeriF;(_, wrote 45 subordi- writing measures and nine postwriting measures nate clauses in 1110,) words of writing.Of these a 2 x 2 x 3 factorial design with six Ss per cell 45 structures about one-fourth were subordinate = 72).The covariate was RAT score. The adjective clauses. There were no differences factors were sexmale or female; ability- between any of the groups in the number of these above or below the median RAT score of 113; adjective clanses written in the prewriting sam- and treatmentW, WO, or C. ple, on the postwriting sample there was b.An analysis of covariance of nine post- evidence of :1 difference between treatment writing measures using a 3 x 3 factorial design groups. The first analysis ,in which ability which included all 114 Ss and, necessarily, (GAT score) was the covariate, showed that Ss had unequal numbers per cell.The covariate in Group wrote sidnificantly .05) more was the equivalent prewriting measure. The subordinate adjective clauses than did Ss in factors were treatmentW, WO, or C; and Group C. The menus are shown in Table 16. abilitylow, medium, or high, i.e.,90-106, However, when the prewriting score was 107-118, or 119-145 on the RAT. used as '1 covariate, no significant treatment c.An analysis of covariance of nine post- differences rei-uained in the adjective clause writing measures using a 3 x 2 factorial design measure.,analysis b is shown in Table 17. which included all 114 Ss and, necessarily, The d analysis, in which two covariates (1:AT had unequal numbers per cell. The covariate score and prewriting score) were used, also was the prewriting measure. The factors were showed 1.10 significant treatment effects for the treatment \V, WO, or C; and schoolA or 13. adjective clause measure, though Group \V was

22 Table 16 The Observed N:eins of Nine Writing l\.1eas9res Obtained fromthe 1000-,V,Jr(1 Prewritinf: 1).):.,:twri'And Simples of 72 Ss

With tliol it AllSs Nlale 111(91:l.f,l :,ow1: /Il' ('ontrol Exercises 1-1>:erci:e5-' \lein sd.

Prewritin(1 10.7 10.8 10.5 10.6 4.7 1. Ad j. 18.0 11.3 1,,.7 10.6 2.Adv. 20.1 10.2. 19.7 20.5 20.0 )0.n i.0.3 20.1 6.7 1 2, 6 12. 8 11 .3 13. 2 5.3 3. 1,,::)9n 1 3.3 1 3.1 12.9 13.6 4.(.'omp. .07 .91 1. 11 .78 1. 25 1 .nl .58 .96 1.11 5. WA' 7.9 7.7 7.7 7.9 7.7 8.0 7.7 7.8 .84 .18 6. ( '1' 1.56 1.57 1.55 1.59 1.51 1.59 1.58 1.57 7.w71' 12.3 12.1 11.9 1.2.5 11.8 1 2. 6 12.2 12.2 1.8 8.T 'S 1.38 1.28 1.29 1.37 1.30 1.38 1.31 1.33 .20 9.\V /--: 18.8 15.5 17.1 17.1 15.4 17.1 18.6 17.1 7.9 Postwritino 1.Adi. 11 .1 10.8 10.9 10.9 12.6 10.5 9.6 10.9 3.9 2.Ad v. 18.1 18.1 16. 0 20,1 17.9 2 0.1 16.7 18.2 7. 2 3.Noun 13.9 14.0 11.5 13.4 13.2 14.9 13.8 14.0 5.9 4. Comp. 1.08 .92 1 .03 .97 1.00 1.17 .83 1.00 1.32 5.1,V iC 7.9 7.9 7.9 7.9 7. 9 7. 9 /.8 7. 9 .74 6. C 'T 1.56 1.55 1.52 1.59 1.58 1.60 1.48 1.55 .17 .5 12. 3 1.9 7. W/1' 1 2.3 1 2.3 1 1 . 9 1 2.6 1 2 1 2 . 6 11 .7 8.T 'S 1.48 1.26 1.28 1.46 1.35 1.37 1.38 1.37 .34 9. \V 18.3 15.5 15.3 18.- 17.0 17.4 16.2 16.9 5.2

writing 11.8 adjective clauses per 1000 words ference which was shown in the prewriting compared to 10.0 for Group C at the end of the sample also (See Table 18). No interactions experimental treatment. approached significance in any of the analyses No other factor was significant in the analy- of this variable. ses of adjective clauses.Differences between The regression of the frequency of adverb males and females, differences among the three clauses in the prewriting sample was a signifi- ability groups, and differences between schools cant source of variance in the analysis of the were not significant, nor were interactionsof postwriting sample (j < .05).This significance the effects significant. The relevant means for indicated that the use of adverb clauses had the various groups arc shown in Tables 16, 17, some stability across the two writingsamples. and 18. The regression coefficients between Table 19 gives the summary of analysis b of each of the covariates and the dependent vari- this variable. able were very small and not significant. Noun Clauses Adverb Clauses Approximately 13 suboidinate noun clauses About 44/, of the clausal structures written were written in the 1000-word F-1mples bythe were subordinate adverb clauses. There were average S.Adjective, adverb, md noun clauses no significant differences between sexes or comprised 97% of the subordinate clausal struc- among ability groups in the use ofadverb clauses tures written by eighth graders. The analysis in the prewriting sample, and again no differ- of prewriting showed that low ability Ss wrote ences in the postwriting sample. Theeffect of more subordinate noun clauses thanthe higher treatment also did not approach significance. ability Ss. On the postwriting sample this was Tables 16 and 19 present these means. Only no longer true, as shown in Table 16.In Table the factor of school was near the significance 20 it can be seen that the effects of treatment level, with the students in School B writing and ability were not significant and that the more adverb clauses in the postwritingsample regression between prewriting and postwriting than the students in School A (E.- .06), a dif- was not significant. The differencebetween 23 Table 17 Table 19 Subordinate Adjective Clauses in the Subordinate Adverb Clauses in the Postwriting Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate Using Prewriting as the Covariate

3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance 3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance

Sum of Sum oiT Source Squares df F Source Squares df

Within Cells 1411.82 104 Within Cells 4342.10 104 Regression 6.17 1 .45 ns Regression 229.96 1 5.51 .021 Treatment 64.37 2 2.37 .098 Treatment 108.71 1.30 nE, IQ 21.11 2 .78 ns IQ 156.64 9 1.88 ns Interaction 29.99 4 .55 ns Interaction 244.89 4 1.47 ns Means and Adjusted Contrasts Means and Adjusted Contrasts Factor Level Means Contrast Factor Level Means Contrast Pre Post (x' 7(1) Pre Post (x'

Treatment Treatment 34 Without Ex. 10.3 11.0 + .14 34 Without Ex. 19.9 19.4 + .82 37 With Ex. 9.9 11.8 + .88 37 With Ex. 19.3 18.2 .20 43 Control 9.5 1000 - 1.02 43 Control 19.6 17.0 - 1.1.%1 IQ IQ 38 Low 10.6 10.4 -.63 38 Low 19.8 19.7 + 33 Medium 9.0 11.3 + .65 33 Medium 21.6 18.4 - .18 43 High 9.9 11.0 .02 43 High 17.9 16.5 - 1.26 Raw Regression Coefficient: +.05 Raw Regression Coefficient: +.20 Table 18 The Means for the Writing Measures schools shown in Table 18 was significant at for Ss in Each School the .001 level.Even with the linear trend in the prewriting sample removed from considera- tion in the postwriting, Ss in School B wrote Measure School A. School B significantly more subordinate noun clauses (63) (51) than Ss in School A (15.8 compared to 12.2). A complete summary of analysis c is given in Prewriting Table 21.Note that the school-by-treatment Adj. 9.8 10.1 interaction is significant (2_ < .05).Individual Adv. 18.0 21.7 comparisons of the postwriting means of the Noun 12.6 14.4 six groups in this interaction showed that Comp. .95 .94 Group WO in School B was significantly higher W/C 8.1 7.5 than all the groups in School A and also higher C/T 1.53 1.61 than Group W of School. B < .05). Group C W/T 12.3 12.1 in School B was higher than Group C in School T/S 1.30 1.32 A (2_ < .05). The more conservative Newman- W/S 15.9 16.0 Keuls comparison revealed two differences; Postwriting Groups WO and C in School B were higher than Adj. 11.3 10.4 Group C in School A (2.< .05). Adv. 16.9 19.6 Sex, ability, and the regression coefficient Noun 12.2 15.8 were not significant factors in the frequency Comp. 1.08 1.10 of noun clauses. Treatment was significant W/C 8.1 7.4 only insofar as it interacted with the difference C/T 1.54 1.56 between schools. No interpretation of the in- W/T 12.5 11.7 teraction of the difference between schools is T/S 1.35 1.37 apparent, except that the higher frequency of W/S 17.0 15.9 noun clauses in School B was already notice- able in the prewriting. 24 Table 20 Table 21 Subordinate Noun Clauses in the Postwriting Subordinate Noun Clauses in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatment Using Frewriting as the Covariate-Treatment by IQ by School 1 3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance 3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance

Sum of Sum of Source Squares df F _2 Source Squares df

Within Cells 3324.13 104 Within Cells 2981.16 107 Regression 74.95 1 2.34 ns Regression 39. 25 1 1.41 ns Treatment 55.39 2 0.87 ns Treatment 58.81 2 1.06 ns IQ 89.73 2 1.40 ns School 347. 29 1 12.46 .001 Interaction 115.64 4 0.90 ns Interaction 205.94 2 3.70 .028 Means and Adjusted Contrasts Means and Adjusted Contrasts

Factor Level Means Contrast Factor Level Means Contrast Pre Post (x' >7') Pre Post (x'

