Chapter 14 Emergent Behaviours in Autonomous Robots

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Chapter 14 Emergent Behaviours in Autonomous Robots Chapter 14 Emergent behaviours in autonomous robots B. Hutt, K. Warwick & I. Goodhew Department of Cybernetics, University of Reading, UK. Abstract It is well known from the field of artificial life that complex and life-like behaviours can be generated by apparently simple systems. Such behaviours are considered as being emergent, as the simple systems that generate the behaviours do not themselves explicitly mention the behaviour generated. Using such an approach it is possible to generate complex life-like behaviours in robots by merely following a simple set of rules. Flocking in birds, herding in cattle and shoaling in fish are all common examples of emergent coordinated group behaviours found in nature. Despite the apparent complexity of these behaviours, it is possible to convincingly synthesise such behaviours in robots using only simple sensors and programming. The evolution of life itself can be considered as an emergent process with intelligence and many other interesting properties emerging through the mechanisms of evolution. Algorithms based on the mechanisms of evolution have been used with great success to evolve robot behaviours that are doubly emergent – where the evolved behaviour is itself an emergent property of the evolutionary process and the implementation of that behaviour is also emergent, as it is the product of simple rule following that does not mention the behaviour. 1 Introduction The natural world is teeming with a vast range of different biological organisms many of them exhibiting extremely complex behaviours. It is well known that apparently complex and life-like behaviours can be generated in artificial systems by surprisingly simple systems – a good example of this is Conway’s game of life [1]. In Conway’s game high-level patterns and structures form from extremely simple low-level rules. The game of life is played out on an infinitely large grid of square cells rather like a chessboard. In this system every cell in the grid is surrounded by eight other cells; with each cell on the grid in one of two states – dead or alive. It is the number of live cells surrounding each cell that determines the next state of the cell. The rules that govern the game of life are as follows – a dead cell with exactly three live neighbours becomes a live cell, a live cell with two or three live neighbours remains alive, in all other cases the cell becomes (or remains) dead. The elaborate patterns that can WIT Transactions on State of the Art in Science and Engineering, Vol 27, © 2006 WIT Press www.witpress.com, ISSN 1755-8336 (on-line) doi:10.2495/978-1-85312-853-0/14 448 Design and Information in Biology be produced are really quite surprising when we consider they are purely the result of following such simple rules – the game of life is one of the simplest examples of emergent complexity. 2 Complexity from simplicity – emergent behaviour from simple rules Amongst the earliest work to explore the complexity of behaviour that could be produced with simple artificial systems is the work of William Grey Walter. 2.1 Early reactive robots In 1948 William Grey Walter built his first ‘tortoise’robot. Walter was interested in the complexity of behaviour that minimally simple robots were capable of producing. Consequently, the ‘brain’ of his tortoise robot was deliberately restricted to just two functional elements (miniature radio valves). In order to discover what degree of complexity of behaviour and independence could be achieved with the smallest number of elements in a system providing the greatest number of intercon- nections [2]. The robot was based on a three wheeled (tricycle shaped) chassis enclosed by a hard shell – it had two motors one driving both rear wheels, in order to propel the robot, and the other controlling the front steering wheel. It had only two sensors; a photocell mounted on top of the steering column, at the front of the robot, pointing in the same direction as the steering wheel and a contact switch attached via the shell so that when an object pushed on the shell it closed a contact enabling collisions to be detected. Each tortoise had two basic reflexes (or ‘instincts’) that enabled it to head towards sources of light and negotiate obstacles. The primary, light seeking, behaviour used the photocell to search for and drive the robot towards sources of light, however, if the light source was too strong it caused the robot to steer away from the light instead of towards it. The second behaviour, obstacle avoidance, was triggered by the contact sensor, which caused the tortoise to retreat and then sidestep, thereby allowing the robot to avoid or push aside obstacles. The robots behaved in a phototropic manner somewhat analogous to flight paths of moths – executing graceful curves as they searched