Orthographic Neighbourhood in Czech Language and Its Use in Research of Reading Impairment Original Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Orthographic Neighbourhood in Czech Language and Its Use in Research of Reading Impairment Original Study Linguistic Frontiers • 3(1) • 54–59 • 2020 DOI: 10.2478/lf-2020-0007 Linguistic Frontiers Orthographic neighbourhood in Czech language and its use in research of reading impairment Original Study Monika Ptáčková, Kateřina Vitásková Palacký University Olomouc Received: June 2020 ; Accepted: June 2020 Abstract : Reading is a complex function that affects our daily living. It requires the cooperation of many structu- res and functions and is influenced by many different factors. One of the factors influencing the word reading process may be the characteristics of the word, such as its frequency of use in the language, its length, ortho- graphic neighbourhood, and others. These characteristics can affect the speed and accuracy of reading in re- aders with and without learning disability. The following paper presents some aspects of our current research focused on the influence of the properties of the word on their reading in the Czech language environment. The concept of orthographic neighbourhood will be explained in more detail including some specifics in its calcu- lation and use in the Czech language. Keywords : reading, orthographic neighbourhood, Damerau-Levenshtein distance, visual word recognition INTRODUCTION Reading is a complex process consisting of many impor- on visual word recognition and reading in people with tant components, such as visual recognition, phonolo- dyslexia (see Davies, 2007) and in the typical population gical and orthographic processing, and others (Grainger (see Grainger 2017). Due to the variance between lan- 2017). There is a variety of different impairments of guages, the results of research studies differ as well1. reading ability, which can be generally divided into two Although in the last ten years many kinds of research in main groups—developmental reading impairments (see various language environments have been performed, e.g. Penolazzi et al. 2009) and acquired reading impair- most experiments are still based on the English language ments (see e.g. Karanth 2003). In the present research, and English speakers (Share 2008). the authors focus mainly on the developmental reading The important word characteristics include for exam- disorders, in some classifications also called dyslexia ple word length, word frequency (frequency of using (see e.g. Hulme, Snowling 2016). Dyslexia research in- the word), and orthographic neighbourhood (ON). The volves a lot of interesting aspects including linguistic frequency effect generally means that reading of high- characteristics of words. It suggests how the reading -frequency words is usually faster and more correct than process is involved in some of these word characteris- low-frequency words (Ghyselink et al. 2004). This is noti- tics in people with or without reading impairment. It can ceable in many languages, such as English (Grainger 2017), then help us understand reading disorders better in the Spanish (González-Nosti et al. 2013), etc. Ghyselinck et whole context of language acquisition. al. (2004) also mentioned “the cumulative frequency hy- In foreign literature, there is plenty of research on pothesis“, which combines the effect of frequency and this topic focusing on the effect of word characteristics age of word acquisition. Many studies focus on other 1 For example, in Spanish (Gonzáles-Nosti et al. 2014) the effect of orthographic neighbourhood density was shown only in short words, and there was stronger length effect than in English. Open Access. Open Access. © 2020 Monika Ptáčková, Kateřina Vitásková, published by Sciendo. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons BY 4.0 license Orthographic neighbourhood in Czech language and its use in research of reading impairment variables related to word frequency, such as the age of literature—either an inhibitory effect, no effect, or a facili- acquisition or frequency trajectory 2 (Juhasz et al. 2019). tatory effect on reading and visual word recognition. For Information about word frequency can be found in lists example, Weekes et al. (2006) suggests a hypothesis that based on the national corpus. As far as the Czech lan- the ON effect changes during development. A research guage in concerned, the Czech National Corpus website study by Andrews and Hersch (2010) suggests a conclu- also includes the frequency list 3. sion that in individuals with worse spelling skills, there is As for the word length effect, it also represents a higher facilitatory effect of the orthographic neighbou- a broad topic which has been in the centre of attention rhood, while in individuals with good spelling skills the of many studies. Word length usually affects the speed effect is generally inhibitory. The question is how language and accuracy of its recognition. Word length can also affects this variable. Furthermore, properties such as word change the influence of other properties, for example in length, frequency, and