Treatment Treatment 34 Without Ex. 13.9 14.8 + .90 34 Without Ex. 13.9 14.8 + 1.51 37 With Ex. 12.2 12.9 .51 (WO) 43 Control 14.0 13.6 .39 37 With Ex. 12.2 12.9 .82 IQ (W) 38 Low 13.0 13.9 +-.19 43 Control (C) 14.0 13.6 .69 33 Medium 15.0 15.1 -+-.78 School 43 High 12.5 12.5 .97 63 A 12.6 12.2 1.72 51 B 14.4 Raw Regression Coefficient: +.15 15.8 + 1.72 Interaction 21 WO A 13.2 13.1 .73 Clauses of Comparison 23 A 11.7 13.0 .68 19 C A 13.0 10.1 3.75 Clauses of comparison were used very in- 13 WO B 15.1 17.8 + 3.75 frequently, approximately once per 1000-word 14 W B 13.1 12.9 .96 sample of eighth-grade writing. No main ef- 24 C B 14.8 16.4 + 2.37 fects or interactions were significant. The means are shown in Tables 16, 18, and analy- Raw Regression Coefficient: +.10 sis b is shown in Table 22. per clause. The clause length of male Ss was Other Tabulated Structures not significantly different from female Ss, nor did the ability groups differ in clause length The frequencies of seven other structures (See Tables 16 and 23).The treatment groups were too small to analyze rigorously. The did not write longer elm., F. es than were written deferred subject appeared 10 times in the by the control group. The one significant factor 114,000 words which constituted the prewriting that appeared was a differenceetween schools. sample; subjunctive word order, 0 times; the School A students wrote long(( r clauses than cleft sentence, 23; clauses complementing an School B students (Table 24,a < .001). The adjective 34; clauses complementing a Vs, 23; difference in clause length was not brought "the more the merrier" construction, 4; and about by the experimental treatments but ex- the special which clause, 17.In the post- isted in the prewriting samples as well (Table writing sample these structures appeared 3, 1, 18). Drawing upon the significant differences 14, 36, 16, 3, and 24 times, respectively. between schools shown in the frequency of ad- verb and noun clauses and in clause length, it Clause Length appears that students of School A were writing longer clauses while the trend in School B was The 114 eighth graders from whom the writing towards a higher quantity of shorter adverb and samples were obtained wrote a mean of 7.8 words noun clauses. 25 Table 22 Table 23 Clauses of Comparisonin the Postwriting Clause Length in the Postwriting Using *UsingPrewriting as the C;ovariate ['rewriting as the Covariate-Treatment

3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance x 3 ;-,halysis of ('ovari,Ince

Sum of Su::: of Source Squares df Source Squares df Within Cells 157.26 104 Within Cells 49.16 104 Regression 0.24 1 0.16 ns Regression 11.77 1 24.90 .0.)1 Treatment 1.73 2 0.57 ns Treatment .05 0.06 ns IQ 0.97 2 0.32 ns IQ 1.70 2 1.80 ns Interaction 5.18 4 0.86 ns Interaction .74 4 0.39 ns Means and Adjusted Contrasts Means and Adjusted Contrasts

Factor Level Means Contrast Factor Level Means Contrast Pre Post (x' Pre Post (x' xIl

Treatment Treatment 34 Without Ex, L0 1. 2 + .20 3.4 Without Ex. 7.9 7.8 .023 77 98 29 37 With Ex. 1.2 1.1 .03 37 With Ex. 77 88 6 43 Control 0.5 0.9 .17 43 Control IQ IQ 38 Low 0.9 0.9 .15 38 7.9 7.8 .024 33 Medium 0.7 1.1 + .10 33 Medium 7.6 7.6 .134 43 High 1.0 1.1 + .05 43 High 7.9 8.0 ± .158 Raw Regression Coefficient:-.04 Raw Regression Coefficient: +.36

The regression between prewriting and post- Groups W and WO in School A increased the writing was highly significant, indicating that number of clauses per T-unit, but the other four clause length was a consistent measure of groups showed declines. Thus, the treatment writing behavior.Clause length and the other seems to have affected the Ss in the twoschools four synopsis scores each had a regression slightly differently; and by Newman-Neuls coefficient significant beyond the .001 level. analysis of differences it was shown that Group C in School A was significantly lower than Clauses per 1 -unit Groups W and WO in School A and Group WO in School B < .05). This writing measure showed a significant treatment effect.As shown in Tables 25 and T-unit Length 26, the two experimental groups increased the average number of clauses perT-unit while There were no significant effects due to treat- the control group decreased. The difference ment, sex, or ability with this variable. Only between Groups WO and C was shown by the regression coefficient between pre- and Newman-Keuls analysis to be significant postwriting was significant.Analysis b is < .01) in analyses b and c, which are shown shown in Table 27.The mean number of words in Tables 25 and 26, respectively. When both per T-unit on both prewriting andpostwriting prescore and KAT score were used as covari- was 1 2.2.As Table 27 shows, tais consistent ates ,the effect of treatment was significant mean "ctually represents an increaseby the beyond the .01 level.Group WO wrote more treatm(unt groups and a decrease by the control clauses per T-unit than Group C. Sex and group; although these were notstatistically ability, and interactions of these factors, were significant differences. not significant in any of the analyses of clauses Table 28 presents analysis c,in which per T-unit. school and treatment were tested, and T-unit The clarity of the treatment effect was length in prewriting was the covariate. On the clouded somewhat by the presence of the school- postwriting, Ss in School A wrote longer T-units by-treatment interaction shown in Table 26. than those in School B (p .05). 26 Table 24 Table 26 Clause Length in the Postwriting Using Clauses per T-unit in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatment by SchoolPrewriting as the Covariate-Treatment by School

3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance 3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance

Sum of Sum of Source Squares Of Source Squares(If

Within Cells 14. 33 107 Within Cells 2.66 107 .34 1 13.68 .001 aegression 7.58 1 1 3,16 .001 Regression Treatment 0., 1 ns Treatment .)2 2 4.35 .015 nu 1 ns School 7.91 19.09 1 School 0.12 2 .049 Interaction )-) H . 2 e: Interaction .15 3.09 Means and Adjusted Contrasts :'weans aft! Adjusted Contrasts

Factor Level. :\:eans Lantr :'actor Level Means Contrast Pro Post (x' - Pre Post(x'

Treatment Freatment 1.60 1. 61 + .049 34 Without Ex. 7.9 7.8 .039 34 Without Ex. 7.9 .016 (WO) 37 With Ex. 7.8 + .009 43 Control 7.8 7.8 .(155 37 With Ex. .5 2 1.55 School (W) 1.57 1.50 .05E 63 A 8.1 8.1 4- .".97 43 Control ((1 51 13 7.5 7. .287 School 63 A 1.53 1.54 .002 t-.25 Raw Regression Coefficient: 51 13 1.61 1.56 .002 Table 25 Interaction 21 WO 1.-)8 1.61 .055 Clauses per T-unit in the Postwriting Using 23 + .048 Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatment by I0 \'`: 1.19 1.57 19 - 1.51 1.42 .108 13 ) 1 .6 i 1.61 0 4,08 4

3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance i 1 1 1.58 1.52 1,61 1.56 Sum of Source SquaresOf a Raw Re,uression. .

Within Cells 2.65 104 Regression .34 1 13.33 .001 Treatment ?? 2 4.26 .017 was astabler.asure, as IQ .08 2 1.60 ns shown by the siqnific rew.-ession between Interaction .09 0.86 ns nre,^:111:1'-z ni)stv,Titinc:, the treatment did chw.ge Ss' behavior on Means and Adjusted Contrasts this ir1a;;16,. females were ap- proxin-=atel- and the ability groups Factor Level Contrast did not differ (Table 16). Pro Post (x' 71) T-units per Sentence Treatment 34 \Vithout 1.60 1.61 .0 36 Ss wrote a E.(1:: i1 1.31 and 37 With Ex. 1.52 1.55 .018 per sentence on nreWriti:'- i1postwritinq, 13 Control 1.57 1.50 .054 respectively. T:u, 1:.e.r.s ref the 72 Ss in analy- IQ sis a are shown in fahle1 0. [he difference 38 Low 1.59 1.0 .0u between male a.-,(1 fei:IaleH.; was significant 33 Nledium 1.59 1.51 J018 in analysis a of the noS1Writ.Inn(n .1111, 13 High 1.53 1.51 Analysis 0, in which ilrewri'lnu score .1:1,1 LIU score were used as covariat es showed Raw Regression Coefficient:1-.31 factor of sex not significant. Table 27 Tables 29 and 30 present the other analyses T-unit Length in the Postwriting Using T-units per sentence. Note that even after Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatment by IQ controlling for the linear effect of differences in prewriting, the low ability group tended to 3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance put more T-units into each sentence than the medium and high Ss.The interaction of treat- Sum of ment-by-school was highly significant, and Source Squares df the contrasts in Table 30 show the pattern of P_ the interaction.Individual comparison tests of Within Cells 287. 29 104 the six groups in the interaction showed that Group C in School B and Group W in School A Regression 84.49 1 30.73 .001 Treatment 1 2. 70 2 2.30 .106 wrote more T-units per sentence than Group C IQ 3.29 2 0.60 ns in School A and Group W in School B (a< .05). Interaction 10.99 4 1.00 ns The regression coefficient between T-units per sentence in prewriting and postwriting was Means and Adjusted Contrasts h'.ghly significant, but KAT score, as in all the analyses in which it was used as a covariate, Factor Level Means Contrast accounted for only a minor portion of the post- Pre Post (x' 5-(1) writing variance. Treatment was not significant with this variable. Treatment 34 Without Ex. 12.6 12.6 + .17 Sentence Length 37 With Ex. 11.9 12,2 + .24 43 Control 12.2 11.7 .41 Ss wrote sentences containing a mean of IQ 16.0 and 16.6 words on prewriting and post- 38 Low 12.5 12.5 + .22 writing, respectively. Males tended to write 33 Medium 12.0 11.8 - .22 longer sentences than females (Table 1 6), and 43 High 12.1 12.1 + .00 the sentences of low ability Ss were longer on

Raw Regression Coefficient: +.47 'Fable 29 Table 28 T-units per Sentence in the Postwriting t:sing T-unit Length in the Postwriting Using Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatment by IQ Prewriting as the Covariate--Treatment by School 3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance 3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance Sum of

Sum of Source Squares _cif Li)

Squares df P_ Within Cells 7.92 104 Within Cells 273.74 107 Regression 3.72 1 18.86 .001 Regression 83.56 1 32.66 .001 Treatment .07 2 0.45 ns Treatment 1 2.7 2 2 2.49 .088 IQ .47 2 3.06 .051 School 13.99 1 5.47 .021 interaction .32 1 1.05 ns Interaction 13.89 2 ) 2.72 .071 Means and Adjusted contrasts Means and Adjusted Contrasts Factor Level Means Contrast Factor Level Means Contrast Pre Post (x' Pre Post. (x1 Treatment Treatment 31 WithoutLx. 1.31 1.36 .014

34 Without Ex. 1 26 1 2. 6 4- . 37 With 1:x. 1.30 1.37 .037 37 With Ex. 11.9 12.2 .12 4 i Control 1.30 1.35 + .007 43 Control 12.2 11.7 .36 School 38 Low 1.10 1.53 .091 63 A 1 2. 3 1 2.5 .10 33 NiediHli 1._'ri 1.32 .011 51 B l').1 11.7 - .40 13 High 1...),0 1.21 .11 G7 Raw Regression Coefficient: +.46 Raw Regression Coefficient:F.99

28 Table 30 Table 31 Using Sentence Length in the PostwritingUsing T-units per Sentence in the Postwriting by IQ Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatmentby School Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatment

3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance 3 x 3 Analysis of Covariance

