<<

Br. I. soc. din. Psychol. (1978), 17, 25-29 Printed in Great Britain 25

Reactions to flattery as a function of self-esteem: Self-enhancement and cognitive consistency theories

Andrew M. Colman and Kevin R. Olver

Male subjects who had previously scored either very high or very low on a self-esteem scale were interviewed about personal characteristics, biographical details, social activities, etc. They then received, via closed-circuit television, flattering or neutral character assessments which were apparently based on their performance in the interviews. A highly significant interaction (P< 0.(01) showed that whereas the subjects of high self-esteem responded with far greater liking for the evaluator in the flattery than in the neutral condition, those of low self-esteem somewhat preferred the neutral evaluator. These findings provide clear-cut support for the cognitive consistency theory regarding reactions to flattery, but do not rule out a concomitant though weaker self-enhancement effect.

One of the most well-established techniques of encountered in everyday social interaction is flattery, or what Edward Jones (1964), in his influential monograph, calls 'complimentary other-enhancement'. An admonition given great prominence in Dale Carnegie's best-selling manual, How to Win Friends and Influence People (Carnegie, 1936), is to 'dole out lavishly', and there is now a considerable body of empirical evidence indicating that people do indeed typically increase their liking for someone who expresses approval of them (e.g. Backman & Secord, 1959; Jones, Gergen & Davis, 1962). From a common-sense point of view these findings are not surprising, although as Jones has pointed out, one might expect excessive flattery to backfire in certain circumstances. The theoretical interpretation of the effects of flattery presents certain problems, even in straightforward cases where the compliments offered by the flatterer are accepted by the target person as sincere rather than manipulative in their intent. Under these circumstances is flattery bound to succeed? The answer depends upon which of two theoretical interpretations is given to the process. One of the most popular is self-enhancement theory, which is based upon certain elements of Rogers' (1959) personality theory and the motivational concept of drive reduction. The fundamental assumption is that people are motivated to evaluate themselves favourably, and for this reason they respond positively, by (among other things) increased liking for people who flatter them. The other major approach is in terms of cognitive consistency theory, whose roots in Heider's (1946, 1958) balance model, and which entails the assumption that people are imbued with a drive towards cognitive consistency. Granted this assumption, a person's reaction to flattery can be shown to depend upon how favourably he evaluates himself: if he has a high self-esteem he will tend to judge the flatterer favourably since this satisfies his drive towards consistency in terms of the balance model, but if he has a low self-esteem he will tend to judge the flatterer unfavourably for the same reason. Even in the case of apparently sincere flattery, therefore, the latter theory allows the possibility of a boomerang effect when compliments are directed at a person of low self-esteem, thereby tying him up in one of Laing's 'knots ': 'I am bad/You love me/therefore you are bad' (Laing, 1970, p. 10). It is of course possible that both major theories are true, since people may be motivated towards both self-enhancement and cognitive consistency. Recent reviews of the literature in this area have, nevertheless, tended to regard the theories as being in competition with each other to account for the available evidence (Jones, 1973; Shrauger, 1975). If the two theories were for some reason mutually exclusive, it would be possible to devise an experimentum crucis to decide between them, since opposite predictions flow from the theories regarding the relationship between reactions to flattery on the one hand and self-esteem on the other; 26 Andrew M. Colman and Kevin R. Diver

self-enhancement theory implies that low self-esteem subjects will increase their liking for the flatterer more than high self-esteem subjects (since the consequent drive reduction is presumed to be greater) whereas cognitive consistency theory clearly makes the opposite prediction (as explained above). The theories are not mutually exclusive, however, so experiments along these lines can do no better than give an indication of the relative potency of the self-enhancement and consistency tendencies under various conditions.

Method Subjects The subjects were 30 male undergraduates living in various halls of residence at Leicester University. Each subject responded to an abridged version of the Janis-Field Self-Esteem Questionnaire (Janis et al. 1959) consisting of 23 items relating to 'feelings of inadequacy'. On the basis of the self-esteem scores the upper and lower tertiles of the sample, designated the high self-esteem and low self-esteem groups respectively, were requested to report to the laboratory for further testing.