orthographic neighbourhood are a Spanish research study (Gonzales-Nosti et al. 2014), relatively closely related because short words are generally the effect of orthographic neighbourhood was significant more frequent and have more orthographic neighbours only in a group of short words. Schroeter and Schroe- than long words (largely addressed by Gonzales-Nosti et der (2014) point out the fact that the word length effect al. (2014) in their research). decreases during life with the strongest word length Many recent research studies suggest another appro- effect being usually in childhood. ach to the “orthographic neighbourhood density” value, The orthographic neighbourhood effect is the focus which is The Damerau-Levenshtein distance. As reported of the present research and is not frequently mentioned by Yarconi et al. (2008), it can be more exact than the in Czech scientific literature. The orthographic neighbou- traditionally used Coltheart’s N. The Damerau-Leven- rhood may be explained as the quality of a word, which shtein distance (DL) also reports about word similarity describes the number of words that can be created from compared with other words in the corpus. In this way, the word by changing a single letter (Grainger et al. 2005). the word is compared with all words, not only words This means for example that the Czech word “klíč” has of the same length. This value reflects the minimum 3 orthographic neighbours (“kleč”, “klín” and “klít”), while number of operations needed to change the word to the word “hmyz” has no orthographic neighbours be- another one. An operation is considered substitution, cause no existing Czech word is generated by changing addition, deletion, or transposition of one letter (two one letter to another in this letter string. Some authors letters in the case of transposition). The resulting value use the term “orthographic neighbourhood density” (La- is the sum of the 20 lowest values. In other words, it su- xon et al. 2002) or “Coltheart’s N” (Yarkoni et al. 2008) ggests how many operations are needed to transform which refers to the same concept. the word into the twenty closest words. This means that, Analogically, phonological neighbourhood refers to for example, in the Czech language the word “soused”, how many words can be made by changing one phoneme which has 0 orthographic neighbours according to the to another (Clarkson et al. 2017). This is more important Coltheart’s N (no other word arises by changing one in languages with non-transparent orthography such letter), has a DL value of 32, which is quite low as in 20 as English or French, while in the Czech language (with proximate words there is only 1 or 2 operations needed transparent orthography) similar to German, in most ca- to change to the word “soused”. ses the orthographic and phonological neighbourhood The present research uses both the Coltheart’s N and density is the same (Marian et al. 2012). LD distance, so the influence of both values and their The impact of the orthographic neighbourhood on rea- differences can be compared. ding is examined by means of various types of tasks. For example, as used in the present research, a lexical decision task can be used in which the participant has to decide EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES as soon as possible whether the presented letter string is The aim of this paper is to introduce a tool which the an existing word or a pseudoword. In this type of experi- authors developed for counting the Coltheart’s N and DL ment, reaction times and error rates are reported. Another distance in the Czech language and propose its applica- approach is the use of technology-based methods, such tion in ongoing research. The research aims to answer as data collection from eye-tracking or fMRI during word whether or not the Coltheart’s N and Damerau-Leven- reading (for more information see for example Schloss shtein distance can be used in reading research in the 2017). This method provides more comprehensive infor- Czech language and what the specifics of its use are in mation, but at the same time the procedure is difficult and the Czech orthography system. sometimes it is not possible to examine sufficient number Foreign literature includes many tools for ON calcula- of participants and collect sufficient data. tion such as N-watch, which is a program that works with There are various findings concerning the ortho- any language database (corpus). Using this programme, graphic neighbourhood effect on reading in foreign it is possible to identify the same characteristics as in the 2 Frequency trajectory expresses how common is the word in childhood and adulthood. 3 Available at: <https://wiki.korpus.cz/doku.php/seznamy:srovnavaci_seznamy> 55 Ptáčková, Vitásková case of the Coltheart’s N only by insertion of source data In[4]:= DamerauLevenshteinDistance ["máma", "máslo"] (national corpus) and the list of words one is interested Out[4]= 3 in (Davis 2005). Unfortunately, this tool does not work witch Czech diacritics— ´ and ˇ, so it is not practicable Figure 1: Example of WolframMathematica using the for the purposes of the present research.