Sum of Sum of Squares df F Source Squaresdf 11 Source Within Cells 8.03 107 Within Cells 1670.16 104 740.18 1 46.09 .001 Regression 4.56 1 60.75 .001 Regression 42.04 2 1.31 ns Treatment .07 2 0.50 ns Treatment 124.23 2 3.87 .024 School .00 1 0.02 ns IQ 39.90 4 0.62 ns Interaction .71 2 4. 72 .011 Interaction Means and Adjusted Covariates Means and Adjusted Contrasts Means Contrast Factor Level Means Contrast Factor Level. Pre Post(x' - X') Pre Post (x' Treatment Treatment 17.0 17.3 .33 34 Without Ex. 1.34 1.36 .028 34 Without Ex. 16.8 + .98 (WO) 37 With Ex. 15.3 15.7 15.7 .65 37 With Ex. 1.30 1.37 + .016 43 Control (W) IQ 17.4 19.2 + 1.56 43 Control (C) 1.30 1.35 '4- .012 38 Low 33 Medium 15.4 15.4 .78 School 43 High 15.1 15.1 .78 63 A 1.30 1.35 .003 51 B 1.32 1.37 .003 Raw Regression Coefficient: .78 School Treatment 21 WO A 1.38 1.41 .014 A 1.28 1.42 +.109 23 W Individual compari- A 1.25 1.20 .086 and is shown in 'fable 32. 19 C interaction groups showed that B 1.29 1.28 .041 son of the six 13 WO higher than Groups 14 W B 1.34 1.30 .07P, Group W in School A was W and WO in School B and GroupC in School 24 C B 1.33 1.47 +.109 A (.2_ .05) .The regression between prewrit- Raw Regression coefficient: +1.05 ing and postwriting sentencelength was highly significant.

postwriting than the sentences.:61the high and Total of All Subordinate Clauses medium groups (Tables 1 6 and31). These facts The tabulations of all elevenkinds of clause may indicate a greater useof coordination by single score males and low ability Ss ratherthan use of structures were combined into a Using prewriting totaland subordination. for each S. Treatment was not significant,but an inter- score as covariates,the postwriting total was action of school-by-treatment wassignificant analyzed in a 3 x2design. The difference Table 32 among treatment groupswas nearly statistically Sentence Length in the PostwritingUsing significant (.05 < < . 10). The experimental Prewriting as the Covariate-Treatmentby School groups, and especially GroupWO, increased their use of subordinationabove that of the control group (See 3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance Table 33). Individual com- parisons }Ielyeengroups showed that themean of Group WO Sum of was higher than Group C (2< .05) Source Squares df F Q Table 33 Within Cells 1693.23 107 The Sum of the Eleven ClauseStructures in the Regression 830.07 1 52.45 .001 Postwriting Using Prewriting andIQ as the Treatment 31.15 2 0.98 ns Covariates School 32.27 1 2.04 ns Interaction 167.13 2 5.28 .007 3 x 2 Analysis of Covariance

Means and Adjusted Covariates Sum of Source Squares df F Factor Level Means Contrast Pre Post(x' - X') Within Cells 7196.68 88 Regression Treatment 724.13 2 4.43 .015 Treatment 431.44 2 2.64 .077 34 Without Ex. 17.0 17.3 - .025 Sex 22.08 1 .27 (WO) ns Interaction 330.30 2 2.02 37 With Ex. 15.3 16.8 + .465 ns (W) Means and Adjusted Contrasts 43 Control (C) 15.7 15.7 .440 School Factor Level Means Contrast 63 A 15.9 17.0 + .668 Pre Post (x' X') 51 B 16.0 15.9 - .668 Treatment Interaction 34 Without Ex. 46.5 48.1 21 WO + 2.64 A 17.5 18.5 +1.058 37 With Ex. 43.7 23 W 44.5 - 0.08 A 15.5 18.2 +2.357 43 Control 44.7 42.6 - 2.56 19 C A 14.7 13.9 -1.411 Sex 13 WO B 16.1 15.2 -1.108 63 Male 43.5 45.5 + 0.49 14 W B 15.4 14.4 -1.427 51 Female 46.4 44.6 - 0.49 24 C B 16.3 17.1 + .531 Raw Regression Coefficients: Raw Regression Coefficient: +.76 Prewriting,+ .19; IQ, .13 V DISCUSSION AND SUMMARY

covariate, reveate follo,..^:incr effects in the SUMMARY pretests and posttests: Themes were collected fromeighth-grade 1.There were no differences amongCroups students until each studenthad produced a W, WO, and C' on PretestI,Pretest II, or on 1000-word sample of writing.Students were the sentence-combining tasksof Pretest III. then placed in an experimentalsituation which 2.Females scored higher thanmales on lasted for five weeks .During these five weeks, Pretest I and Pretest li(I" < .011 and on Pretest students were Ss in either anexperimental or III (R < .05). control condition. The experimentalcondition 3.There were no significantinteractions of was a series of 22programed lessons in struc- sex-by-treatment on the pretests. tural and transformational grammar.One ex- 4.The regression coefficientsbetween KAT perimental group (WO) receivedonly these 22 score and pretest scoreswere not significant. lessons, a second experimental group(W) re- 5.There were significantdifferences be- ceived the lessons plus writingexercises which tween treatment groups onPosttest I and Post- followed each lesson.The control group (C) test II.Groups W and WO scoredsignificantly did not study grammarduring the five-week higher than Groua C (o <.1)1) on both posttests. period but had units onliterature and speaking. No differences, though,existed between W and A pretest was given toall Ss on the first day WO. On the sentence-combiningtasks (Post- of the experiment. Theexperimental groups test III) no significantdifferences among Groups studied 10 daily lessons onstructural grammar W, \AI°, and C appeared. which consisted of basic sentencepatterns and 6.Females scored higher than males on the structures within each pattern.The pretest Posttests 1 and II < .011, but no significant was given as a posttestfollowing these 10 les- sex differences werepresent on Posttest III. sons.A second pretest and asentence- 7.There were no significantsex-by- combining task preceded theremaining 12 treatment interactions on anyof the posttests. lessons, which were a studyof transform pro- 8.The regression coefficientsbetween NAT cesses. This secondpretest was used as a scores and posttest scores werenot significant. second posttest on the lastday of the experi- ment.Writing samples amounting to1000 A more thorough analysisof the posttests, an words were again collectedfrom all students analysis which used all 207 Ss,pretest score following the experiment. as a covariate,and tested the factors ofability The pretests and posttests of207 Ss were sex, school,and experimental vs. control, analyzed by analysis of covariancefor signifi- showed the following: cant effects due to treatment,ability, sex, postwriting samples 1. The experimental Ssscored higher than and school. The pre- and and II (R - .001) of 114 Ss were analyzedby analysis of covari- the control Ss on Posttest I The covariate and on Posttest < .05). ance for these sameeffects. males on was either the 10 scoreon the Kuhlmann- 2.Females scored higher than Anderson Test or the prescore.l'or some Posttest II < .011, but no sexdifferences and orescore appeared on Posttests I and III. analyses of postscores, both 10 measured by KAT, were used as covariates. 3.Ss of high ability, as tested the effects scored higher than Ss ofmedium ability who The initial analysis, which of low ability on of sex and treatment, andused NAT score as a in turn scored higher than Ss

.31 Posttests I and II(2. < .01). The trend was sentence. On postwriting, Group W wrote more similar but not significant on Posttest III. adjective clauses than Group C < .05), and 4.Ss of School A scored higher than those Groups W and WO wrote more clauses per 1- of School B on Posttest I (2.< .01) and Post- unit than Group < .05). test III(2. < .001). No differences were pres- 2.Males wrote more T-units per sentence ent on Posttest II. than females on the prewriting (2. < .05) and 5. A treatment-by-school interaction was on postwriting (2, e .01).Males wrote longer significant on Posttest I (2 < .01).It appeared sentences than females on postwriting that the experimental Ss of School A gained No other measures showed significant differ- more from the treatment than the experimental. ences between sexes on pre- or postwriting. Ss of School B. A treatment-by-IQ interaction 3.Ss below the median in ability wrote was significant on Posttest III (2 < .05) and more noun clauses than Ss above the median also an ability-by-school interaction (2.< .05). on the prewriting sample (E. < .01).This dif- 6.The regression coefficients between ference disappeared on postwriting and no other pretest and posttest score were highly signifi- differences between ability levels were present. cant (2 < .001) for all three sets of tests. 4.There was a significant three-way inter- action in the number of noun clauses written in The posttests were again analyzed using the prewriting sample (2. < .05) and in the num- both KAT score and pretest score as covariates. ber of adverb clauses written in the postwriting The factors tested were school, sex, and treat- sample (2. < .05). ment (W vs. WO vs. C). The sum of Posttests 5. The regression coefficients between KAT I and II was also analyzed. This design using score and the various writing measures were not 144 Ss revealed the following: significant. 1.Groups W and WO scored higher than Group C on Posttests I and II and the sum of The postwriting measures were analyzed I and II (p < .01). No significant treatment again, this time using prewriting scores as the differences were found on Posttest III. covariate and 114 Ss. The factors of ability 2.Females scored higher than males on and treatment were considered. The results Posttests I and II and the sum of I and II showed: < .05). No significant sex differences 1.The measure of clauses per T-unit was were found on Posttest III. the only measure revealing a significant differ- 3.Ss of School A scored higher than Ss ence between experimental and control groups. of School B on Posttests I and II(2. < .01) and W and WO wrote more clauses per T-unit than on the sum of Posttests I and II(2. < .001). Group,C (2. < .05). No significant school differences were found 2.Low ability Ss wrote longer sentences on Posttest III. than medium and high ability Ss (2. < .05). No 4.There was a treatment-by-school inter- other significant differences existed between action on Posttest I and the sum of Posttests ability groups. I and II(2. < .01).Experimental Ss in School 3.No significant interactions were present A scored higher than the experimental Ss of on any of the postwriting measures. School B.There was a school-by-sex inter- 4.The regression coefficients between pre- action on Posttest III (2. < .05). and postwriting measures were significant for 5.The regression coefficients between the six of the measures: adverb clauses (2.< .05), covariates and the posttest scores were sig- clause length, clauses per T-unit, T-unit length, nificant for each posttest and the sum of Post- T-units per sentence and sentence lenc.'.h < .001). tests I and II (2. < .001). Another analysis of covariance tested the The writing scores from the 1000-word pre- factors of school and treatment. Using 114 Ss, writing samples and the 1000-word postwriting and with prewriting score as the covariate, the samples were analyzed for the factors sex, analysis revealed: ability, and treatment.Nine writing measures were anlayzed with a 2 x 2 x 3 design using 1.The number of clauses per T-unit was the KAT score as a covariate. The results showed: only measure revealing a significant difference between experimental and control groups. W 1. No significant differences between treat- and WO wrote more clauses per T-unit than ment groups on any of the prewriting measures, Group C < .05). which included adjective, adverb, and noun 2.The students in School A wrote fewer clauses and clauses of comparison, and meas- adverb clauses (2. < .06), fewer noun clauses, ures of clause length, sentence length, T-unit longer clauses (E. < .001), and longer T-units length, clauses per T-unit, and T-units per (2. < .05) than students in School B.

