Signal Detection and the Evolution of Avian Singing Modes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Signal Detection and the Evolution of Avian Singing Modes Behaviour 150 (2013) 995–1013 brill.com/beh Review Why is birdsong so repetitive? Signal detection and the evolution of avian singing modes J. Jordan Price ∗ Department of Biology, St. Mary’s College of Maryland, 18952 E. Fisher Road, St. Mary’s City, MD 20686, USA *E-mail: [email protected] Accepted 20 December 2012 Abstract Signal repertoires, such as the song type repertoires of many songbirds, are thought to play a role in male mating success, with females preferring larger male repertoires over smaller ones. Yet, in many songbird species, males sing in a way that does not readily reveal their repertoire sizes by repeating each song type multiple times before switching to the next. Here I describe a potential explanation for such signal redundancy, based on the predictions of signal detection theory as it applies to intersexual communication during mate choice. According to this idea, a female’s response threshold to male mating signals (i.e., her ‘choosiness’) should select for male signal features that elicit favorable female responses (i.e., that are more ‘detectable’). Males can increase the detectability of their signals by producing them with higher redundancy, as well as by increasing their intensity and distinctiveness. Thus, in species with relatively high female response thresholds to males, such as taxa in which the sexes associate only briefly during breeding, males are expected to produce mate attraction signals that are especially stereotyped and repetitive. High signal stereotypy is also expected to be associated with features within signals that are relatively extravagant. Phylogenetic studies of a songbird group with a wide range of mating patterns, the oropendolas and caciques (family Icteridae), provide evidence consistent with these evolutionary predictions. Singing modes in this group have become more repetitive in some lineages along with the evolution of polygynous mating systems, even as various features within songs have become more extravagant. Keywords bird song, communication, mating systems, redundancy, sexual selection, signal detection theory. © 2013 Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003051 996 Signal detection and avian singing modes 1. Introduction The evolution of extravagant traits by sexual selection, particularly male traits used in mate attraction, has long been a central focus of evolutionary bi- ology (Cronin, 1991; Andersson, 1994). Complex and exaggerated features, such as the elaborate trains of peacocks (Petrie, 1994), are thought to have evolved because they are preferred by females over less elaborate features and thus confer mating advantages to males that exhibit them (Darwin, 1871; Fisher, 1930; Kirkpatrick & Ryan, 1991). In animal communication systems, female choice presumably explains many extravagant features in the mate at- traction displays of males, such as predator-attracting components in the calls of some frogs (Ryan, 1985, 1997; Akre & Ryan, 2011), the brightly colored dewlap used in anoline lizard displays (Fleishman, 1992; Fleishman & Pal- lus, 2010), and the diverse song type repertoires of many oscine passerines, or songbirds (Searcy & Yasukawa, 1996; Searcy & Nowicki, 2005; Catch- pole & Slater, 2008). While the presence of extravagant display features has been well stud- ied, another common characteristic of these signals, their stereotypy, has received comparatively little attention. Yet, this characteristic might be an equally important aspect of signal evolution. Mate attraction displays in a wide variety of taxa, including those mentioned above, are often performed in a highly stereotyped and repetitive manner, and examples include acoustic, visual and even electrical signals (Bradbury & Vehrencamp, 2011). Further- more, in species that exhibit different signaling patterns in different social contexts, displays used specifically in the context of mate attraction are usu- ally less variable and more repetitive than are signals directed at individuals other than potential mates (e.g., Spector, 1992; Kroodsma, 1999). Signaling patterns can range in organization from highly stereotyped, in which a signal is repeated with high levels of precision, to highly versatile, in which a diversity of different signals are presented in series with little rep- etition of any particular signal type. A common presumption, especially in studies of at least some songbirds (e.g., song sparrows, Melospiza melodia; red-winged blackbirds, Agelaius phoeniceus), is that female choice favors di- versity in male displays (Searcy & Yasukawa, 1996). Indeed, evidence from various well-studied songbird species suggests that females prefer males with larger song repertoires and that repertoire size is associated with male fitness (reviewed by Catchpole & Slater, 2008). Yet, surprisingly few male songbirds sing in a way that advertises their repertoire sizes. Rather, in many J.J. Price / Behaviour 150 (2013) 995–1013 997 species, males repeat each of their song types multiple times before switch- ing to the next. This suggests that signalers might be under two competing selection pressures: selection for large repertoire sizes and selection to pro- duce each signal type with high stereotypy (Nowicki et al., 2002; Searcy & Nowicki, 2005). Such signal stereotypy is common in animal mate attrac- tion displays, yet few studies have presented a testable explanation for its existence. Here, I present a potential explanation for the widespread evolution of repetitive singing patterns in songbirds, based on previous discussions of signal evolution by Wiley (1983, 1994, 2000a) suggesting that choosy fe- males should favor the evolution of male signals that are highly stereotyped and redundant. These predictions are based on signal detection theory, as it applies to intersexual communication during mate choice. 