Recommended publications
  • Teaching Initial Reading and Writing at the Very Beginning of Compulsory School Attendance in the Czech Primary School Phdr
    FELA Initial Teaching in Europe 1st Symposium in Klagenfurt 2015 Teaching initial reading and writing at the very beginning of compulsory school attendance in the Czech Primary School PhDr. Marie Ernestová, Czech Republic 1 Introductory Facts • language typology: Czech is a flectional language • 7 cases in both singular and plural noun declination patterns and 6 forms in verbal conjugation (3 in the singular and 3 in the plural) • illogical grammar gender system (table=masculine, chair=feminine, window=neuter). 2 • language genealogy: Czech is a West Slavic language • with highly phonemic orthography it may be described as having regular spelling • the Czech alphabet consists of 42 letters (including the digraph ch, which is considered a single letter in Czech): • a á b c č d ď e é ě f g h ch i í j k l m n ň o ó p q r ř s š t ť u ú ů v w x y ý z ž 3 • the Czech orthographic system is diacritic. The háček /ˇ/ is added to standard Latin letters for expressing sounds which are foreign to the Latin language. The acute accent is used for long vowels. • There are two ways in Czech to write long [u:]: ú or ů. 4 • only one digraph: ´ch´ always pronounced the same, as /X/ as in the English loch. • All over Europe the ch-consonant combination makes problems as this ´/X/´ sound can be spelt in as many as eighteen ways: c, ć, ç,ċ,ĉ, č, ch, çh, ci, cs,cz,tch, tj,tš, tsch, tsi, tsj, tx and even k.
    [Show full text]
  • ABSTRACT Orthographic Reform and Language Planning in Russian
    ABSTRACT Orthographic Reform and Language Planning in Russian History Sylvia Conatser Segura Director: Adrienne Harris, Ph.D. An oft-neglected topic of book history and culture, both in histories of writing development and in cultural histories of Russia and Eastern Europe, is the rich history of orthographic reform and language planning in Russian and Slavic history. Even more neglected are the cultural, religious, and political drivers of language planning and the relationship of identity to writing systems. This thesis aims to correct this oversight by synthesizing histories on the development of written Russian until its codification by Peter the Great in the early 18th century as well as the language reforms accomplished under the Bolsheviks in the 20th century. It will present analysis on motivations for these two periods of language reform as well as impulses behind resistance thereof; the resulting evidence will demonstrate the close ties of political and cultural, particularly religious, identity to the use of written systems as illustrated by the relationship of Eastern Orthodoxy to the Cyrillic alphabet. APPROVED BY DIRECTOR OF HONORS THESIS _________________________________________ Dr. Adrienne Harris, Department of Modern Languages and Culture APPROVED BY THE HONORS PROGRAM ___________________________________ Dr. Elizabeth Corey, Director DATE: ____________________ ii ORTHOGRAPHIC REFORM AND LANGUAGE PLANNING IN RUSSIAN HISTORY A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Baylor University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Honors Program By Sylvia Conatser Segura Waco, Texas May 2020 TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements . iii. Introduction . 1 Chapter One: Peter I and the Rise of Russian Print Culture . 7 Chapter Two: The 20th Century and the Maturation of a Writing System .
    [Show full text]
  • Orthographies in Early Modern Europe
    Orthographies in Early Modern Europe Orthographies in Early Modern Europe Edited by Susan Baddeley Anja Voeste De Gruyter Mouton An electronic version of this book is freely available, thanks to the support of libra- ries working with Knowledge Unlatched. KU is a collaborative initiative designed to make high quality books Open Access. More information about the initiative can be found at www.knowledgeunlatched.org An electronic version of this book is freely available, thanks to the support of libra- ries working with Knowledge Unlatched. KU is a collaborative initiative designed to make high quality books Open Access. More information about the initiative can be found at www.knowledgeunlatched.org ISBN 978-3-11-021808-4 e-ISBN (PDF) 978-3-11-021809-1 e-ISBN (EPUB) 978-3-11-021806-2 ISSN 0179-0986 e-ISSN 0179-3256 ThisISBN work 978-3-11-021808-4 is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 3.0 License, ase-ISBN of February (PDF) 978-3-11-021809-1 23, 2017. For details go to http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/. e-ISBN (EPUB) 978-3-11-021806-2 LibraryISSN 0179-0986 of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Ae-ISSN CIP catalog 0179-3256 record for this book has been applied for at the Library of Congress. ISBN 978-3-11-028812-4 e-ISBNBibliografische 978-3-11-028817-9 Information der Deutschen Nationalbibliothek Die Deutsche Nationalbibliothek verzeichnet diese Publikation in der Deutschen Nationalbibliogra- fie;This detaillierte work is licensed bibliografische under the DatenCreative sind Commons im Internet Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs über 3.0 License, Libraryhttp://dnb.dnb.deas of February of Congress 23, 2017.abrufbar.