32 3.There was a significant treatment -by -- Measures of the error rates and the time school interaction in four of the postwriting requirements of each lesson showed: measures: noun clauses, clauses per T-unit (p. < .05), T-units per sentence, and sentence 1.All lessons except. \o. 14 had an error rate below . length (p. < .01). 2.The error rate for ss below the median 4.The regression coefficients between pro- KA!' score Was aporoximaIely twice the error and postwriting measures were significant for six of the measures: adverb clauses (p_< .05), jitr? of Ks iii)u ,/tLt-in 3.The average rime spent on a lesson W119 clause length, clauses per T-unit, T-unit 19.8 minutes.Ss below the median on 1AT T-units per sentence, and sentence length Iveraged 20,6 minutes, S. above the median, < 001). I 9,1 minutes. A fourth analysis of postwriting measures used both KAT and prewriting as eovariates. CONCLUSIONS Treatment and sex were the factors tested using 96 Ss. The sum of all clatiscs was analyzed Ss in Groups W and WO learned concepts of using this design. The results were: structural and transformational grammar. They were able to use the vocabulary of these gram- 1.On the measure, clauses per T-unit, Group mars, apply the concepts in test situations, WO was higher than Group C < .01).Group and, to some extent, use transform processes WO was higher than Group C when the total of to rewrite a series of basic sentences. There all clause structures was considered (p- .05). was some evidence that the learning of these No other significant differences between treat- concepts transferred to Ss' independent writing. ment c _,ups were present. In the postwriting, more clauses per T-unit were 2.No significant sex differences appeared written by the experimental Ss than by the con- in any of the measures. trol Ss.This difference indicated that the use 3.No significant interactions were shown. 4. The regression coefficients between the of subordination was influenced by the experi- covariates and postwriting measures were sig- mental treatment. When the total number of nificant in the analysis of adverb clauses and eleven kinds of subordinate clauses was con- sidered, the average S in Croups W, WO, and clauses per T-unit < .01), clause length, C' wrote 44,5, 49.1, and 42.6 clauses, respec- T-unit length, sentence length, and T-units per tively, in 1000 words of writing. Though no sentence (p. <.001),and the sum of all clause differences were significant in the other meas- structures (p_ < .05). ures of the postwriting samples, all differences pointed in the direction of greater writing matu- In addition to tests and writing samples, the rity for the expern:.ental (iroups.It must be experimental Ss responded to a questionnaire kept in mind that the present experimental treat- which asked for opinions on aspects of the ex- ment required a rather short daily study period perimental treatment.This questionnaire re- on each of 22 days. The average student spent vealed the following results: 7 to 8 hours working with the programed lessons. 1.The length, the pace, and the vocabulary This was a comparatively brief and short-term were judged appropriate by the majority of stu- exposure to a set of new concepts. A longer dents. time period with more extensive and intensive 2.Most students peeked at answers a small training and practice in applying the concepts number of times. may have produced a more apparent increase in 3.Students found the summary sheets helpful. writing maturity.The fact that the concepts had 4.Almost all students thought that the pro- been grasped, however, was shown in the im- gramed lessons were more interesting and more provement of the experimental groups from pre- helpful than the traditional approach. test to posttest.Mor.:over, the students felt. 5.The items rated most helpful were the that the programs were a pleasant break from the clues for recognizing nouns,verbs,adjectives, usual teacher lecture, and that many of 'he con- and adverbs, and the test-sentences.Basic cepts would be useful to them. sentences and transform processes were not The differences between the two experimental rated as highly. groups , W and WO, were minimal. On some

6.Basic sentence patterns were judged easier measures, adjective clauses and Posttests { than the transform processes.Particular transformand II, the group that 1_:sed the exercises and processes were all judged about erpialL- difficn1:, received some teJ1cher feedhacl tended to score 7.Students believed that programed lessons higher, but on other measures, adverb clases, with teacher's helo would he the preferred learn- noun clauses, clauses per T-unit, total clauses, ing situation. sentence length, and Posttest III, the group that 33 1

used only the programed lessons was higher. and high ability Ss. Medium Ss were highest However, none of these differences was sta- in the number of adjective and noun clauses, tistically significant.Thus, the use of exer- while the high Ss wrote the longest clauses. cises as supplements to the programed lessons Aside from these small differences on the writing may have some slight justificationfrom the re- measures, the pre- and posttests showed the sults, but the major portion of teaching was expected differences due to ability.The total already achieved by the programed lessons. scores for low, medium, and highability Ss on In absolute terms the changes caused by the the pretests were 47.0, 60.8, and 76.4, re- experimental treatment were not as great as spectively. On the posttests the totals were anticipated. On Posttest I and II, the experi- 55.0, 68.9, and 85.2, respectively. mental groups gained approximately eight and The school variable was difficult to inter- four points, respectively.For the combined pret, or rather, its presence as a significant tests the 12-point improvement represented one- variable was difficult to justify.Since pro- eighth of the questions ,or alternatively, it was grarLed lessons minimize the role of the teacher, equivalent to an increase of 27/ over pretest no significant differences betweenTeacher A score. On the two sentence-combiningprob- in School A and Teacher B in School 13 were ex- lems, the experimental groups achieved an in- pected. However, School A Ss scored three crease in clause length averaging 2.8words points higher on the total of the posttests than from pretest to posttest.This increase repre- did School B Ss.Interactions showed that ex- sented about one-eighth of the embedding pos- perimental Ss in School A gained especially from sible, or an increase of 18/, over pretest score. the lessons.Other results from writing meas- Writing measures showed even less change in ures showed that Ss in School A seemed tobe most instances, and apparently the treatments more mature writers at the end of theexperi- were not highly successful in achievingposi- mental treatment than the Ss in School 13.School tive transfer on writing behavior. The use of A Ss were writing longer clauses, T-units, and subordinate clauses increased 2.7A, clause sentences while School B Ss were using more length remaine ' the same cit 7.9 words , clauses adverb and noun clauses.Interactions again per T-unit increased 1.3%), T-unit length re- seemed to imply that Ss in School A gained from mained approximately the same at 12.4 words, the experimental treatment to a greater extent T-units per sentence increased 3.4% rather than than those in School B. We have no way of declining as intended, and sentence length in- knowing what caused this difference between creased 5.6A. the two school populations.It may have been There were some slight differences between simply the result of one teacher helping the male and female students. These differences students adjust to the programed learning situa- tended to show that the males were slightly less tion, and the other teacher achieving this adjust- mature writers than the females and that their ment to a lesser degree. achievement was less on the tests.Males One purpose of the present study was to averaged 6.5 points less on Posttests I and II show that eighth graders in a normal school than females, differences that were evident on setting would learn concepts of structural and the pretests.The males did less embedding on transformational grammar when these concepts the sentence-combining problems (approximately were presented in a programed format.After two words). On the writing measures, males five weeks, representing approximately 8 hours tended to show immaturity by writing slightly of actual study, Ss who worked with such pro- longer sentences and more T-units per sentence. gramed lessons scored significantly higher on The use of subordination by both sexes was not tests than their classmates in a control condi- significantly different.The experimental treat- tion.In the writing of themes there was a sig- ment affected males and females similarly, and nificant difference favoring the experimental no special adjustments seem necessaryin the Ss in the use of subordination. Other measures programed lessons. of writing maturity favored the experimental One surprising result was the lack of signifi- groups also, but were not significant.Thus, cant differences between different ability levels the second purpose, transfer of knowledge to on the writing measures. Theonly significant the writing situation, was not fully achieved. differences showed low ability Ss writing longer The transfer and use of grammatical knowledge sentences and more T-units per sentence than in the writing of themes has historically been the medium and high ability Ss.This implies difficult to demonstrate.It may in fact require that the low Ss were slightly less mature writers. a much longer period of time than five weeks. However, the low Ss wrote longer T-units, more Achieving transfer may require extensive drill clauses per T-unit, and more adverb clauses (in in combining previously prepared kernel sen- all cases not significantly more) than medium tences. Such pseudoproduction of complex

34 [;PO 811-289-4 transformed sentences has been shown to in- present study and in the sentence-combining crease the complexity of the student's inde- tasks of the Mellon study would, if applied for pendent writing, though the overall quality of a period of at least one school year, produce themes is not enhanced (Mellon, 1967).Per- substantial increases in the quality and maturity haps a combination of the methods used in the of student writing.

35 APPENDOX A GENERAL OUTLINE OF CONCEPTS IN PROGRAM

BASIC SENTENCE PATTERNS Lesson 8:Adjectives with Patterns 5 and 6 AND FORM CLASSES Vs verb like seem Vb verb like become Lesson A Look at Basic Sentences adj: markers basic sentence position subject and predicate groups endings y, -able, NP ish, -en, noun test-sentence ful, -ous Lesson 2: Basic Sentence Patterns 1 and 2 Lesson 9: Basic Sentence Pattern 7 and 8 forms of be two-word verbs completer adj test-sentence pronouns as completers Lesson 10:Basic Sentence Pattern 9 and Lesson 3: Basic Sentence Pattern 3 and Review General. Review adv-place, adv-time test pattern to distinguish prep phrase Pattern 8 and 9 Lesson 4: Basic Sentence Pattern 4 VP TRANSFORMS-EXPAND 1NG THE NP verb (identify by position) adv-manner Lesson 11:Introduction to Transform optional adv Sentences and Related Clauses NP expansion Lesson 5: More About Nouns transform sentence vs. noun markers basic sentence plural form transform (as a method) pronouns related clause Lesson 6: Recognizing Verbs relating pronoun verb markers Lesson 12:T-rel (transform process for verb forms and endings: related clauses) simple (to), present (-sl, T-rel process:Step 1 and past)(-d, -ed), Step 2 past2(-n, -en) with have, has, had insert and base -ing form Lesson 13:T-rel Again with form of be relating adverb use of whom Lesson 7: Recognizing Adverbs position Lesson 14:T-rel after T-pos (transform process adv markers for possessive construction) adv endings:-ly, -wise, possessive NP -ward(s) use of whose