2. Mate choice as a signal detection problem In the study of animal communication, signal detection theory is most often applied to communication in noisy environments, such as in situations where signals degrade during transmission (Wiley & Richards, 1982) or are masked by other stimuli (Klump, 1996; Wollerman & Wiley, 2002; Slabbekoorn, 2004; Luther & Wiley, 2009). All forms of communication occur in the pres- ence of some form of background noise (Wiley, 2006). However, this model can also be applied effectively to communication during mate choice, as the difficulties faced by male signalers are largely the same (Wiley, 1994, 2000a). In both cases, there is a less than certain probability that a re- ceiver will respond favorably to a male’s signal, in the former case because of environmental interference and in the latter case because of a female’s fastidiousness in choosing a mate. For displaying males, these situations (environmental noise during transmission and ‘neural noise’ due to female choosiness) are effectively identical and should favor the evolution of simi- lar modifications in signal design to improve receiver response (Wiley, 1983, 1994, 2000a). When considering the evolution of mating signals, it is important to recog- nize that male signalers and female receivers have different interests (Searcy & Nowicki, 2005). Females seek reliable information about male quality as potential mates, either by direct assessment or through more indirect means (Wiley & Poston, 1996; Wiley, 2000a), whereas males often seek to attract 998 Signal detection and avian singing modes females regardless of quality (Wiley, 1983; Krebs & Dawkins, 1984). Se- lection favors females who respond only to optimal males and males who successfully elicit favorable responses (e.g., copulations) from females. The underlying assumption of signal detection theory is that communication un- der natural conditions is never noise-free and that receivers therefore make mistakes (Wiley, 1983, 1994, 2000a, 2006, 2013). In the context of mate choice, this implies that a female receiver faces inescapable trade-offs be- tween two kinds of error: choosing to mate with a suboptimal male (a false alarm) or failing to respond to the signals of an optimal male (a missed de- tection). It is impossible for females to minimize both of these types of error simultaneously; consequently, they must evolve thresholds of response based on the probabilities and relative costs of each (see Figures 1 and 2 in Wiley, 2013, this issue). The higher the response threshold, the lower the respon- siveness of a female to male signals and thus the choosier she is. For a female, the consequences of mating with a suboptimal male can include less assistance in raising offspring, less than optimal genes for her progeny, or even no progeny at all. Missing an opportunity to mate with an optimal male, on the other hand, has far less serious long-term consequences, especially when the cost of additional searching is low. Females are there- fore expected to evolve response thresholds that minimize false alarms in order to reduce the potential for costly mistakes in mating, even when such thresholds increase the probability of missed signal detections (Wiley, 1994). Females that have less time for making their choices or limited information about males should evolve even higher thresholds of response so as to mini- mize costly false alarms. As a result, these females also ignore more signals from potential mates, provided this does not cause them to miss their win- dow for successfully breeding. In contrast, when females have ample time and multiple cues for assessing the qualities of potential mates, the probabil- ities of both types
Recommended publications
  • A Bestiary of the Arts
    LA LETTRE ACADEMIE DES BEAUX-ARTS A BESTIARY OF THE ARTS 89 Issue 89 Spring 2019 Editorial • page 2 News: Annual Public Meeting of the Five Academies News: Installations under the Coupole: Adrien Goetz and Jacques Perrin News: Formal Session of the Académie des beaux-arts • pages 3 to 7 Editorial The magnificent Exhibition: “Oriental Visions: Cynocephalus adorning the cover of this edition of From Dreams into Light” La Lettre emanates a feeling of peaceful strength Musée Marmottan Monet true to the personality of its author, Pierre-Yves • pages 8 and 9 File: Trémois, the oldest member of the Académie des Beaux-Arts after being elected to Paul Lemangy’s “A bestiary of the arts” seat on 8 February 1978. • pages 10 to 34 Through the insatiable curiosity and astounding energy that he brings to the table at the age of News: “Concerts for a seat” ninety-eight, Pierre-Yves Trémois shows us Elections: Jean-Michel Othoniel, the extent to which artistic creation can be Marc Barani, Bernard Desmoulin, regenerative, especially when it is not seeking to conform to any passing trend. News: The Cabinet des estampes de la In May 2017 we elected forty-three year-old composer Bruno Mantovani to Jean bibliothèque de l’Institut Prodromidès’ seat. Tribute: Jean Cortot Watching the two passionately converse about art, we realized that the half • pages 35 to 37 century separating them was of no importance. The Académie des Beaux-Arts is known for the immense aesthetic diversity Press release: “Antônio Carlos Jobim, running throughout its different sections. highly-elaborate popular music” This reality is in stark contrast with academicism.