Procedure The experiment took place in a room conspicuously fitted with a one-way mirror, so that the subjects would be fully aware that they could be observed from an adjacent room. Each subject was seated comfortably and was interviewed by the experimenter about biographical details, what his friends are like, how he gets on with people, incidents which he is proud or embarrassed to recall, problems with work, etc. The same structured interview schedule was used in each case, and the interviews normally lasted for approximately 10--15 min. The subject was told that the purpose of the experiment was to investigate the way in which people form impressions of personality without direct personal interaction with the individual being assessed. He was further informed that a postgraduate student had been observing the interview through the one-way mirror, and that his evaluation of the subject would now be relayed via closed-circuit television, since it was still necessary to avoid face-to-face interaction. A television monitor in the corner of the room was then switched on and the subject was presented with either a flattering or a neutral evaluation of his personality, apparently based upon the impression he conveyed in the interview, and spoken by a 30 year old man who appeared on the screen. Jones (1964) has pointed out that is unfortunately unavoidable in this area of research, and the present study is no exception; There was, in fact, no postgraduate observing the interview from behind the one-way mirror (this is a reversal of the more common situation in which the subject is deceived into thinking he is not being observed!) and the evaluation of the subject relayed via closed-circuit television was one of two which had been pre-recorded on videotape. Careful probing revealed, however, that none of the subjects doubted the existence of an observer in the adjacent room or the genuineness of the evaluations, and all accepted the 'cover story' about the avoidance of direct face-to-face interaction, Half the high self-esteem subjects and half the low self-esteem subjects were randomly assigned to the flattery condition, the remainder being assigned to the neutral condition. In the flattery condition, the pre-recorded evaluation relayed to subjects on the television monitor ran as follows: 'Are you ready?' (Pause, during which the experimenter responded appropriately.) 'Well, in my opinion, he creates a very favourable impression. He's the kind of person I enjoy talking to. He seemed a little ill-at-ease but no more than one would expect. I paid close attention to the things he said about himself and he shows healthy psychological adjustment. I think he's going to do well in the future and he's got nothing to worry about. All in all, he seems a very capable person. It's a bit embarrassing that there's nothing really to say on the negative side, but that's the way I honestly feel.' The evaluation used in the neutral condition was as follows: 'Are you ready?' (Pause) 'Well, in my opinion, he creates a fair impression. I don't think he has any particularly serious problems and his level of psychological adjustment doesn't seem too bad. He seemed a little iIl-at-ease but not too much. The rest of his university life shouldn't prove any more difficult for him than for most people as he seems to be a fairly capable person. He probably has a reasonable number of friends and shouldn't do too badly in the future. There's nothing really outstanding to say, so that's about it.' It is not, of course, suggested that the evaluation used in the neutral condition contained nothing which any subject could interpret as flattering. What is important is that it was in any event unambiguously less flattering than the deliberately flattering evaluation used in the other condition. Reactions to flattery as a function of seLf-esteem 27

Mter listening to the evaluation of his personality, each subject was told that his reactions to the postgraduate would be of great help; he was given a counterbalanced semantic differential containing 15 bipolar adjective pairs with high evaluative loadings, separated by seven-point rating scales, and he was requested to give his impressions of the postgraduate. He was further asked to comment on the postgraduate's evaluation of him, and he was probed for any suspicions about the experimental situation before being debriefed.

Results The mean score on the self-esteem scale of the low self-esteem group was 38·60, with a range of 21-45. The mean score of the high self-esteem group was 65·50, and the range in this group was 62 to 77. Since the theoretical range of this scale is 0 to 92, it is clear that a good separation was achieved between the two groups. The dependent variable was the subjects' ratings of the evaluator on a 15-item semantic differential. For each adjective pair a highly unfavourable rating was scored 0 and a highly favourable rating was scored 6; the overall rating therefore had a theoretical range of 0-90. The mean ratings of the flattering and neutral evaluators by subjects of high self-esteem were 72·4 and 46·8 respectively; those of the low self-esteem subjects were 51·6 and 60·8. The ratings were analysed by means of a two-way analysis of variance. The effect of flattery

was found to be significant (F=; 4· 78, dJ. =; 1, 16, P < 0·05), with subjects giving more favourable ratings in response to flattering than to neutral evaluations. This finding, though incidental to the present study, provides further corroboration for the conclusions reached by previous workers regarding the efficacy of flattery as an ingratiation technique.