37 Lesson 15; T-pass (transform process for TRANSFORMS-REPLAC1NG THE NP passive construction) Lesson 19: T-NC (transform process for con- structing noun clauses) Lesson 16: T-ph (transform process for phrase noun clause constructions) -ing phrase Lesson 20:T-Vto (transform process for con- past2 phrase structing Vto nominals) prep phrase nominal Vto Lesson 17; T-BN (transform process for placing Vto phrase single words before the noun) Lesson 21: T-Ving (transform process for con- structing -ing nominals) Lesson 18: T-NP and Double Nouns (transform Ving process for placing one NP after Ving phrase another) double nouns Lesson 22;Adj and Adv Nominals and Review of Nominals

38 APPENDIX B EXAMPLE WORKSHEET

A. Use each V listed below in a Pattern 4 sen- avoid sounding uninteresting and monoto- tence (`,.P-I-V) by supplying an tiP subject nous. Whether you expand en with a group. You may use an ootional ad,' after related clause or with a phrase depends the V if you wish. on what you want to say and what you think is a more pleasing way to say it. . crackled Tech sentence below contains a related clause.TIe ,formation night be told in a ,._lifferen-t equally acceptable, B. For each cf the sentences below, write in waz ire(hicin(7 the related clause to a a V of your own.If there are verb markers i)hrase. '1 -ph, rewrite eac1.- given, be careful to use the appropriate tr(712:-, form sentence. verb form. l'he ton.atoes t nit were picked yes- 1. Aunt Hilda in the garden teria., are rioe. yesterday.

t an u:',acinary story you are Using T-rel, combine the insert and the writinq ta::es :dace in ':\lexico.In one base for each pair of sentences. part of the star, , you want to describe a small e7ent, and you want to use the in- 1. Base:< The snow paralyzed the forinatio:, ,:iven below.in a more inter- Insert: The snow fell last night. esting tell this same inforration. any tr 'storm to place par:.s D. almost anything you write, you will of one sentence iiimother sentence. probably have a chance to use one NP to expand anotherin a report for science, I he don:-.ev trudged ,)own the a a paragraph for social studies, en essay the donkey was urea._- for English.Could a term be clarified? or Ike donkey was pl]Iling a cart. a definition made more precise and inforn,a- The cart was wooden. Live? Write three sentences, making each The cart was hasty. one contain an NP expanded by another. :-!everal children ran at his side.-: Hie children were laughing.- The children had straight hair and F. In addition to using related clauses, you sparkling, black eyes may often use various kinds of phrases to

jcj APPENDIX C

SUMMARY SHEET FOR LESSON I

For the first ten lessons, we will be working S (sentence) with nine kinds of very simple sentences called BASIC sentences. NP subject group) All English sentences have two main parts, predicate group which are called the SUBJECT GROUP and PREDICATE GROUP. Many kinds of structures (the, a, noun can act as the subject group of a sentence, some, but in basic sentences the subject group is many, always a NOUN PHRASE, or NP. etc.) If an NP consists of more than one word, the NP, too, can be divided into two main A noun isa word like carnivals, butterfly, structures. One of the structures may be des- happiness, and Joe.Later you will learn sev- cribed as a NOUN. The following sentence eral other clues for recognizing nouns.For "tree" may help you see more clearly how the now a fairly reliable way to determine whether structures of a sentence are related to each or not you can describe a certain word as a other.As we go along we will add other noun is to use it in the noun test-sentence: "branches" to the "tree." < (The) is /are here.>

40 APPENDIX D STUDENT QUESTIONNAIRE

In answering these questions, please give the 7.How do you feel about the helpfulnes6 of answer which truly says how you feel. the summary sheet after every lesson? Very helpful 1. How do you feel about the length of the Somewhat helpful lessons? Not helpful Too long Just right 8. How often did you refer back to the sum- Too short mary sheet of a previous lesson? Not at all 2. Flow interesting were the lessons to you? One to five times Very interesting More than five times Somewhat interesting Not interesting 9.To what extent is this new grammar more interesting than what you have been learn- 3. How do you feel about doing one lesson ing? per day? Very much Too fast A little Comfortable pace Not at all Too slow 10. To what degree do you feel that what you 4.To what extent do you feel that this new learned about the following things will be kind of grammar is more helpful to you helpful to you in the future? than what you have been learning? Very Somewhat Not Very much helpful helpful helpful A little a. basic Not at all sentences b. test- 5. How frequently did you peek at the correct sentences answer to a frame before writing down your c, clues for own answer? recognizing Not at all a noun, verb, One to three times a lesson adj,adv More than three times d. transform processes 6 How do you feel about the vocabulary used for combining in the lessons? shorter sen- fences to Very difficult expand an NI) Somewhat difficult (T-rel, Not difficult 1' -ph, c

.11 Very Somewhat Not 16. About how many times did your teacher helpful helpful helpful have to help explain the directions for e. transform the worksheets? processes One to three times for making Three to five times nominals More than five times (T-NC, T-Vto, 17. To what extent did the worksheets help T-Ving) you understand the lesson better? Substantially 11. Did you do a worksheet after each lesson? To some extent Very little Yes GO TO QUESTION 112 No GO TO QUESTION 18 18. Decide which of the items listed below was the most difficult for you to learn, 12. To what extent did the worksheets help and then write "1" next to it.Rank the you to see how you could use what you others in decreasing difficulty.The had learned in the lessons? least difficult will then be "8." Substantially T-rel (related clauses) To some extent basic sentence patterns Very little subject group and predicate group T-pass (passive construction) 13. For the lessons on transforms, what kind T-NC (noun clauses) of exercise did you like best? T-Vto (Vto nominals) T-BN (single words before a noun) Combining a series of 3 or more sentences Breaking down a transform sentence (Write in your own choice.) into basic sentences 19. How would you like to have new material Free writing on anything I chose presented to you? Rank the following alternatives, assigning "1" to the method 14. Ilow often were you NOT able to finish a you think would help you most. lesson and a worksheet within one class programed lessons alone period? programed lessons with teacher's One to three times he] p Three to five times standard textbook form I always finished teacher lecturing

15. What do you think about the directions 20. What advice can you give which would help given to you on the worksheets? in revising the lessons, worksheets, and summary sheets? Usually clear Sometimes not clear Usually not clear What other comments do you have? BIBLIOGRAPHY

Bateman, D. R. & Zidonis, F. J.The effect of Teachers of English Research Report No. 3. a study of transformational grammar onthe Champaign, Illinois:National Council of writing of ninth and tenth graders.National Teachers of English, 1965. Council of Teachers of English Research Hunt, K. W. Sentence structures used by Report No. 6.Champaign, Illinois: National superior students in grades four and twelve, Council of Teachers of English, 1966. and by superior adults.Project 5-0313, Blake, R. W. Linguistics and punctuation. Cooperative Research Program, Office of English Record, 1964, 15, 9-13. Education, U. S. Department of Health, Blount, N. S., Johnson, S. L., & Fredrick, Education, and Welfare, Tallahassee: W. C. A comparison of the writing of Florida State University, n.d. eighth- and twelfth-grade students. Tech- Johnson, F. S.Structural versus non-structural nical Report from the Wisconsin Research teaching.College Composition and Communi- and Development Center for Cognitive Learn- cation, 1960, 11, 214-215. ing, University of Wisconsin, in press. Kraus, S. The teaching of written composition Blount, N. S., Klausmeier, H. 5., Johnson, in the public schools: A summary of research. S. L. ,Fredrick, W. C., & Ramsay, T. C. University of Oregon Curriculum Bulletin, The effectiveness of programed materials in 1959, 15, No. 190. English syntax and the relationship of se- Loban, W. D. The language of elementary lected variables to the learning of concepts. school children.National Council of Technical Report from the Wisconsin Research Teachers of English Research Report No. I. and Development Center for Cognitive Learn- Champaign, Illinois:National Council of ing, University of Wisconsin, 1967, No. 17. Teachers of English, 1963. Braddock, R. , Lloyd-Jones, R., & Schoer, L. Meckel, IL C.Research on teaching compo- Research in Written Composition. Cham- sition and literature.In N. L. Gage (Ed.), paign, Illinois: National Council of Teachers Handbook of Research on Teaching: A Project of English, 1963. of the American Educational Research Associ- Frogner, E. Grammar approach versus thought ation. Chicago: Rand McNally & Co., 1963. approach in teaching sentence structure. Mellon, J. C. Transformational sentence- English Journal, 1939, 28, 518-526. combining: A method for enhancing thede- Gale, I. F.An experimental study of two fifth- velopment of syntactic fluency inEnglish grade language-arts programs: An analysis composition.Project 5-8418, Cooperative of the writing of children taught linguistic Research Program, Office of Education, grammars compared to thosetaught traditional U. S. Department of Health, Education, grammar.Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 28, and Welfare, Cambridge, Massachusetts: 4156A. Harvard University, 1967. Harris, R. J.An experimental inquiry into the Menyuk, P.Alternation of rules in children's functions and value of formal grammar in the grammar. Journal of VerbalLearning and teaching of English, with special reference Verbal Behavior, 1964, 3, 480-488. to the teaching of correct written English to Menyuk, P.Syntactic structures in the 1,7nc:nage children aged twelve to fourteen. Unpub- of children.Child Development, 1963, 31, lished Ph. P. thesis, University of London, 407-422. 1962. Milligan, J. P.An evaluation of two methods Ilunt, F. W. Grammatical structures written of teaching written sentence structure.He- at three grade levels.National Council of mentary English Review, 1939, 16,9 I -9'2, d3 O'Donnell, R. C. The correlation of awareness Sam, N. II. & Stine, E. S.Structural analysis of the of structural relationships in English and written composition of intermediate grade chil- ability in written composition.Project No. dren.Project No. S-057, Cooperative Research 1524, Cooperative Research Program, Office Program, Office of Education, U. S. Department of Education, U. S. Department of Health, of Health, Education, and Welfare, Bethlehem, Education, and Welfare, Mount Olive, North Pennsylvania:Lehigh University, 1.965. Carolina: Mount Olive College, 1963. Schuster, E. H. How good is the new grammar? O'Donnell, R. C., Griffin, W. J., & Norris, English Journal, 1961, 50, 392-397. R. C.Syntax of kindergarten and elemen- Stanley, J. C.Measurement in Today's Schools. tary school children: A transformational Fourth Edition. Englewood Cliffs, New analysis.National Council of Teachers of Jersey:Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1964. English Research Report No. 8.Champaign, Strickland, R. G. The language of elementary Illinois: National Council of Teachers of school children:Its relationship to the English, 1967. language of reading textbooks and the quality Riling, M. E.Oral and written language of of reading of selected children.Bulletin of children in grades 4 and 6 compared with the School of Education, Indiana University, the language of their textbooks.Project 1962, 38, 1-131. No. 2410, Cooperative Research Program, Suggs, L. R.Structural grammar versus tradi- Office of Education, U. S. Department of tional grammar in influencing writing.English Health, Education, and Welfare, Durant, Journal, 1961, 50, 174-178. Oklahoma: Southeastern State College, 1965.