the environment for sources of light and retreating when they encountered an obstacle or got too close to a light source. Walter’s next robot was even more complex, having the ability to learn a simple conditioned response. This robot possessed an additional module ‘bolted on’ to the back of his previous robot which created a connection between the robot’s light reflex, its contact reflex and a third sensor that detected when a whistle was blown. The robot could be trained by blowing the whistle and then kicking the robot initiating the obstacle avoidance reflex. After five or six beatings, whenever the whistle was blown [the robot] turned and backed from an ‘imagined’ obstacle [3]. The level of autonomy and behavioural complexity achieved by Walter’s tortoise robots is really quite surprising, especially considering the technological limitations of the day. Walter had produced a proof of concept that complex behaviours can stem from the interaction of a few ingeniously connected but simple parts [4]. These machines are perhaps the simplest that can be said to resemble animals. Crude though they are, they give an eerie impression of purposefulness, independence and spontaneity [2]. WIT Transactions on State of the Art in Science and Engineering, Vol 27, © 2006 WIT Press www.witpress.com, ISSN 1755-8336 (on-line) Emergent Behaviours in Autonomous Robots 449 2.2 Thought experiments with simple robots Unfortunately it appears that the significance of Walter’s work was not properly understood at the time and it was not until the mid 1980’s that researchers once again started to consider the poten- tial capabilities of simple mobile autonomous robots [5]. Through a set of thought experiments Braitenberg investigated the properties of a number of simple robot controllers capable of generat- ing unexpectedly complex behaviours. He argued that if we did not know the simple principles by which the robots were functioning we might call such behaviours ‘aggression’, ‘love’, ‘foresight’ and even ‘optimism’. Interestingly, Braitenberg also considered the possibility of using evolution to design robot controllers as well as the effects of Hebbian style learning. 3 Modern reactive robots In the mid 1980’s Rodney Brook’s group at MIT began to examine the performance of simple layered controllers with real robots using his subsumption architecture ([6, 7]). Robots designed and programmed using such methods are capable of exploring a typical office environment in real time, reacting to and avoiding obstacles with few problems. The performance of such robots was significantly better than other mobile robots reportedly in existence at the time so that in [8] the following claim was (rather modestly) made: We claim as of mid-1987 our robots, using the subsumption architecture to implement complete Creatures, are the most reactive real-time mobile robots in existence. Most other mobile robots are still at the stage of ‘experimental run’in static environments, or at best in completely mapped static environments. Ours, on the other hand, operate completely autonomously in complex dynamic environments at the flick of their on switches, and continue until their batteries are drained [8]. 3.1 Reactive robots Essential to these systems is the concept of emergent behaviour, i.e. the idea that a behaviour is often the by-product of simple rule following that does not specifically mention the behaviour exhibited. Basic Braitenberg vehicles of the type shown in Fig. 1 provide an extremely simple example of emergent behaviour, such vehicles are incredibly minimal but can exhibit surprisingly complex behaviour [5]. Some schematics of basic Braitenberg vehicles are shown in Fig. 1. Two different vehicles are shown. Each vehicle has two light-sensitive sensors and two motors, with which to move. The sensors are used to directly control the motors, such that if a sensor is activated by a light source its output enables power to be supplied to a connected motor. There are two obvious possibili- ties, either the sensors can control the motors on the same side of the vehicle as the sensor, or they can control the motors on the opposite side. When exposed to a light source the vehicle pictured on the left in Fig. 1 will try to evade it, whilst the vehicle shown on the right will tend to approach it. There is no explicit mention of any light evading or following goal in such systems, rather the behaviour is implicit in the structure of the robot itself and the way in which its body allows it to interact with the environment. More importantly there is no representation or internal model of the world and therefore there is no separate perception, planning or action. Effectively the control system is entirely decentralised. WIT Transactions on State of the Art in Science and Engineering, Vol 27, © 2006 WIT Press www.witpress.com, ISSN 1755-8336 (on-line) 450 Design and Information in Biology Figure 1: Braitenberg vehicle schematic.