There was no main effect due to self-esteem (F=; 0·82, dJ. =; 1, 16), but a highly significant

interaction (flattery x self-esteem) emerged from the analysis (F=; 27·13, dJ. =; 1, 16, P< 0.(01). Whereas high self-esteem subjects gave much more favourable ratings of the evaluator in response to flattery as compared with a neutral evaluation, the low self-esteem subjects tended somewhat to prefer the evaluator in the neutral condition, i.e. the flattery backfired in their case.

Discussion The findings reported here provide unambiguous support for the cognitive consistency theory regarding the effects of flattery, since subjects of high self-esteem reacted with a far greater liking for the flatterer than did the subjects of low self-esteem who, for their part, tended actually to prefer the neutral evaluator. This is not, however, the first experimental confirmation of the theory; Deutsch & Solomon (1959) provided the earliest confirmatory evidence, and further support was subsequently provided by Wilson (1965), followed by Dutton & Arrowood (1971) and most recently by Koeck & Guthrie (1975). It has been pointed out (e.g. Jones, 1973; Shrauger, 1975) that the interpretation of these previous experiments is problematical for a number of reasons. One important difficulty arises from the fact that in all cases the flatterer delivered compliments which amounted to appraisals of some specific competence or skill on the part of the subjects. In the Dutton & Arrowood (1971) experiment, for example, the subjects presented arguments on draft resistance to a panel of peers who then ostensibly judged the performance 'a good job' or 'a poor job '. In view of the demand characteristics of experimental situations of this type, the reactions of the subjects in this experiment, and in the others since they all possess the same design flaw, may reflect their judgements of the flatterer's accuracy rather than their Liking for him. This possibility vitiates the previous experiments insofar as they purport to confirm the cognitive consistency theory, since a subject would obviously tend to consider an evaluator as more accurate when he agrees with a subject's opinion of himself, whether high or low, than otherwise, so the results are easily accounted for without recourse to consistency theory. The findings reported in the present study, on the other hand, cannot be explained away in this fashion, since the flatterer's compliments were of a more general nature and expressed a diffuse liking and respect for the 28 Andrew M. Colman and Kevin R. Olver

subjects as human beings; the question of accuracy seems hardly applicable in this case. Up to now, no experimental support has been forthcoming when such generalized flattery has been used, the results always being more easily interpreted in terms of self-enhancement theory (Shrauger, 1975). The results reported here therefore provide, apparently for the first time, a clear-cut corroboration of the cognitive consistency theory regarding reactions to flattery. While the theory cannot therefore be considered superfluous (cf. Jones, 1973; Krauss & Critchfield, 1975), these results do not rule out the possibility of a concomitant (though weaker) self-enhancement effect. It may be illuminating to examine the two theoretical approaches and the results of this experiment in the light of the literature concerned with reciprocity of liking in interpersonal attraction. There is a great deal of empirical evidence (see Berscheid & Walster, 1969) in support of the common-sense belief that people tend on the whole to like those who like them and dislike those who dislike them. Although this 'reciprocity-of-liking' rule is well supported by the available evidence, and although some writers (e.g. Ossorio & Davis, 1966) regard it as a necessary and 'non-empirical' principle of human interaction, exceptions to the rule are by no means unknown. This fact is attested to by the recurrence of the theme of unrequited love in romantic novels and in life. One exception of particular relevance to the present discussion emerges from the literature on ingratiation: flattery does not always succeed in its aim of eliciting liking from a target person, even when it is perceived as being sincere (Jones, 1964). The results of the experiment described above provide a particularly clear instance of flattery backfiring and therefore of the reciprocity-of-liking rule apparently breaking down, in the responses of the low self-esteem subjects. The results cannot adequately be accounted for purely in terms of self-enhancement theory, since this theory clearly generates the prediction (at variance with the results) that subjects of low self-esteem, who lack confidence and feel insecure about their worth, will respond with greater liking for the flatterer than those of high self-esteem since the flattery will have resulted in greater need satisfaction (Jones, 1973). Cognitive consistency theory, on the other hand, leads to an equally unambiguous but opposite prediction, and one which was fully corroborated by the results reported above. The fundamental assumption underlying cognitive consistency theory is that people are motivated to organize their beliefs, feelings and actions in meaningful and sensible ways. In terms of Heider's (1946, 1958) original balance model, for example, an individual's (p's) liking or disliking for another person (0), his attitude towards some entity or event (x) and his perception of o's attitude towards x will tend to become organized into a state of balance rather than one of imbalance. According to the model, given that two of the three sentiment relations are positive, for example if p likes x and 0 likes x, the third relation must necessarily be positive for a state of balance to exist, i.e. p must like o. Similarly, if p dislikes x but 0 likes x, the only p-o relation which results in balance is one of disliking, i.e. p must dislike o. Now in most applications of the balance model (and the same applies, mutatis mutandis, to derivative cognitive consistency models) x is taken to represent some psychological object external to p and 0, such as 'modern art' or 'communism '. A special case germane to the present discussion, however, arises when x represents p's 'self '. In this interesting case, if 0 is an apparently sincere flatterer, one of the three sentiment relations (0 likes x) is fixed. If p has a positive self-evaluation, then in addition we can infer that p likes x, and the drive towards balance will therefore tend to ensure that p likes 0 (the target person will tend to like the flatterer). But if p has a negative self-evaluation, then we have two dissimilar sentiment relations (p dislikes x and 0 likes x) and the model suggests a tendency for the third relation to be p dislikes x (the target person dislikes the flatterer). The balance model thus predicts that an apparently sincere flatterer will elicit liking from a person of positive self-esteem, but disliking from one of negative self-esteem. Reactions to flattery as a function of self-esteem 29 The results of the experiment reported above are exactly in line with these predictions derived from congitive consistency theory. In view of the fact that there already exists a great deal of evidence for a self-enhancement effect (Shrauger, 1975), further research is needed to delineate the conditions under which one or other of these two tendencies becomes prepotent. Since virtually all previous research has made use of American subjects, one factor among many which might be considered is the cultural one: perhaps English subjects react differently to flattery than do Americans.