    [Show full text]
  • Downloaded on 2017-02-12T11:34:32Z a Cross-Linguistic Database of Children’S Printed Words in Three Slavic Languages
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Cork Open Research Archive Title A cross linguistic database of children's printed words in three Slavic languages Author(s) Garabik, Radovan; Caravolus, Marketa; Kessler, Brett; Hoeflerova, Eva; Masterson, Jackie; Mikulajova, Marina; Szczerbinski, Marcin; Wierzchon, Piotr Editor(s) Levicka, Jana Garabik, Radovan Publication date 2007 Original citation Garabík, R., Caravolas, M., Kessler, B., Höflerová, E., Masterson, J., Mikulajová, M., Szczerbiński, M., Wierzchoń, P. (2007). 'A cross- linguistic database of children’s printed words in three Slavic languages'. In Levická, J., & Garabík, R. (Eds.). Computer Treatment of Slavic and East European Languages: Fourth International Seminar, Bratislava, Slovakia, 25−27 October 2007: Proceedings (pp. 51−64). Bratislava: Tribun. Type of publication Conference item Link to publisher's http://spell.psychology.wustl.edu/Garabík2007/ version http://korpus.juls.savba.sk/~slovko/2007/ Access to the full text of the published version may require a subscription. Rights © 2007, the respective authors. The articles can be used under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 3.0 Unported License. http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/ Item downloaded http://hdl.handle.net/10468/736 from Downloaded on 2017-02-12T11:34:32Z A Cross-linguistic Database of Children’s Printed Words in Three Slavic Languages Radovan Garabík1, Markéta Caravolas2, Brett Kessler3, Eva Höflerová4, Jackie Masterson5, Marína Mikulajová6,
    [Show full text]
  • Orthographic and Morphological Correspondences Between Related Slavic Languages As a Base for Modeling of Mutual Intelligibility
    Orthographic and Morphological Correspondences between Related Slavic Languages as a Base for Modeling of Mutual Intelligibility Andrea Fischer, Klára Jágrová, Irina Stenger, Tania Avgustinova, Dietrich Klakow, and Roland Marti Saarland University, Collaborative Research Center (SFB) 1102: Information Density and Linguistic Encoding, Campus A 2.2, 66123 Saarbrücken, Germany Project C4: INCOMSLAV – Mutual Intelligibility and Surprisal in Slavic Intercomprehension {kjagrova, avgustinova}@coli.uni- saarland.de {ira.stenger, rwmslav}@mx.uni- saarland.de {andrea.fischer, dietrich.klakow}@lsv.uni- saarland.de http://www.sfb1102.uni-saarland.de Abstract In an intercomprehension scenario, typically a native speaker of language L1 is confronted with output from an unknown, but related language L2. In this setting, the degree to which the receiver recognizes the unfamiliar words greatly determines communicative success. Despite exhibiting great string-level differences, cognates may be recognized very successfully if the receiver is aware of regular correspondences which allow to transform the unknown word into its familiar form. Modeling L1-L2 intercomprehension then requires the identification of all the regular correspondences between languages L1 and L2. We here present a set of linguistic orthographic correspondences manually compiled from comparative linguistics literature along with a set of statistically-inferred suggestions for correspondence rules. In order to do statistical inference, we followed the Minimum Description Length principle, which proposes to choose those rules which are most effective at describing the data. Our statistical model was able to reproduce most of our linguistic correspondences (88.5% for Czech-Polish and 75.7% for Bulgarian-Russian) and furthermore allowed to easily identify many more non-trivial correspondences which also cover aspects of morphology.