    [Show full text]
  • The Function of Greetings Between Male Guinea Baboons
    Ethology 109, 847—859 (2003) Ó 2003 Blackwell Verlag, Berlin ISSN 0179–1613 Primate Rituals: The Function of Greetings between Male Guinea Baboons Jessica C. Whitham & Dario Maestripieri Animal Behavior Research Group, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA Abstract The bond-testing hypothesis suggests that social animals can obtain honest information about the quality of their dyadic relationships by exchanging costly, high-risksignals (Zahavi & Zahavi 1997). We evaluated this hypothesis by investigating whether adult male baboons use intense greeting interactions to test the quality and strength of their social bonds. Intense greetings involve intimate and risky behaviors such as embracing and the diddling of the penis and/or scrotum. Data were collected on a colony of 40 Guinea baboons (Papio papio)at the Brookfield Zoo in Chicago. Fifteen adult male baboons were focally observed for 30-min sessions over a 6-mo period, resulting in 195 h of observation. We assessed the quality of male–male relationships using measures of affiliation, aggression, and social tolerance. As predicted by the bond-testing hypothesis, dyads with strong social bonds exchanged a higher frequency of intense greetings than did pairs with poor relationships. We found no support for the competing hypotheses, that suggest that greetings have an aggressive or submissive function or are used as a form of post-conflict reconciliatory behavior. Neither dominance relationships nor contextual variables were predictive of intense greeting patterns. We suggest that by imposing on his partner, a male baboon is able to obtain reliable information about this individual’s current willingness to cooperate and invest in the relationship.
    [Show full text]
  • (CCAC) Guide to the Care and Use of Experimental Animals Volume
    Canadian Council on Animal Care Conseil canadien de protection des animaux Guide to the Care and Use of Experimental Animals Volume 1, 2nd Edition Sections of this document that have been revised are replaced by links to the relevant documents. The remaining sections are undergoing revision; however, they will continue to be used for CCAC assessments until revised guidelines are published. Editors Dr E.D. Olfert Dr B.M. Cross Mrs A.A. McWilliam Director Asssistant Director Information Officer Animal Resources Centre Animal Resources Centre Canadian Council on Animal Care University of Saskatchewan University of Saskatchewan 1000-151 Slater Street Saskatoon, Saskatchewan Saskatoon, Saskatchewan Ottawa, Ontario K1P 5H3 S7N 0W0 S7N 0W0 In keeping with the CCAC policy of revising statements and guidelines as needed, users of this Guide are encouraged to forward any comments to the Secretariat. Citing certain devices or manufacturers is not to be perceived as the endorsement of the Canadian Council on Animal Care (CCAC) of one particular product over another. Publication Date: 1993 Revision Date: April 2020 © Canadian Council on Animal Care, 1993 ISBN: 0-919087-18-3 Canadian Council on Animal Care 190 O’Connor St., Suite 800 Ottawa, Ontario, K2P 2R3 http://www.ccac.ca Table of Contents TABLE OF CONTENTS DEDICATION ...................................................................................................................1 PREFACE.........................................................................................................................2
    [Show full text]
  • Integrating Behaviour Into Wildlife Conservation: the Multiple Ways That Behaviour Can Reduce Ne Laura L
    Biological Conservation 95 (2000) 303±315 www.elsevier.com/locate/biocon Integrating behaviour into wildlife conservation: the multiple ways that behaviour can reduce Ne Laura L. Anthony a, Daniel T. Blumstein a,b,c,* aDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia bDepartment of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia cThe Cooperative Research Centre for the Conservation and Management of Marsupials, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia Received 10 June 1999; received in revised form 19 August 1999; accepted 16 October 1999 Abstract There has been a recent interest in integrating an understanding of behaviour into conservation biology. Unfortunately, there has been no paradigm for such a process. Without a clear framework for integration, conservation biologists may have diculties recognising how behavioural knowledge can help solve real-world conservation problems. Eective population size (Ne) is a key demographic parameter used to understand population viability. A variety of behaviours and behavioural traits impact Ne, yet their importance for conservation is under-appreciated. We suggest that identifying behavioural traits that aect Ne provides a paradigm for integrating behavioural biology into conservation biology. Behaviour can aect Ne through at least three dierent mechanisms: reducing N Ð the population size; reducing r Ð the population growth rate, and/or by increasing reproductive skew. We discuss how nine common behavioural traits can reduce Ne, and suggest how an understanding of these traits may inform management of both free-living and captive animals. # 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Behaviour and conservation; Eective population size; Population viability 1. Introduction knowledge of animal behaviour to conservation pro- blems, there is no clear framework to help conservation Conservation biology is a crisis discipline aimed at biologists identify the speci®c cases when they should be saving biodiversity (Soule , 1986).