Recommended publications
  • Introduction to Robotics. Sensors and Actuators
    Introduction to Computer Vision and Robotics Florian Teich and Tomas Kulvicius* Introduction to Robotics. Sensors and Actuators Large portion of slides are adopted from Florentin Wörgötter, John Hallam and Simon Reich *[email protected] Perception-Action loop Environment Object Eyes Action Perception Arm Brain Perception-Action loop (cont.) Environment Sense Object Cameras Action Perception Robot-arm Computer Act Plan Outline of the course • L1.CV1: Introduction to Computer Vision. Thresholding, Filtering & Connected Coomponents • L2.CV2: Bilateral Filtering, Morphological Operators & Edge Detection • L3.CV3: Corner Detection & Non-Local Filtering • L4.R1: Introduction to Robotics. Sensors and actuators • L5.R2: Movement generation methods • L6.R3: Path planning algorithms • L7.CV4: Line/Circle Detection, Template Matching & Feature Detection • L8.CV5: Segmentation • L9.CV6: Fate Detection, Pedestrian Tracking & Computer Vision in 3D • L10.R4: Robot kinematics and control • L11.R5: Learning algorithms in robotics I: Supervised and unsupervised learning • L12.R6: Learning algorithms in robotics II: Reinforcement learning and genetic algorithms Introduction to Robotics History of robotics • Karel Čapek was one of the most influential Czech writers of the 20th century and a Nobel Prize nominee (1936). • He introduced and made popular the frequently used international word robot, which first appeared in his play R.U.R. (Rossum's Universal Robots) in 1921. 1890-1938 • “Robot” comes from the Czech word “robota”, meaning “forced labor” • Karel named his brother Josef Čapek as the true inventor of the word robot. History of robotics (cont.) • The word "robotics" also comes from science fiction - it first appeared in the short story "Runaround" (1942) by American writer Isaac Asimov.
    [Show full text]
  • For Biological Systems, Maintaining Essential Variables Within Viability Limits Is Not Passive
    Second-Order Cybernetics Maintaining Essential Variables Is Not Passive Matthew Egbert « 8 » Proponents of the enactive ap- sible failure to do so will necessarily result “ the survival standard itself is inadequate for proach agree that the homeostat’s double in the homeostat’s death” (§10). However, at the evaluation of life. If mere assurance of per- feedback architecture made an important the level of its physical body, an organism is manence were the point that mattered, life should contribution, but at the same time they always far-from-equilibrium with respect to not have started out in the first place. It is essen- struggle to overcome its limitations (Ike- its environment. Falling into an equilibrium tially precarious and corruptible being, an ad- gami & Suzuki 2008). Franchi briefly refers is the same as dying, because the organism venture in mortality, and in no possible form as to evolutionary robotics models inspired by would lose its ability to do the work of self- assured of enduring as an inorganic body can be. this ultrastability mechanism, but he does producing its own material identity, i.e., the Not duration as such, but ‘duration of what?’ is the not mention that further progress has been very process that ensures that the double question.” (Jonas 2001: 106) difficult. Ezequiel Di Paolo (2003) showed feedback loop between behavior and inter- that implementing Ashby’s mechanism is a nal homeostasis is intrinsically connected Tom Froese graduated with a D.Phil. in cognitive science significant step toward more organism-like within a whole. Only non-living matter can from the Univ.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Jeffrey Gu, Nicolas Wilmer Professor Evan Donahue ISS390S 12/7/18 the Evolution of the Roomba and the AI Field Recently, the C
    1 Jeffrey Gu, Nicolas Wilmer Professor Evan Donahue ISS390S 12/7/18 The Evolution of the Roomba and the AI Field Recently, the consumer marketplace has been flooded with a number of intelligent technologies, including many that integrate virtual assistants such as Amazon’s Alexa and Apple’s Siri. Even household appliances like Samsung’s newest refrigerators integrate “smart” technologies. For many in the tech world, artificial intelligence (AI) has long represented the future of cybernetics and technology. For many years, however, AI technologies were not marketed to the average consumer. Rather, most AI companies were focused on corporate, military and governmental