    [Show full text]
  • A Diglossia Approach
    University of Kentucky UKnowledge Linguistics Faculty Publications Linguistics 2002 Slovak Standard Language Development in the 15th–18th Centuries: A Diglossia Approach Mark Richard Lauersdorf University of Kentucky, [email protected] Right click to open a feedback form in a new tab to let us know how this document benefits oy u. Follow this and additional works at: https://uknowledge.uky.edu/lin_facpub Part of the Anthropological Linguistics and Sociolinguistics Commons, Comparative and Historical Linguistics Commons, Other Linguistics Commons, and the Slavic Languages and Societies Commons Repository Citation Lauersdorf, Mark Richard, "Slovak Standard Language Development in the 15th–18th Centuries: A Diglossia Approach" (2002). Linguistics Faculty Publications. 33. https://uknowledge.uky.edu/lin_facpub/33 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Linguistics at UKnowledge. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of UKnowledge. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Slovak Standard Language Development in the 15th–18th Centuries: A Diglossia Approach This article is available at UKnowledge: https://uknowledge.uky.edu/lin_facpub/33 Twenty Mark R. Lauersdorf Slovak Standard Language Development in the 15th–18th Centuries: A Diglossia Approach 1. Introduction Since Charles Ferguson’s initial formal description of “diglossia” in 1959 (Ferguson 1959), research based on this sociolinguistic concept has flourished.1 The framework of diglossia, in its original, Fergusonian configuration and in various expanded and revised forms, has been applied to languages and linguis- tic situations around the world, including many of the Slavic languages.2 Charles Townsend, to whom this Festschrift is dedicated, has himself con- tributed to the body of literature dealing with questions of diglossia in the Slavic-speaking world (e.g., Townsend 1987).
    [Show full text]
  • Sound Form Signalization in L1 Polish, Czech and Slovak Textbooks
    SOUND FORM SIGNALIZATION IN L1 POLISH, CZECH AND SLOVAK TEXTBOOKS In search of best practices ELŻBIETA AWRAMIUK*, JANA VLČKOVÁ-MEJVALDOVÁ** & ĽUDMILA LIPTÁKOVÁ*** *Uniwersytet w Białymstoku, Poland; **Univerzita Karlova, Czech Republic, *** Prešovská univerzita v Prešove, Slovakia Abstract Phonetic transcription is concerned with how the sounds used in spoken language are represented in written form. In specialized sources, phonetic transcription is a conventionalized notation system; in non- specialist sources, the methods of sound form signalization (SFS) are less conventionalized, but they have important educational functions. The purpose of this study is to present the results of a comparative analysis of several L1 Polish, Czech, and Slovak textbooks to answer the following questions: how sound form is signalized and what practices are best for the development of pupils’ phonetic awareness and more generally for the improvement of their spoken and written communication skills. Textbooks from the second stage of primary schools (Grades 4–6, age 10–13) were analyzed. This quali- tative analysis focuses on searching for instances where orthographic representation changes to fulfil the needs of SFS and where the sound form of language represents the point of didactic interest; it illustrates the function of SFS and its means, as well as compares results obtained in three countries. Keywords: primary school, textbook analysis, respelling, West Slavic languages 1 Awramiuk, E., Vlčková-Mejvaldová, J., Liptáková, Ľ. (2021). Sound form signalization in L1 Polish, Czech and Slovak textbooks: In search of best practices. L1-Educational Studies in Lan- guage and Literature, 21, 1-27. https://doi.org/10.17239/L1ESLL-2021.21.01.01 Corresponding author: Elżbieta Awramiuk, Faculty of Philology, Uniwersytet w Białymstoku, Plac Niezależnego Zrzeszenia Studentów 1, 15-420 Białystok, Poland.