44 DCCUMENT RESUME

ED 036 516 TE 001 709

AUTHOR MILLER, BAEBARA D. ;NEY, JAMES W. TITLE THE EFFECT OF SYSTEMATIC ORAL EXERCISES ON THE WRITING OF FOURTH-GRADE STUDENTS. INSTITUTION NATICNAL CCUNCII OF TEACHERS OF ENGLISH, CHAMPAIGN, TLL. PUB DATE 68 NOTE I9P. JOURNAL CIT RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING Of ENGLISH; V2 N1 P44-61 SPR 1968

EDES PRICE EDES PRICE MF-$0.25 HC NOT AVAILABLE FROM EDES. DESCRIPTORS COMPOSITION (LITERARY), *COMPOSITION SKILLS (LITERARY), *ENGLISH INSTRUCTION, EXPOSITORY WRITING, *GRADE 4, *PATTERN LRILLS (LANGUAGE), SENTENCES, *SENTENCE STEUCTURE, TEACHING METHODS, VERBAL COMMUNICATION, WRITING, WRITING EXERCISES, WRITING SKILLS ABSTRACT TO DETERMINE THE EFFECTS OF ORAL SENTENCE-STRUCTURE EXERCISES UPCN STUDENT WRITING, A 1-YEAR EXPERIMENT WAS CONDUCTED WITH RANDOMLY MATCHED FOURTH-GRADE CLASSES OF APPROXIMATELY EQUAL CNE CONTROL CLASS AND CNE EXPERIMENTAL CLASS. THE EXPERIMENTAL GRCUP REGULARLY COMPLETED EXERCISES EESIGNID TO TEACH STUDENTS TO PRODUCE SENTENCES INCORPORATING (1) "WHO" AND "WHICH" ADJECTIVAL CLAUSES OE ELEMENTS DERIVED FROM THESE CLAUSES, (2) ADVERBIAL CLAUSES IN INITIAL AND FINAL POSITION, AND (3) NOMINALIZATIONS IN THE SUBJECT AND PEEDICATE. WRITTEN EXERCISES BASED CN THE ORAL DRILLS WERE COMPLETED AT THE END OF EACH CLASS PEEIOD. EEFORE AND AFTER EACH OF THE TWO SEMESTER-LONG EXPERIMENTAL PHASES, TESTS WERE ADMINISTERED IN WHICH STUDENTS WROTE A 30-MINUTE IMPROMPTU COMPOSITION ABOUT A SHORT FILM. iiESULTS SHOWED THAT STUDENTS IN THE EXPERIMENTAL GROUP WROTE MOLE WORDS IN LESS TIME, USED MCEE CF THE PRACTICED SENTENCE STRUCTURES, ANL USED A GREATER PROPORTION OF COMPLEX SENTENCES THAN DID CCNTROI GECUP STUDENTS. CONSEQUENTLY, THE ORAL DRILLS AND SUPPLEMENTARY WRITTEN EXERCISES WERE CONCLUDED TO HAVE FAVORABLY AFFECTED THE WRITING CF FCUETH GEADERS. (TABLES OF FINDINGS ARE INCLUDED.) (JE) "PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL BY MICROFICHE ONLY HAS BEEN GRANTED BY TIE: ,AAA.-oe a / TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U. S, OFFICE OF EDUCATION. FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REQUIRES PERMISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT OWNER." RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OF ENGLISH

VOLUME 2, NUMBER 1, SPRING 1968

Contents ARTICLES 5Some thoughts on research and the teaching of English by Roland Harris

14Something for everyone: A standard corpus of contemporary American expository essays by Margaret E. Ashida

24The sensitivity of speakers of standard English to usage by lack R. Cameron

32National or mother language in beginning reading: A comparative study by Nancy Modiano

44The effect of systematic oral exercises on the writing of fourth -grade students by Barbara D. Miller and James W. Ney

DEPARTMENTS r 63Roundtable review 65Notes and comment

71Bibliography of research in the teaching of English: July 1, 1967-December 31, 1967 by Nathan S. Blount U,S, DEPARTMENT OFHEALTH, EDUCATION & WELFARE OFFICE Of EDUCATION

THIS DOCUMENT HAS KEW REPRODUCED EXACTLY ASRECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIGINATING IT.POINTS OF VIEW OR OPINIONS STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICEOf EDUCATION POSITION OR POLICY. After giving a class of fourth graders regular oral practice in the combining ofsentences, the authors found that students wrotemore complex sentences than a control group which had no such practice,

The effect of systematic oral exercisesonthe writing of fourth-grade students BARBARA D. MILLER Okemos Public Schools, Michigan JAMES W. NEY Michigan State University

Although a number of studies have investigated the relation- ship of a knowledge of grammar, especially thenewer gram- mars such as transformational grammar, on the writing ability of school students, very few have attemptedto examine the effect of the students' manipulation of grammaticalstructures on their ability to write. Recently, Wardhaught and Bateman and Zidonis2 have investigated the effect ofa knowledge of transformational grammar on writing. (Both writersgive a summary of previous research using other types of grammars.) Fisher,3 however, reports a study dealing with the raanipula- 1R. Wardhaugh, "Ability in written composition and transforma- tional grammar," The Journal of Educational Research, 1967, 60, 427- 429. 2D. R. Bateman and F. J. Zidonis, The effectof a study of trans- formational grammar on the writing of ninth and tenth graders (Cham- paign, 111.: NCTE, 1966). 3J. C. Fisher, Linguistics in remedial English (The Hague:Mouton, 1966). 44 lion of grammatical structures by remedial writing students on the college level and the effect of these manipulative exercises on the writing of the students hi the experiment Similar ex- periments in the grades have been performed by Ney4 and Raub.5 These experiments have been discussed in detail by Griffin,6 None of these studies, however, was performed with a large group of elementary school students over an extended period of time. Thus, through the entire 1966-1967 academic year, a classroom experiment was conducted to determine the effect of systematic oral language exercises on the writing of fourth-grade students in a typical suburban middle class school. EXPERIMENTAL The experimental design called for two randomly matched DESIGNfourth-grade classes with an approximately equal number of students in each class. One of these classes functioned as an experimental group, the other as a control group. The research- er, serving as the instructor, subjected the experimental group to a predetermined amount of oral language drilling with ex- ercises designed to foster transfer of training to writing. The control group pursued a normal course of studies (reading, and writing free compositions) without being subjected to any amount of the oral drilling which formed the basis of the ex- perimental methodology. The effectiveness of the oral drilling was measured on pretests and posttests based on the method- ology developed by O'Donnell, Griffin, and Norris for eniting language from elementary school children.? In short, this meth- odology consisted of the elicitation of oral or written language by the showing of a film. Since, in this experiment, the pur- pose was to study only written langto ge, in the pretests and posttests, the students wrote a half hour long impromptu com- position about the film shown. These compositions-were then subjected to an intensive analysis using the measurements de- 4 J. W. Ney, "Applied linguistics in the seventh grade," English Journal, 1966, 55, 895-897, 902. 5 Donna K. Raub, The audio-lingual drill technique: an approach to teaching composition (Master's thesis, George Peabody College for Teachers, 1966). W. J. Griffin, Developing syntactic control in seventh grade writing through audio-lingual drill on transformations (Paper read at the an- nual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York City, February Its, 1967). R. C. O'Donnell, W. J. Griffin and R. C. Norris, Syntax of kinder- garten and elementary school children: a transformational analysis (Cham- paign, M.: NCTE, 1967). 45 46 RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OF ENGLISH

.10 ;,,i01;,1 ,:;, Vised by the aforementioned researchers, 'Kellogg W. Hunt,8 , and others to see if the oral exercises had in fact effecteda change in the writing of the experimentalgroup. EXP 'MENTAL '1.The experiment was divided into two periods oftime. The 'PROCEDUREfirst phase extended from September 19 to December 9, 1966. On the first day and the last day of this period both theex- perimental and the controlgroup were administered a test using Coronet film No. 309: Spotty, Story ofa Fawn. Contrary to the Methodology used by O'Donnell, Griffin and Norris, the film was shown with the soundon; throughout the film, a narrator told the story of Spotty, with music filling in the back- ground. Although it was realized that the Students mightuse 1,:,... ; , ,, the grammatical structures peculiar to the narration andnot common in their own writing, it was felt that this would not invalidate the experiment since the contamination would beas ;I, I; great for the control group as it would be for the experimental group. Thus the difference in the performance of the two groups would be due to the experimental methodology. Similarly, a pair of pretests and posttestswas administered January 9 and June 1, 1967, using Encyclopaedia Britannica film No. 878, The Hunter and The Forest. (Forpurposes of

,I experimentation, this film proved to be superior to the previ- ,0,: ously mentioned film since the sound track containsno dia- logue or narration.) These tests marked the beginning and the end of the second phase of the experiment. In the first phase, . the experimental class was exposed to the experimental meth- ,: odology four days a week during 37 periods of from thirtyto forty minutes. In the second phase of the experiment, thestu- . dents in this class were exposed to the experimental method-

.1 ology two days per week during 30 periods which averaged from forty to fifty minutes in length. For the entire first phase of the experiment and for the first two months of the second, the class hour for theexperimental group was conducted within the following format: ,,,; 1. The structure to be practiced, writtenon the blackboard, was read by the students orally following the teacher's model