    [Show full text]
  • COMMUNICATION in TERRESTRIAL MAMMALS Karen Mccomb & David
    COMMUNICATION IN TERRESTRIAL MAMMALS Karen McComb & David Reby Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, BRIGHTON BN1 9QH, UK. Introduction Research on vocal communication in terrestrial mammals had until recently lagged behind work on birds and anurans (frogs and toads), perhaps because of the greater difficulty of recording, analyzing and playing back acoustically more complex signals to animals who are usually free-ranging and difficult to confine for experimental purposes. One exception is the non-human primates, which have received intense scrutiny because of their shared ancestry with humans. Although primates can be classified as terrestrial mammals, we will not attempt to comprehensively cover research on this group here (much of this is presented in other chapters of the encyclopedia) but rather focus on the other terrestrial mammals, where advances in research techniques (enabled by the revolution in digital technology) are now beginning to yield interesting results. Research on these mammals integrates study at several levels of investigation, including the functional anatomy of the vocal apparatus, the acoustic structure of the calls, the context of their emission and their relationship and co-evolution with the social structures and reproductive strategies of the animals that give them. As a consequence, research in this area now uses a range of methods and tools, including anatomical investigation, sound analyses and re-synthesis and playback experiments. While mammalian vocal communication is qualitatively different from human speech, humans are also mammals and our mechanisms of vocal production are largely shared. This means that a common framework for understanding the structure and evolution of mammal vocalizations already exists, the source- filter theory of voice production.
    [Show full text]
  • Factors That Affect Harem Stability in a Feral Horse (Equus Caballus) Population on Shackleford Banks Island, NC
    University of New Orleans ScholarWorks@UNO University of New Orleans Theses and Dissertations Dissertations and Theses Summer 8-4-2011 Factors That Affect Harem Stability in a Feral Horse (Equus caballus) Population on Shackleford Banks island, NC Jessa Madosky University of New Orleans, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.uno.edu/td Part of the Population Biology Commons, and the Zoology Commons Recommended Citation Madosky, Jessa, "Factors That Affect Harem Stability in a Feral Horse (Equus caballus) Population on Shackleford Banks island, NC" (2011). University of New Orleans Theses and Dissertations. 450. https://scholarworks.uno.edu/td/450 This Dissertation is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by ScholarWorks@UNO with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Dissertation in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/ or on the work itself. This Dissertation has been accepted for inclusion in University of New Orleans Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UNO. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Factors That Affect Harem Stability in a Feral Horse (Equus caballus) Population on Shackleford Banks island, NC A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the University of New Orleans in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Conservation Biology by Jessa Madosky B.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Role of the Elephant Pinna in Hearing
    628 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 30, 2 198A (Lighthill 1952). Because of the effects of the half-muting Lighthill, M. J. 1952. Oq sound generated aerodynami- operation on the quality of courtship song, we surmise cally. I. General theory. Proc. Roy. Soc. A., 211, that G. chopardi makes use of both of these variables to -564-587. - . - - . produce both hisses and whistles with the same pair of Masters, W. M. 1980. Insect disturbance stridulation: spiracles. The courtship whistles of half-muted males are characterization of airborne and vibrational com- consistently higher in frequency (by p to 1 kHz) than ponents of the sound. J. comp. Physiol., 135, the same males' pre-operative whisJes; they are also 259-263. interrupted frequently by brief lapses into broad-band Nelson, M. C. 1979. Sound production in the cockroach, noise. If males have a fixed motor programme for the Gromphadorhina portentosa : the sound-producing abdominal compression that forces air through the apparatus. J. comp. Physiol., 132, 27-38. tracheal horn, the rate of air flow should be higher than Nelson, M. C. & Fraser, J. 1980. Sound production in the normal through the single spiracle that is capable of cockroach, Gromphadorhina portentosa: evidence opening, causing a shift toward a higher frequency in the for communication by hissing. Behav. Ecol. resulting sound. If whistles are produced with the valve Sociobiol., 6, 305-314. partly closed and hisses are produced with the valve fully Pipher, R. D. & Morris, G. K. 1974. Frequency modu- open, the higher rate of flow in half muted males might lation in Conocephalus nigropleurum, the Black- well lead to an occasional 'blow-out' of the valve, ~idedmeadow katydid (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae).