    [Show full text]
  • Historical Perspectives of English Studies in Czech Humanities
    Historical Perspectives of English Studies in Czech Humanities A Working Programme of English Studies for Democritus Association and Linguistic Residualism DEMOCRITUS ASSOCIATION Prague 2001 2 First edition Printed in the Czech Republic 2001 © Pavel Bělíček 2001 ISBN 80-86580-02-4 3 C O N T E N T S HISTORICAL PROGRESS OF LINGUISTIC METHODOLOGY ..... 5 1. The Linguistic Method .................................................. ........................ 5 2. Ancient Origins of Philology .................................................................... 9 3. The Origins of English Philology ............................................................ 13 4. The Systematic Taxonomy of English Linguistic and Literary Studies ... 17 PATHWAYS OF COMPARATIVE LINGUISTICS.............................. 33 1. The Story of Comparative Linguistics ...................................................... 33 2. The Methods of Comparative Linguistics ................................................. 38 3. Applicative Levels in English Studies ...................................................... 41 4. The Growth of the Prague School Methodology....................................... 43 THESES TO MACROLINGUISTIC TYPOLOGY.......................... 52 1. The Typological Approach to Linguistic Change ............................... 52 2. A Typological Approach to English Grammar ........................................ 54 3. A Typological Account of English Phonology ......................................... 60 4. Theses to Macrolinguistics as a Concept of
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Machine Translation Models with Back-Translation for the Extremely Low-Resource Indigenous Language Bribri
    Neural Machine Translation Models with Back-Translation for the Extremely Low-Resource Indigenous Language Bribri Isaac Feldman Dartmouth College [email protected] Rolando Coto-Solano Dartmouth College [email protected] Abstract This paper presents a neural machine translation model and dataset for the Chibchan language Bribri, with an average performance of BLEU 16.9±1.7. This was trained on an extremely small dataset (5923 Bribri-Spanish pairs), providing evidence for the applicability of NMT in extremely low-resource environments. We discuss the challenges entailed in managing training input from languages without standard orthographies, we provide evidence of successful learning of Bribri grammar, and also examine the translations of structures that are infrequent in major Indo-European languages, such as positional verbs, ergative markers, numerical classifiers and complex demonstrative systems. In addition to this, we perform an experiment of augmenting the dataset through iterative back-translation (Sennrich et al., 2016a; Hoang et al., 2018), by using Spanish sentences to create synthetic Bribri sentences. This improves the score by an average of 1.0 BLEU, but only when the new Spanish sentences belong to the same domain as the other Spanish examples. This contributes to the small but growing body of research on Chibchan NLP. 1 Introduction State-of-the-art systems in neural machine translation require large amounts of rich and varied parallel data between the source and target language (Lui et al., 2019). While this is less of a problem for high- resource language pairs like English-German or English-Spanish, there are much fewer resources when one of the languages is an Indigenous language.
    [Show full text]
  • The Language of the Old Czech Legenda "O Svaté Kateřinĕ"
    Slavistische Beiträge ∙ Band 87 (eBook - Digi20-Retro) George M. Cummins The Language of the Old Czech Legenda "O Svaté Kateřinĕ" Verlag Otto Sagner München ∙ Berlin ∙ Washington D.C. Digitalisiert im Rahmen der Kooperation mit dem DFG-Projekt „Digi20“ der Bayerischen Staatsbibliothek, München. OCR-Bearbeitung und Erstellung des eBooks durch den Verlag Otto Sagner: http://verlag.kubon-sagner.de © bei Verlag Otto Sagner. Eine Verwertung oder Weitergabe der Texte und Abbildungen, insbesondere durch Vervielfältigung, ist ohne vorherige schriftliche Genehmigung des Verlages unzulässig. «Verlag Otto Sagner» ist ein Imprint der Kubon & Sagner GmbH. George M. Cummins - 9783954793235 Downloaded from PubFactory at 01/10/2019 05:58:22AM via free access S l a v is t ic h e B eiträge BEGRÜNDET VON ALOIS SCHMAUS HERAUSGEGEBEN VON JOHANNES HOLTHUSEN UND JOSEF SCHRENK REDAKTION: PETER REHDER Band 87 George M. Cummins - 9783954793235 Downloaded from PubFactory at 01/10/2019 05:58:22AM via free access 0004741Б GEORGE М. CUMMINS THE LANGUAGE OF THE OLD CZECH LEGENDA 0 SVATÉ KATERINÈ VERLAG OTTO SAGNER • MÜNCHEN 1975 George M. Cummins - 9783954793235 Downloaded from PubFactory at 01/10/2019 05:58:22AM via free access ן ôayensche bllothek׳6taat8b München J ISBN 3 87690 099 9 Copyright by Verlag Otto Sagner, München 1975 Abteilung der Firma Kubon und Sagner, München Druck: Alexander GroBmann 8 München 19, Ysenburgstraße 7* George M. Cummins - 9783954793235 Downloaded from PubFactory at 01/10/2019 05:58:22AM via free access ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This book is the revised text of my Harvard Ph.D. disser- tation, directed by Horace G. Lunt, and presented to the Depart- ment of Slavic Languages and L iteratures in June, 1973• The following three essays and transcription of the Old Czech Legenda 0 syaté Katefinë do not make up a rigorous textual study in the familiar sense—this work was completed half a century and more ago by the philologists Spina and Gebauer.