References BACKMAN, C. W. & SECORD, P. F. (1959). The JONES, S. C. (1973). Self- and interpersonal effect of perceived liking on interpersonal evaluations: Esteem theories versus consistency attraction. Hum. Relat. 12, 379-384. theories. Psychol. Bull. 79, 185-199. BERSCHEID, E. & WALSTER, E. H. (1969). KOECK, R. & GUTHRIE, G. M. (1975). Reciprocity in Interpersonal Attraction. Reading, Mass.: impression formation. J. soc. Psychol. 95, 67-76. Addison-Wesley. KRAUSS, H. H. & CRITCHFIELD, L. L. (1975). CARNEGlE, D. (1936). How to Win Friends and Contrasting self-esteem theory and consistency Influence People. New York: Simon & Schuster. theory in predicting interpersonal attraction. DEUTSCH, M. & SOLOMON, L. (1959). Reactions to Sociometry 38, 247-260. evaluations by others as influenced by LAING, R. D. (1970). Knots. New York: Pantheon. self-evaluations. Sociometry 22, 93-112. OSSORIO, P. G. & DAVIS, K. E. (1966). The self, DUTrON, D. G. & ARROWOOD, A. J. (1971). intentionality, and reactions to evaluations of the Situational factors in evaluation congruency and self. In C. Gordon & K. J. Gergen (eds), Self in interpersonal attraction. J. Person. soc. Psychol. Society. New York: WiJey. 18, 222-229. ROGERs, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, HEIDER, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive personality, and interpersonal relationships as organization. J. Psychol. 21, 107-112. developed in the client-centered framework. In S. HEIDER, F. (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Koch (ed.), Psychology: A Study of a Science. Relations. New York: WiJey. New York: McGraw-Hill. JANIS, I. L. et al. (1959). Personality and SHRAUGER, 1. S. (1975). Responses to evaluation as Persuasibility. New Haven, Conn.: Yale a function of initial self-perceptions. Psycho/. University Press. Bull. 82, 581-596. JONES, E. E. (1964). Ingratiation. New York: WILSON, D. T. (1965). Ability evaluation, Appleton-Century-Crofts. postdecision dissonance, and coworker JONES, E. E., GERGEN, K. J. & DAVIS, K. E. (1962). attractiveness. J. Person. soc. Psychol. 1, 486-489. Some determinants of reactions to being approved or disapproved as a person. Psychol. Monogr. 76, whole no. 521.

Received 26 July 1976; revised version received 4 February 1977

Requests for reprints should be addressed to Andrew M. Colman, Department of Psychology, University of Leicester, Leicester LE! 7RH. Kevin R. Olver is at the School of Behavioural and Social Science, Plymouth Polytechnic.