    [Show full text]
  • Rodent Societies
    Chapter 23 Nonparental Infanticide Luis A. Ebensperger and Daniel T. Blumstein Male marmot 100 moved into the Grass Group. Male 69 siops truncatus, Patterson et al. 1998), giant otters (Ptero- seemed to oppose 100’s sudden entry, but the females of the nura brasiliensis, Mourão and Carvalho 2001), hippos group appeared to accept 100. Before male 100 moved in (Hippopotamus amphibius, Lewison 1998), plains zebras there were 9 healthy marmot pups crawling around the Grass (Equus burchelli, Pluhácˇek and Bartosˇ 2000), sportive le- Group’s main burrows. Within two weeks there was one in- murs (Lepilemur edwarsi, Rasoloharijaona et al. 2000), and jured marmot pup limping around—apparently avoiding mar- suricates (Suricata suricatta, Clutton-Brock et al. 1998). mot 100. The injured pup did not survive hibernation. (Blum- Infanticide has been noted in the wild or under labora- stein 1993:14) tory conditions in two species of hystricognath rodents and 35 species of sciurognath rodents (table 23.1). Despite the A female invaded an adjacent coterie territory and entered a difficulty of observing and quantifying infanticide in these burrow containing a recently emerged, healthy juvenile. The typically semifossorial and often nocturnal species, we know marauder emerged 5 minutes later with a distinctly bloody face, a considerable amount about the proximate regulation, evo- and then showed licking the front claws [behavior]. Several lution, and function of infanticide in rodents. Understand- minutes later the disoriented juvenile emerged with fresh, se- ing the causes and consequences of infanticide in rodents vere wounds on the face and neck. The juvenile disappeared a provides a basis for developing and testing alternative hy- few days later.
    [Show full text]
  • Infanticide-Among-Animals.Pdf
    Infanticide Among Animals: A Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for the Reproductive Strategies of Females Sarah Blaffer Hrdy Peabody Museum, Harvard Unirqersity Infanticide among animals is a widespread phenomenon Within the same species, infanticide may occur in some with no unitary explanation. Although the detrimental areas but not others, as evidenced by the variable outcome for the infant is fairly constant, individuals expression of infanticidal behavior among Hanuman responsible for infanticide may or may not benefit, and langurs. At present, the most obvious factor influencing when they gain in fitness there may be considerable facultative expression of the infanticidal trait is popu- variation in how they gain. Sources of increased fitness lation density. from infanticide include: (1) exploitation of the infant Where it occurs, sexually selected infanticide is a as a resource, (2) elimination of a competitor for re- significant cause of mortality. As such, it has important sources, (3) increased maternal survival or lifetime re- implications for the evolution of behavior, particularly productive success for either mother or father by elim- for patterns of association between males and females, ination of an ill-timed, handicapped, or supernumerary for female reproductive physiology, and for the pat- infant, and, finally, (4) increased access for individuals terning of sexual receptivity by females. It is hypothe- of one sex for reproductive investment by the other sex sized that the threat posed by infanticide is one of sev- at the expense of same-sex competitors. Predicted attri- eral pressures selecting for a shift among higher butes of the perpetrators (such as sex and degree of primates away from strictly cyclical estrous receptivity relatedness to the infant), attributes of the victim (i.e., towards socially determined or situation-dependent re- age and vulnerability), as well as schedule of gain, vary ceptivity.