applications. Thus, there seemed to be a marked difference in expectations for AI between ordinary people and those involved in the AI field. When asked about the potential of AI, many people would passionately discuss its future applications in education and healthcare.1 However, Mitch Waldrop during a panel discussion featuring several prominent figures in the AI field noted that these applications “seem to be in roughly the inverse priority to what AI people give these subjects.”2 This disconnect began to disappear in 2002, when iRobot, a company founded in 1990 by three former MIT AI Lab scientists, released the Roomba, a robot that autonomously roams and cleans the floors of your house. The commercial success of the Roomba and its business model brought the AI conversation to households around the world. 1 McDermott, Drew, M. Mitchell Waldrop, B. Chandrasekaran, John McDermott, and Roger Schank. 1985. “The Dark Ages of AI: A Panel Discussion at AAAI-84.” AI Magazine 6 (3): 122–122.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cybernetic Brain
    THE CYBERNETIC BRAIN THE CYBERNETIC BRAIN SKETCHES OF ANOTHER FUTURE Andrew Pickering THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS CHICAGO AND LONDON ANDREW PICKERING IS PROFESSOR OF SOCIOLOGY AND PHILOSOPHY AT THE UNIVERSITY OF EXETER. HIS BOOKS INCLUDE CONSTRUCTING QUARKS: A SO- CIOLOGICAL HISTORY OF PARTICLE PHYSICS, THE MANGLE OF PRACTICE: TIME, AGENCY, AND SCIENCE, AND SCIENCE AS PRACTICE AND CULTURE, A L L PUBLISHED BY THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS, AND THE MANGLE IN PRAC- TICE: SCIENCE, SOCIETY, AND BECOMING (COEDITED WITH KEITH GUZIK). THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS, CHICAGO 60637 THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS, LTD., LONDON © 2010 BY THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. PUBLISHED 2010 PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 1 2 3 4 5 ISBN-13: 978-0-226-66789-8 (CLOTH) ISBN-10: 0-226-66789-8 (CLOTH) Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Pickering, Andrew. The cybernetic brain : sketches of another future / Andrew Pickering. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN-13: 978-0-226-66789-8 (cloth : alk. paper) ISBN-10: 0-226-66789-8 (cloth : alk. paper) 1. Cybernetics. 2. Cybernetics—History. 3. Brain. 4. Self-organizing systems. I. Title. Q310.P53 2010 003’.5—dc22 2009023367 a THE PAPER USED IN THIS PUBLICATION MEETS THE MINIMUM REQUIREMENTS OF THE AMERICAN NATIONAL STANDARD FOR INFORMATION SCIENCES—PERMA- NENCE OF PAPER FOR PRINTED LIBRARY MATERIALS, ANSI Z39.48-1992. DEDICATION For Jane F. CONTENTS Acknowledgments / ix 1. The Adaptive Brain / 1 2. Ontological Theater / 17 PART 1: PSYCHIATRY TO CYBERNETICS 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Boden Grey Walter's
    Grey Walter’s Anticipatory Tortoises Margaret Boden Grey Walter and the Ratio Club The British physiologist William Grey Walter (1910–1977) was an early member of the interdisciplinary Ratio Club. This was a small dining club that met several times a year from 1949 to 1955, with a nostalgic final meeting in 1958, at London’s National Hospital for Neurological Diseases. The founder-secretary was the neurosurgeon John Bates, who had worked (alongside the psychologist Kenneth Craik) on servomechanisms for gun turrets during the war. The club was a pioneering source of ideas in what Norbert Wiener had recently dubbed ‘cybernetics’.1 Indeed, Bates’ archive shows that the letter inviting membership spoke of ‘people who had Wiener’s ideas before Wiener’s book appeared’.2 In fact, its founders had considered calling it the Craik Club, in memory of Craik’s work—not least, his stress on ‘synthetic’ models of psychological theories.3 In short, the club was the nucleus of a thriving British tradition of cybernetics, started independently of the transatlantic version. The Ratio members—about twenty at any given time—were a very carefully chosen group. Several of them had been involved in wartime signals research or intelligence work at Bletchley Park, where Alan Turing had used primitive computers to decipher the Nazis’ Enigma code.4 They were drawn from a wide range of disciplines: clinical psychiatry and neurology, physiology, neuroanatomy, mathematics/statistics, physics, astrophysics, and the new areas of control engineering and computer science.5 The aim was to discuss novel ideas: their own, and those of guests—such as Warren McCulloch.