8 K. W. Hunt, Differences in grammaticalstructures written at three grade levels, the structures to be analyzed by transformationalmethods (U.S. Office of Education Cooperative ResearchProject No. 1998. Tallahassee, Fla.: Florida State Univer., 1964).See also K. W. Hunt, Grammatical structures written at three grade levels (Champaign,111.: NCTE, 1965 ). EFFECT OF ORALD,i1 WES 47

reading. Thus the instructor would read twocue sentences such as the following: The boy put the old man down. The boy was very tired. After the reading of each sentence by the teacher, the stu- dents would perform a reading in chorus from the graphicrep- resentation of these sentences on, the blackboard. Then the in- structor would read these sentences in their combined form as the response sentence which is required in the exercise: The boy, who was very tired, put the oldman down. The students also would perform choral reading of thissen- tence from the blackboard. 2. Ten sets of sentences with the same structure as theex- ample sentence were then practiced orally by the entire class. The practice was conducted in the following fashion: ( a) The instructor read the two cue sentences. (b) Individual students were requested on a voluntary basis to combine the two cue sentences into the required response sentence orally. ( c) If the sentences were combined in the required form, the entire class was requested to say the sentence in unison. If the sentences were not combined as required, the instructor modeled the response for the students to repeat in unison. (d) Individual students were then called on to say the response sentence which had just been practiced. This last stepwas designed to make sure that each student could at least repeat theresponse sentence. After two or three exercises with uniform sentence types had been practiced, review exercises were constructed which contrasted the differing sentences. 3. After the oral practice, the students and the instructor joined, in a choral reading of a passage ofprose. Usually, this prose was taken from textbooks which the students were cur- rently using for their science or social studies. At the start of the experiment, however, a rewritten version of Mark Twain's "The Celebrated Jumping Frog" and rewritten selections from Huckleberry Finn were used. In the second phase of theex- periment, folk-tales revised and edited for foreign students were read in this fashion.° Although the readings were not con- sidered central to the experimental methodology, theywere nevertheless deemed valuable because (a ) they provided a linguistic context for the language exercises. (b) they provided V. 0. Binner, American folktales I (New York: Thomas Y. Crowell, 1966), and International folktales I (New York: Thomas Y. Crowell, 1967). 48 RESEARCH IN ME TFACIIING OF ENGLISH

a convenient source for thevocabulary and structures used in the exercises, ( c ) they gave the students additional practice in the manipulation of oral language, and ( d) they helped main- tain the interest of the students in the language period since these readings were in themselves interesting. 4. Written exercises from the preceding day weredistribu- ted and reviewed by the students. Scores were announced so that the class as a whole would receive some idea of the progress made. 5. The exercise for the day was reviewed with the instruc- tor reading the cue sentences and thestudents performing the combination exercises in unison. 6. One or two sets of cue sentences were readby the in- structor, and the correct response sentences werewritten by the students. These exercises were graded out of classby the instructor to see if the number of papers on which all sen- tences were combined as required bythe exercise increased from day to day. It is hypothesized that this exercisealso helped to effect transfer of training from oral manipulationof sentences to the writing of these sentences. After the second month of the second phase of the experi- ment, the methodology was variedsomewhat. The exercise for the day was not reviewed (step 5). Rather a second setof cue and response sentences was practiced, generally sentencesof a different structural type than the first set of cue and response sentences for any particular day. In other respects,however, each instructional period duplicated the format of the pre- ceding one. In all, the experimental methodology was designed to con- dition the students to produce three types of sentences: ( a ) sentences with who and which adjectival clausesand sentences with elements derived from these clauses, ( b ) sentenceswith adverbial clauses in initial and final position, and (c) sentences with nominalizations in the subject and predicatederived from underlying source sentences. Under the first type ( a), students produced response sentences such as the following from their respective cue sentences: CUE RESPONSE 1. He looked at the boy. He looked at the boy, who came The boy came out of the river. out of the river. 2. The people, who were workingThe people, working in the day, in the day, might see me. might see me. EFFECT OF ORAL EXMICISES 49

3, The men, whowere in the mid-The men, in the middle of the raft, dle of the raft, might catch him, might catch him. 4, A wind, whichwas strong, be-A strong wind began to blow. gan to blow. 5. The old man wasvery heavy. The old man, who the boy carried, The boy carried the oldman. was very heavy. 6. The girlwouldn't wash theThe girl wouldn't wash the horse, horse. The horse's backwas very whose back was very dirty. dirty. (Sentences of the type illustrated by examples 5 and6 were not included in the exercises until the second phase of theex- periment largely because theyare quite difficult for elemen- tary school students to handle. Approximatelyan equal num- ber of sentences with which and whowere included in the exercises.) In the second type of sentences practiced (b), studentscom- bined two cue sentences using adverbialconnectors. In the first phase of the experiment they attached thesentence with the adverbial subordinator in sentence initialposition; in the second phase of the experiment they attached thesubordinate clause in sentence final position, thenthey shifted it to sen- tence initial position as in the following illustrative examples: CUE RESPONSE 1. Hophra would be caught. Hophra would be caught unless he He could be freed, could be freed. Unless he could befreed,Hophra would be caugh` 2. The princess couldn't bemar-The princess couldn't bmarried tied. She was too proud. because she was too proud. Because she was too proud, the princess couldn't be married. The third type of sentence. practiced (c)included a variety of subtypes, all of whichwere formed by the process of sen- tence combination; some of these, however,were later changed by the switching of sentence elementsfrom one position to another as in the following examples: CUE RESPONSE 1. Something disturbed the king. The talking of the princess dis- The princess talked. turbed the king. 2. Something angered the beast. The ungratefulness of the merchant The merchant was ungrateful. angered the beast. 50 RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OFENGLISH

3. It was interesting. It was interesting to listen to the He listened to the speaker. speaker. To listen to the speaker was interesting. 4. It seemed very cruel. It seemed very cruel, shooting the He shot the mule- mule. Shooting the mule seemed very cruel. The first type of structure, andwhich relative clauses and the elements derived from theseclauses, was dealt with in 39 lessons. The second type of structure,the adverbial clauses, was dealt with in 31 lessons.The third type of struc- ture, the nominalizations, wasdealt with in 17 lessons. Not every lesson wasdevoted exclusively to one type of structure: many of thelessons, especially in the second part ofphase two, dealt with different types of structures indifferent parts of the lessons; some of the lessons in phase onecontrasted different structural patterns in the same part ofthe lesson, RESULTS In an effort to determine whetherthe experimental method- ology had in fact given the students of theexperimental group greater facility in the useof the structures which were prac- ticed, the number of the occurrencesof these structures on all of the pretests and posttests wascounted. The numerical re- sults thus obtained were submitted to rigorousstatistical analy- sis for the 24 students in thecontrol group and the 26 students in the experimental group whocompleted all of the pretests and posttests. From an examination of Table 1, twothings become evident: (1) both the control group andthe experimental group showed an increase in the structureswhich were taught from the first pretest, but only theexperimental group showed a sta- tistically significant gain, and (2)by the time of the second series of pretests and postteststhe students in the experimental group were usingthe structures which had beenpracticed by them far more frequently thanthe students in the control group even thoughboth of these groups used these structures at an approximatelyequivalent rate in the beginning of the experiment. Furthermore, the gainevidenced by the experi- mental group was significant atthe .001 level of confidence on the final posttest. The reasonfor the increase in the use of these structures by the control group onthe first series of pre- tests and posttests is notknown. At first, it was hypothesized that the number of occurrencesof the taught structures in- creased with the number of wordswritten. (See Table 2. ) This, however, fails to explain thelack of such an increase for Table 1 The Occurrence of the Structures Taught on the Pretests and Posttests

Posttest 1 Pretest 1 Mean Total M SD Total i SD Increment

C Group 29 1.2 1.4 50 2.1 1.7 .9 3.649 .062 E Group 30 1.2 1.4 73 2.8 2.3 1.4 9.588 .003**

Posttest 2 Pretest 2 Mean Total M SD Total M SD Increment

C Group 80 3.3 3.2 79 3.3 2.4 .0 .002 .959 Pi E Group 98 3,8 2.1 191 7.4 3.9 3.7 17.307 .001***

*Significant at the .05 level of confidence or less. **Significant at the ,01 level of confidence or less. ***Significant at the .001 level of confidence or less. il gl

CR Table 2 The Number of Words Written on Pretests and Posttests

Pretest 1 Posttest 1 Mean Total M SD Total M SD Increment

C Group 1935 80.6 41.1 2932 122.2 44,5 42.4 11.289 .002** E Group 1888 72.7 35.9 3934 151.3 54.2 78.6 38.101 < .0005***

Pretest 2 Posttest 2 Mean Total M SD Total M SD Increment

C Group 2368 98.7 44.6 2900 120.8 52.7 22.1 2.477 .122 E Group 2892 111.2 46.6 4149 159.6 56.2 48.4 11.399 .001***

*Significant at the .05 level of confidence or less. **Significant at the .01 level of confidenceof less. ***Significant at the .001 level of confidence or less, EFFECT OF ORAL EXERCISES 53

the control group on the second series of pretests and posttests, In effect, it might be that this increase in taught structureson the part of the control group is related to the narrationon the film used .Lo elicit the pretest and posttest compositions, In any case, it became apparent that the experimental group wrote a greater number of words within the half-hour time limit than the control group and that this difference in theper- formance of the experimental subjects was statistically signifi- cant (The results are given in Table 2.) From the total num- ber of words written the following generalizationscan be made: (1) students writing about a movie usedas a stimulus situation will write more on their second encounter with the movie than on their first and (2) students subjected to oral and written structure drills increase their productivity in writing at a greater rate than those who are not subjected to these drills. In this regard, it is interesting to note that control group subjects produced compositions which averaged 120 words in length on both posttests; the experimental group sub- jects surpassed the control group subjects in productivity by writing compositions averaging almost 160 words on the final posttest. The least that can be said of the increased produc- tivity in writing of the experimental group is that the experi- mental methodology did give these fourth-grade studentsmore fluency and facility in writing. A clearer picture of tlw experimental group's divergence from the control group on these twomeasures can be gained from an analysis of variance. In the number of words written, the performance of the experimental group is clearly superior to that of the control group. From Table 3 it is apparent that on the pretests the perform- ance of the experimental group was not significantly different from that of the control group. On the posttests, however, the performance of the experimental group did differ significantly at the .05 level of confidence and this difference was cumula- tive; that is, the experimental group continued to improve its performance so that the score on the second posttestwas su- perior to the score on the first posttest in a comparison to the scores of the control group. In a similar manner, as is shown in Table 4, the performance of the experimental group differed from the perfoirnsnce of the control group on the measure of the structures taught. ( This measure is obtained by simply counting the number of 54 RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OF ENGLISH

Table 3 Analysis of Variance: Number of Words Written on the Pretests and Posttests

Source of Sum of Mean Variance Squares df Square F P

PRETEST 1 Between CategLi 800.641 1 800.641 .540 .466 Within Categories 71171.779 48 1482.745 Total 71972.420 49 POSTTEST 1 Between Categories 10598.008 110598.008 4.280 .044" Within Categories 118852.871 48 2476.101 Total 129450.880 49 PRETEST 2 Between Categories 1970.051 1 1970.051 .946 .335 Within Categories 99873.949 48 2080.707 Total 101844.000 49 POSTTEST 2 Between Categories 18733.300 1 18733.300 6.293 .016° Within Categories 142881.679 48 2976.701 Total 161614.980 49 Category 1= C Group, N = 24; Category 2 = E Group, N = 26 "Significant at the .05 level of confidence or less. "Significant at the .01 level of confidence or less. """Significant at the .001 level of confidence or less. occurrences on the testsof the sentence structures practiced by the experimental group and discussed earlier in this report.) On the measure of the structures taught, the experimental group showed the same kind ofdevelopment that it did on the measure of the total words written, increasing to the first posttest, falling back on the second pretest andthen increas- ing to the final posttest. On this measure, however, the per- formance of the experimental group did not attain a statisti- cally significant level of difference from that of the control group until the second posttest. Butwhen it did attain this level of performance, the difference between the two groups was significant atless than the .001 level of confidence. This score is particularly gratifying. If the pretests and post- tests simply tested the students on theirability to manipulate the structures as they were taught them from oral cuesa rela- tively weak testit would be expected that the experimental EFFECT OF ORAL EXERCISES 55

group would show abetter score than the control group. On the pretests and posttests for this experiment,however, the test is whether theexperimental group actually uses more of the struci:ares practiced than the control groupin a free com- position, a .eelatively strong test.And the experimental group is clearly superior 10 the control group onthis measure. Table 4 Analysis of Variance: The Number of StructuresTaught

Source of Sum of Mean Variance Squares df Square F P

PRETEST 1 Between Categories .037 1 .037 .018 .893 Within Categories 97.342 48 2.028 Total 97.380 49 POSTTEST 1 Between Categories 6.548 1 6.548 1.541 .220 Within Categories 203.872 48 4.247 Total 210.420 49 PRETEST 2 Between Categories 2.371 1 2.371 ,335 .566 Within Categories 339.949 48 7.082 Total 342.320 49 POSTTEST 2 Between Categories 205.157 1 205.15719.584<.0005*** Within Categories 502.843 48 10.476 Total 708.000 49 Category 1= C Group, N = 24; Category 2 = E Group,N = 26 Significant at the .05 level of confidence or less. **Significant at the .01 level of confidence or less. ***Significant at the .001 level of confidence or less.