    [Show full text]
  • Scripts, Spelling, and Identities
    Scripts, Spelling, and Identities Mark Sebba IN MEMORIAM ALEXANDRA (“MISTY”) JAFFE, 1960–2018 WRITING AS IMAGE: ORTHOGRAPHY AND SCRIPT The written word is pervasive in the contemporary world—probably the most important method of conveying information, conducting social relations, and regulating behavior. But writing is much more than that. Writing, and what we do with it, is fundamentally bound up with our identities. As individuals, we 7 are identified partly by what kinds of literacy we engage in, the kinds of writing we do, how we learn to do it, and the purposes for which it is done. What is true for individuals is also true for larger social groupings, such as nation-states, which are strongly identified with the specific languages—and, in particular, the written languages—they use. While the relevance of written language to identity may be obvious, the role of orthography is less so. In fact, scripts and spellings play a crucial role in identity creation by making choices available at different levels of language structure and different levels of social organization, as this article will show. A well-established principle of sociolinguistics is “where there is choice, there is social meaning.” The choices that people make when they write are part of the process of constructing an identity, both for themselves and for their readers. Unlike spoken language, the written mode always depends on some set of symbols. By definition, there can be no “written language” which does not Mark Sebba works at the Department of Linguistics and English Language, University of Lancaster. His current research focuses on written bilingual and multilingual texts.
    [Show full text]
  • Unicode 4.1 and Slavic Philology - Problems and Perspectives
    https://doi.org/10.20378/irb-49297 Unicode 4.1 And Slavic Philology - ProblemS And PerspeCtiveS - Prof. Dr. Sebastian Kempgen University of Bamberg, Germany Overview Introduction Status Quo : What’s Available Problems: Missing Pieces Perspectives: More Characters ➜ Topics Covered • Russsian Hist. Orthography • Russian Phonetics • Polish Hist. Orthography • Sorbian Hist. Orthography • Croatian Accents • Bulgarian Phonetics • Old Church Slavonic • Transliteration of Glagolitic • Balkan Philology ➜ 1. Introduction ASCII: 2^7 characters = 128 ’Code Pages’: 2^8 characters = 256 Unicode: 2^16 characters = 65.536 Versions: v. 1.0 1991, v. 4.1 2005 Version 4.1: Glagolica ! ➜ Unicode Blocks ➜ Unicode Blocks Basic Latin, Latin-1, Latin Extended- A, Latin Extended-B, Latin Extended Additional; Latin Extd C (5.0) Cyrillic, Cyrillic Extended (= Non-Sl.) Greek, Greek Extended (= Hist.) IPA, IPA Extensions + Supplement Spacing & Combining Diacritics ➜ ‘Private Area’ • 6.400 Slots • Compatibility between fonts and documents not guaranteed • Use at your own risk • Already in use • Use not coordinated between philologies ➜ Unicode.org Submissions, Proposals: Technical and Philological Aspects!! Pipeline of characters and scripts Rejected scripts and characters Published Errata ! ✖ 2. Status Quo What is already available? What has been accomplished? A few appetizers Russian Translit. Transliteration for historical letters: "#: $% ! (Irish Gaelic) &': () ! (no info given) (Latin Extd-Additional) ➜ Russian Translit. * + ! (Spacing Modifiers) ➜ Macedonian Translit. ! ! ! (Latin Extd-A, B) ➜ Cassubian (Didn’t have a single CodePage) ! ➜ Nasal Vowels ,- ./ 01 23– Polish orthogr., !!!!!!!!!!! Lithuanian 45 – 6Sami, Old Icelandic 78 – 6Old Icelandic ! 59 /:6 –6 not precomposed ➜ 3. Missing Pieces Oversights, mistakes, problems, missing characters, encoding problems etc. "tokavian Accents rising falling long ; < short = > Vowels: a e i o u r ➜ "tokavian Accents á é í ó ú $ • Latin 1 à è ì ò ù%% • Latin 1 ! , - .
    [Show full text]