    [Show full text]
  • Kunst Und Kybernetik“
    DIPLOMARBEIT Titel der Diplomarbeit „Kunst und Kybernetik“ Verfasserin Felicia Hönig angestrebter akademischer Grad Magistra der Philosophie (Mag. phil.) Wien, im September 2008 Studienkennzahl lt. Studienblatt A296 Studienrichtung lt. Studienblatt Philosophie Betreuer: Ao. Univ. –Prof. Dr. Herbert Hrachovec Inhalt Vorwort .......................................................................................5 1. Einleitung................................................................................7 2. Kybernetik, der Anfang.........................................................11 2.1. Der Begriff .................................................................................12 2.2. Norbert Wiener ..........................................................................14 2.3. Die Bausteine des Theorie-Werdens, drei Publikationen aus den 1940er Jahren...........................................................................15 2.3.1. Norbert Wiener, Arturo Rosenblueth, and Julian Bigelow , 1943, „Behavior, Purpose and Teleology“, Philosophy of Science 10: 18-24. ........................................................................................................ 15 2.3.2. Warren McCulloch, Walter Pitts: „A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity“. In: Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5 (1943) 115-133........................................................... 18 2.3.3. C. E. Shannon, „A mathematical theory of communication“, Bell System Technical Journal, vol. 27, pp. 379-423 and 623-656, July and October,
    [Show full text]
  • The Biological Turn in Computation, Communication, and Control
    Vital Networks: The Biological Turn in Computation, Communication, and Control By Sandra Robinson A thesis submitted to the Graduate Program in Sociology in conformity with the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Queen’s University Kingston, Ontario, Canada January 2014 Copyright © Sandra Robinson, 2014 Abstract Networks, such as the Internet, are comprised of dense information flows with expansive, multi-directional reach that continuously change—and this changeability is what keeps the network active, relative, and vital. I call the form of network exhibiting those dynamic features the vital network. This form of network is not simply the outcome of connectivity and communication between diverse affiliative objects and actors such as cell phones and humans that together convey a sense or feeling of ‘aliveness;’ it is the outcome of deliberate software programming goals for communication systems inspired by nonhuman, self-organizing biological life. The biological turn in computation produces an organizing logic for the vital network that self-propagates connections and disconnections, services, collectives, and structures proximal to forms that feel vital and dynamic. The vital network can do things, it has capacities to act, and different material consequences emerge out of the organization and coordination of communication with particular implications for human privacy, autonomy, and network transparency. In this dissertation, I examine the biological turn in computing as a crucial feature within a development program for the design of digital network control systems that rely on self-regulation and autonomous communication processes intentionally constructed to be non-transparent—to be unseen. I explore nonhuman models of control as a response to this requirement considered through three objects: microbe, simulation, and control, each understood in process terms that disclose what these things do and how they act.