The experimental group alsoshowed growth in writing abil- ity on one of the units of measurementdeveloped by Kellogg W. Hunt. In his studies, Huntfound that the T-unit, or mini- mal terminable unit, provided thebasis for a number of measures whichindicated that the students were maturing as writers.'° In his study, the T-unit isbasically a repunctuated, or properlypunctuated, sentence. As students mature, they tend to use a greater proportion ofmulti-clause T-units in their

10 Hunt, op. cit. RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OF ENGLISH

writing." On these measures, the experimental group showed a generally greater improvement than the control group. In par- ticular, the ratio of multi-clause T-units (complex sentences ) to single-clause T-units( simple sentences) increased more in the experimental group than in tilo control. ( See Table 5. ) Table 5 The Number of Multi-clause and Single-clause T-units Pretest 1 Posttest 1 Pretest 2 Posttest 2 Total M Total M Total NJ Total M E Group Multi- Clause 34 1.3 84 3.2" 40 1.5 97 3.7". C Group Multi- Clause 41 1.7 79 3.3" 33 1.3 43 1.8 E Group Single- Clause 241 9.3 46818.0***30311.7 36714.1 C Group Single- Clause 236 9.8 33413.9* 25610.7 28211.2 *Indicates that thegain between pretest and posttest is significant at the .05 level of confidence. "Indicates that thegain between pretest and posttest is significant at the .01 level of confidence. ***Indicates that thegain between pretest and posttest is significant at the .001 level ofconfidence.

Generally speaking, the experimental group subjects show a proportionately greater increase in the use of multi-clause T- units when this increase is compared to that of the single- clause T-units. The use of multi-clause T-units more than dou- bles from the second pretest to the second posttest ( cf. mean of 1.5 to 3.7 ). The number of single-clause T-units does not show such a gain. The exception to this is the gains made by both control and experimental groups on the first posttest. Here the mean of the experimental group went from 1.3 to 3.2 and the mean of the control group went from 1.7 to 3.3 in the number of multi-clause T -units written. Since this phenomenon did not occur on the second posttest, it is hypothesized that the contamination from the dialogue on the film used as the

11 Hunt, Differences in grammatical structures, p. 25. EFFECT OF ORAL EXERCISES 57

Table 6 The Number of Words inMulti-clause and Single-clause T-units

Pretest 1 Posttest 1 Pretest 2 Posttest 2 Total M Total M Total M Total M E Group Multi- Clause 33913.0 88233.9" 52920.3 121846.8" C Group Multi- 24.2 Clause 46119.2 80333.4* 43818.3 581 E Group Single- 112.7* Clause 153158.9 2955113.7***232289.3 2931 C Group Single- 06.6 Clause 153664.0 207986.6* 190479.3 2318 *Indicates that thegain between pretest andposttest is significant at the .05 level ofconfidence. "Indicates that thegain between pretest andposttest is significant at the .01 level ofconfidence. *"Indicates that thegain between pretest andposttest is significant at the .001 level ofconfidence. stimulus situation for the first pretestand posttest caused the performance of the control group toequal or even exceed that of the performance ofthe experimental group.(This same contamination effect is apparentin the use of a filmwith narration as a stimulus situationfor testing in the Raub re- search as reported by Griffin.)12 This same phenomenon isobservable in the number of words in multi-clause andsingle-clause T-units. (SeeTable 6. ) Again, the most impressive gainis the gain in thenumber of to the words inPI T-units from the second pretest second posttest for theexperimental:.41 oup.These subjects wrote more thandouble the number ofwords in multi-clause T-units on the second posttestthan they did on the second pretest (cf. mean of20.3 to 46.8). This gain was notmatched by the control group'sperformance (18.3 to 24.2), nor wasit matched by as proportionately alarge gain in the numberof words in single-clause T-units.This same trend is notfound

12Griffin, cp. cit. 58 RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OF ENGLISH in the performance of all theexperimental subjects in the first pretest and posttest. Again, on this seriesof tests the per- formance of the experimental group is veryclosely matched by that of the control group, at least in thenumber of words in multi-clause T-units, the mostcrucial of the measurements for predicting growth in writing. Table 7 Length of Single-clause, Multi-clause, and All T-units and the Subordination Ratio Length of Length of SubordinationLength of Multi-clause Single-clause Ratio T-units T-units T-units

sa4 s:2- C2-0 0szk 0in-' 0 o., 2 0 0 0 C.) C31 C.) 44 C..5 C..) 44 C.) 44

Pretest 1 26.3 23.7 7.0 6.9 11.2 10.0 6.5 6.4 Posttest 1 31.5 26.5 7.1 7.1 10.2 10.5 6.2 6.3 Pretest 2 21.2 20.3 8.2 8.7 13.3 13.2 7.4 7.7 Posttest 2 23.4 34.5 8.6 9.0 13.5 12.6 8.2 8.0 Hunt's Fourth Graders22.2 8.6 13.6 7.2

If this phenomenon is stated in the ternsthat Hunt13 uses, the startling discrepancy in theperformance of the control group on the first pretestis more noticeable. (See Table 7.) Since the control group wrote a greaterproportion of multi- clause T-units, their subordination ratio wasmuch greater than that of the experimental group (cf, 31.5 to26.5) on the first posttest. This trend, however, wasreversed on the second post- test, where the experimental grouphad a much larger subordi- nation ratio (cf. 34.5 to 23.4). Thus, itis reasoned that the con- tamination effect of the narration on thefirst posttest caused the control group to have a highersubordination ratio than the experimental group. Since thiscontamination effect was not operating on the second posttest,the experimental group had the higher subordination ratio. On theother measures, the experimental methodology seemed to have verylittle effect. Both the control group and theexperimental group showed an 13 Hunt, Differences in grammatical structures, pp. 28,22, and Gram- matical structures, pp. 36, 38. LifieLGT OF ORAL EXERCISES 59 ,

Table 8 Analysis of Variance: The Number of Muhl-clause T-units

Source of Sum of Mean Variance Squares df Square F P

PRETEST 1 Between Categories 2.003 1 2.003 .956 .333 Within Categories 100.497 48 2.093 Total 102.500 49 POSTTEST 1 Between Categories .046 1 .046 .006 .935 Within Categories 325.573 48 6.782 Total 325,620 49 PRETEST 2 Between Categories .333 1 .333 .163 .688 Within Categories 98.086 48 2.043 Total 98.420 49 POSTTEST 2 Between Categories 46.926 1 46.926 7.957 .007" Within Categories 283.073 48 5.897 Total 330.000 49 Category 1= C Group, N = 24; Category 2 =E Group, N = 26 *Significant at the .05 level of confidence or less. **Significant at the .01 level of confidence or less. "*Significant at the .001 level of confidence or less.

increase on these measures fromthe first pretest to the last posttest, but neither of the groupsshowed any marked superi- ority over the other. All this can be summarized in ananalysis of variance of the scores of the two groupsin the experiment. (See Table8.) The experimental group did not evidenceuntil the second post- test a statistically significant gainin the number of multi- clause T-units over the number usedby the control group. This is not true for the number ofsingle-clause T-units used. In this measure, the performanceof the experimental group did not surpass the performance ofthe control group on .glee posttest at a statisticallysignificant level of confidence., The reason for this is that the experimental group onthe sec- ond posttest wrote a proportionately greaternumber of multi- clause T-units and words in multi-clauseT-units. Hence, even this measure indicates a favorabledevelopment in the writing eo RESEARCH IN THE TEACHING OF ENGLISH

Table 9 Analysis of Variance: The Number of Single-clause T-units

Source of Sum of Mean Variance Squares df Square F P PRETEST 1 Between Categories 3.971 1 3.971 .1.55 .695 Within Categories 1226.449 48 25.551 Total 1230.419 49 POSTTEST 1 Between Categories 208.087 1 208.087 3.838 .056 Within Categories 2601.833 48 54.205 Total 2809.920 49 PRETEST 2 Between Categories 12.162 1 12.162 .468 .497 Within Categories 1247.217 48 25.984 Total 1259.380 49 POSTTEST 2 Between Cat gories 69.826 1 69.826 2.916 .094 Within Categories 1149.153 48 23.941 Total 1218,980 49 Category 1= C Group, N = 24; Category 2 = E Group, N = 26

*Significant at the .05 level of confidence or less. "Significant at the .01 level of confidence or less. ***Significant at the .001 level of confidence or less.

of the experimental group; the students in this group used a greater proportion of multi-clause T-units. In other words, these students wrote proportionately fewer simple sentences and proportionately more complex sentences than the control group students. The fact that the difference in the number of words in single-clause T-units between the experimental group and the control group almost reaches a statistically significant level of confidence ( .05) on the first posttest indicates that the latter were not writing as well on the first posttest as they were on the second posttest. ( See Table 9.) SUMMARY AND The effect of systematic oral and written exercises on the CONCLUSIONwriting of fourth-grade students can be summarized in three statements: (1) Students who participated in these exercises wrote with greater freedom and facility than those who did not; hence, these students could write a greater number of words in a shorter period of time. EFFECr OF ORAL EXERCISISS Bx

structures in ( 2) Students whopracticed certain sentence their oral and writtenforms used these structures morefre- quently than those whodid not. (3) Students whopracticed putting together sentencesin their oral or written form sothat simple sentences areformed into complex sentences use agreater proportionof complex sen- tences, Forthese three reasons, ithas been judged that oral and written exerciseshave a favorable effect onthe writing of fourth graders.