    [Show full text]
  • 2010 44Th Congress, Uppsala, Sweden
    Proceedings of the 44th Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology (ISAE) Coping in large groups Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences Uppsala, Sweden 4-7 August 2010 Edited by: Lena Lidfors Harry Blokhuis Linda Keeling Applied ethology 2010: Coping in large groups Proceedings of the 44th Congress of the International Society for Applied Ethology (ISAE) Coping in large groups Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences Uppsala, Sweden 4-7 August 2010 edited by: Lena Lidfors Harry Blokhuis Linda Keeling Wageningen Academic P u b l i s h e r s This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned. Nothing from this publication may be translated, reproduced, stored in a computerised system or published in any form or in any manner, including electronic, mechanical, reprographic or photographic, without prior written permission from the publisher: Wageningen Academic Publishers P.O. Box 220 6700 AE Wageningen The Netherlands www.WageningenAcademic.com [email protected] ISBN 978-90-8686-150-7 The individual contributions in this publication and any liabilities arising from them remain First published, 2010 the responsibility of the authors. The publisher is not responsible for possible © Wageningen Academic Publishers damages, which could be a result of content The Netherlands, 2010 derived from this publication. Welcome Research in applied ethology has a long tradition at the Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, as have the links to this society. This is the third time that the international congress has been organised by our department; the previous years were 1978 and 1988.
    [Show full text]
  • Harassment During Arrival on Land and Departure to Sea in Southern Elephant Seals
    Ethology Ecology & Evolution 12: 389-404, 2000 Harassment during arrival on land and departure to sea in southern elephant seals . F. GALIMBERTI t, L. BOITANI 2 andI. MARZETTI 2 1 Elephant Seals Research Group, Via Buonarroti 35, 20145 Milano, Italy (E-mail: [email protected]) 2 Dipartimento di Biologia Animale, Universik degli Studi di Roma “La Sapienza”, Viale dell’kiversitk 32, 00185 Roma, Italy Received 10 August 1999, accepted 17 November 1999 During the breeding season female elephant seals spent most of their time on land inside harems. When they arrive on land before joining harems, and when they leave harems to return to sea, they are exposed to secondary males and may suffer intense harassment. Hence, arrival and departure present an ideal opportunity to test hypotheses concerning female tactics of harassment reduction. We studied harassment during arrival and departure in two southern elephant seal populations at Punta Delgada (Valdes Peninsula; DEL hereafter) and Sea Lion Island (Falkland Islands; SLI hereafter). Females were less likely to be intercepted by males during arrival than during departure. They also arrived mostly at high tide, thereby reducing the distance from water to the harems. Interception rate and harassment during departure were higher at DEL, where male density and the breeding sex ratio affected the likelihood of interception; on SLI, the socionomy had a small effect. Harassment was higher at low tide at DEL but not at SLI, because tide level variation was larger at DEL and this resulted in a larger variation in the distances of the harems from the water. Females departed more often than expected at high tide at DEL but not at SLI.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroscience and Behavioral Research
    http://www.nap.edu/catalog/10732.html We ship printed books within 1 business day; personal PDFs are available immediately. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research Committee on Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research, National Research Council ISBN: 0-309-50587-9, 224 pages, 6 x 9, (2003) This PDF is available from the National Academies Press at: http://www.nap.edu/catalog/10732.html Visit the National Academies Press online, the authoritative source for all books from the National Academy of Sciences, the National Academy of Engineering, the Institute of Medicine, and the National Research Council: • Download hundreds of free books in PDF • Read thousands of books online for free • Explore our innovative research tools – try the “Research Dashboard” now! • Sign up to be notified when new books are published • Purchase printed books and selected PDF files Thank you for downloading this PDF. If you have comments, questions or just want more information about the books published by the National Academies Press, you may contact our customer service department toll- free at 888-624-8373, visit us online, or send an email to [email protected]. This book plus thousands more are available at http://www.nap.edu. Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved. Unless otherwise indicated, all materials in this PDF File are copyrighted by the National Academy of Sciences. Distribution, posting, or copying is strictly prohibited without written permission of the National Academies Press. Request reprint permission for this book. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research http://www.nap.edu/catalog/10732.html GUIDELINES FOR THE CARE AND USE OF MAMMALS IN NEUROSCIENCE AND BEHAVIORAL RESEARCH Committee on Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research Institute for Laboratory Animal Research Division on Earth and Lifes Studies THE NATIONAL ACADEMIES PRESS Washington, D.C.
    [Show full text]