    [Show full text]
  • The First Biologically Inspired Robots Owen Holland
    Robotica (2003) volume 21, pp. 351–363. © 2003 Cambridge University Press DOI: 10.1017/S0263574703004971 Printed in the United Kingdom The first biologically inspired robots Owen Holland Department of Computer Science, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ (UK). E-mail: [email protected] (Received in Final Form: June 15, 2002) SUMMARY Star. After attending Westminster School, he studied Thie first biologically inspired robots, the famous electro- physiology at Cambridge, and began his research career in mechanical tortoises, were designed and built in 1949 by W. neurophysiology with a dissertation on ‘Conduction in Grey Walter. This paper reviews their origins in Walter’s Nerve and Muscle’. In 1935 he became interested in the new theories of the brain and the nature of life, and uses area of electroencephalography, and quickly showed a talent contemporary unpublished notes and photographs to assess for developing new electronic equipment, which he used to their significance then and now. make a series of fundamental technical and clinical discoveries. In 1939 he was appointed Director of Physiol- ogy at the newly founded Burden Neurological Institute KEYWORDS: First biological robots; Robot tortoises. (BNI) in Bristol; he remained there for the rest of his career. His war work involved him in a variety of activities, including the development of radar, and the use of humans INTRODUCTION in control loops. After the war he recruited a brilliant group Most articles and books on the history of robotics make only 1,2,3 of ex-radar engineers to work with him at the BNI; their a passing reference to Grey Walter’s robot tortoises, technical expertise, combined with Grey Walter’s vision and regarding them as quaint period curiosities quite unrelated creativity, made a major contribution to the development of to our intellectually and technically sophisticated modern electroencephalography over the next twenty years.
    [Show full text]
  • William Grey Walter and His Tortoises
    A Wizard and A Pioneer: William Grey Walter and his Tortoises Sir Winston Churchill in his history of the Second World War wrote a chapter on the “wizards” who had helped Britain win the war in the air by the development and use of radar. Sir Winston Churchill in his history of the Second World War wrote a chapter on the “wizards” who had helped Britain win the war in the air by the development and use of radar. William Grey Walter (1910-1977) was one of those young wizards. William Grey Walter William Grey Walter Neurophysiology and Brain Waves Dr. William Grey Walter contributed great improvement to the technologies of brain waves(EEG:electroencephalography). Identify the source of the alpha rhythm Hans Berger in 1929 discovered alpha wave, inferred it came from the frontal lobes William Grey Walter found the source in the occipital lobe. Invented use of delta wave to locate brain tumors This typical instrument displays the brain wave traces with an ink-writing pen on moving papers is conceived by Dr. Walter. Neurophysiology and Brain Waves Discovered contingent negative variation (CNV) This is a very slow change in electrical potential at and around the vertex of the head It was seen only after a warning signal had been given to a human subject, who would then plan a possible movement in anticipation of a second signal Observer can predict a subject will make a response half to one second ahead, before the subject is aware of an intention to act Intentional actions are initiated before awareness of such actions emerges Neurophysiology and Brain Waves Hypothesis about Alpha Wave activities The alpha represented 'scanning' by the brain in search of local centers of activity when none was present It stopped when a 'target' was found in the cortex This hypothesis was and still is controversial, but it is still not disproven Cybernetics and Robot Tortoise Brains are simpler than many of us have supposed In 1951, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Fact Sheet: History of Robotics
    Fact Sheet: History of Robotics www.RazorRobotics.com ≈250 B.C. - Ctesibius, an ancient Greek engineer and mathematician, invented a water clock which was the most accurate for nearly 2000 years. ≈60 A.D. - Hero of Alexandria designs the first automated programmable machine. These 'Automata' were made from a container of gradually releasing sand connected to a spindle via a string. By using different configurations of these pulleys, it was possible to repeatably move a statue on a pre-defined path. 1898 - The first radio-controlled submersible boat was invented by Nikola Tesla. 1921 - The term 'Robot' was coined by Karel Capek in the play 'Rossum's Universal Robots'. 1941 - Isaac Asimov introduced the word 'Robotics' in the science fiction short story 'Liar!' 1948 - William Grey Walter builds Elmer and Elsie, two of the earliest autonomous robots with the appearance of turtles. The robots used simple rules to produce complex behaviours. 1954 - The first silicon transistor was produced by Texas Instruments. 1956 - George Devol applied for a patent for the first programmable robot, later named 'Unimate'. 1957 - Launch of the first artificial satellite, Sputnik 1. I, Robot Turtle robot Sputnik 1 1961 - First Unimate robot installed at General Motors. Used for welding and die casting. 1965 - Gordon E. Moore introduces the concept 'Moore's law', which predicts the number of components on a single chip would double every two years. 1966 - Work began on the 'Shakey' robot at Stanford Research Institute. 'Shakey' was capable of planning, route-finding and moving objects. 1969 - The Apollo 11 mission, puts the first man on the moon.
    [Show full text]
  • Portrayals and Perceptions of AI and Why They Matter
    Portrayals and perceptions of AI and why they matter 1 Portrayals and perceptions of AI and why they matter Cover Image Turk playing chess, design by Wolfgang von Kempelen (1734 – 1804), built by Christoph 2 Portrayals and perceptions of AI and why they matter Mechel, 1769, colour engraving, circa 1780. Contents Executive summary 4 Introduction 5 Narratives and artificial intelligence 5 The AI narratives project 6 AI narratives 7 A very brief history of imagining intelligent machines 7 Embodiment 8 Extremes and control 9 Narrative and emerging technologies: lessons from previous science and technology debates 10 Implications 14 Disconnect from the reality of the technology 14 Underrepresented narratives and narrators 15 Constraints 16 The role of practitioners 20 Communicating AI 20 Reshaping AI narratives 21 The importance of dialogue 22 Annexes 24 Portrayals and perceptions of AI and why they matter 3 Executive summary The AI narratives project – a joint endeavour by the Leverhulme Centre for the Future of Intelligence and the Royal Society – has been examining how researchers, communicators, policymakers, and publics talk about artificial intelligence, and why this matters. This write-up presents an account of how AI is portrayed Narratives are essential to the development of science and perceived in the English-speaking West, with a and people’s engagement with new knowledge and new particular focus on the UK. It explores the limitations applications. Both fictional and non-fictional narratives of prevalent fictional and non-fictional narratives and have real world effects. Recent historical examples of the suggests how practitioners might move beyond them. evolution of disruptive technologies and public debates Its primary audience is professionals with an interest in with a strong science component (such as genetic public discourse about AI, including those in the media, modification, nuclear energy and climate change) offer government, academia, and industry.
    [Show full text]
  • 6 X 10.Long New.P65
    Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-19963-6 - A Cybernetic View of Biological Growth: The Maia Hypothesis Tony Stebbing Excerpt More information 1 Introduction The broadest and most complete definition of Life will be – The continu- ous adjustment of internal relations to external relations. Herbert Spencer The truths of cybernetics are not conditional on their being derived from some other branch of science. Cybernetics has its own foundations. W. Ross Ashby Among the most fertile ideas introduced into biology in recent years are those of cybernetics ... control theory obtrudes everywhere into biology. Peter B. Medawar THE DEVELOPMENT OF BIOLOGICAL CYBERNETICS I watched a kestrel from a building high on the Citadel overlooking Plymouth Sound. It hovered at arms’ length from the window in a stiff breeze, holding its position for long periods so perfectly that one could not detect the slightest movement of its head. To maintain this static hover, its wings beat quickly, varying a little in frequency and angle to adjust to the buffeting breeze, as it watched intently for any move- ment of prey on the ground below. It reminded me of Gerard Manley Hopkins’ lines from his poem The Windhover: ‘how he rung upon the rein of a wimpling wing’. Occasionally the bird sheared away: ‘then off, off forth on swing’; to return again and take up its position as before, as if to dispel any doubts I might still have of its control in the air. ‘The achieve of, the mastery of the thing!’ In the same way our control systems master the continuous variation in the environment in which we live, holding steady a host of different internal processes against the continuous fluctuation of 1 © in this web service Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-19963-6 - A Cybernetic View of Biological Growth: The Maia Hypothesis Tony Stebbing Excerpt More information 2 Introduction external change.
    [Show full text]